Unit 3 Neuropsychology Test Batteries: Structure
Unit 3 Neuropsychology Test Batteries: Structure
BATTERIES
Structure
3.0 Introduction
3.1 Objectives
3.2 Neuropsychological Assessment
3.2.1 The Nervous System and Behaviour
3.2.2 Neuropsychological Examination
3.3 Neuropsychological Understanding of Behavioural Deficits
3.4 Goals of Neuropsychological Assessment
3.5 Nature of Neuropsychological Tests
3.6 Identifications of a Deficit by Neuropsychological Tests
3.7 The Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery
3.7.1 History
3.7.2 Structure and Content
3.7.3 Theoretical Foundations
3.7.4 Standardisation Research
3.8 The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Battery
3.8.1 History
3.8.2 Structure and Content
3.8.3 Theoretical Foundations of Component Tests
3.8.4 Standardisation Research
3.8.5 The NIMHANS Neuropsychological Battery
3.9 Let Us Sum Up
3.10 Unit End Questions
3.11 Suggested Readings
3.0 INTRODUCTION
In this unit we are dealing with neuropsychological batteries and tests. We start
with the definition of neuropsychological assessment and present the various
aspects related to the same. Then we discuss how Neuropsychological Assessment
would also lead to obtaining information regarding the neurological deficits
resulting in behavioural deficiencies. Then we take up Goals of
Neuropsychological Assessment and discuss the various factors and clues that
may be obtained in regard to the neurological problems within the individual.
This is followed by a discussion on the nature of neuropsychological tests and
how to identify a deficit with the help of neuropsychological test battery. Two
major batteries, the Luria Nebraska and the Halstead Reitan Neuropsychological
batteries are presented with their history, structure and content within the tests,
the theoretical foundations underlying these tests and the validity and reliability
of these tests. Then we deal with the NIMHANS Neuropsychological battery
and give a description of the various tests within the same.
49
Brain Behaviour
Inter-relationship 3.1 OBJECTIVES
After completing this unit, you will be able to:
• Define and describe Neuropsychological;
• Explain how neuropsychological tests can be used for understanding of
behavioural deficits;
• Elucidate the goals of neuropsychological assessment;
• Describe the nature of neuropsychological tests;
• Explain how to identify a deficit through neuropsychological tests;
• Describe the various aspects of the Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological
Battery;
• Delineate how the test was evolved and devised;
• Describe the Halstead-Reitan battery and its contents, tests and subtests;
and
• Explain the NIMHANS Neuropsychological Battery.
The typical neuropsychological exam begins with a careful history taking. Areas
of interest include:
• Medical history of patient.
• Medical history of patient’s family.
• Presence of absence of developmental milestones.
• Psychosocial history.
• Character, severity, and progress of any history of complaints.
The MSE deals with questions concerning the addressee’s Consciousness,
Emotional State, Thought Content and Clarity, Memory, Sensory Perception,
Performance of Action, Language, Speech, Handwriting, Handedness.Tests and
assessment procedures assess various aspects of functioning including aspects
of:
• Perceptual functioning
• Motor functioning
• Verbal functioning
• Memory Functioning
• Cognitive Functioning
These tests are also used in screening for deficits and in adjunct to medical
examinations.
The tests can be helpful in the assessment of:
• Change in mental status
• Abnormalities in function before abnormalities in structure can be detected.
• Strengths and weaknesses of patient.
• Ability of individual to stand trial.
• Changes in disease process over time.
The Wechsler Scales are often used as a diagnostic tool for intellectual ability
testing.
Formal testing for memory may involve the use of instruments such as the
Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised:
• The task is to recall stories and other verbal stimuli.
• The test is appropriate for people within the ages of 16-74.
Verbal memory, non verbal memory etc. are tested through the presentation of
stimuli such as verbal learning test, selective reminding test Benton test of visual
retention etc. As for tests of cognitive functioning, difficulty in thinking abstractly
is a relatively common consequence of brain injury. One popular measure of
verbal abstraction ability is the Wechsler Similarities Subtest in which the task is
to identify how two objects are alike. Proverb interpretation is another way to
assess ability to think abstractly. Nonverbal tests of abstraction include sorting
tests such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. 51
Brain Behaviour A neuropsychological assessment is a clinical examination of both the working
Inter-relationship
brain and dysfunctional brain. Neuropsychological tests are an aid in this
examination. The objective of neuropsychological assessment is to chart the
deficits and adequacies in the behaviour of patients. The behavioural deficits are
explained by underlying cognitive, emotional, and volitional deficits as well as
changes in the patient’s behaviour. The outcome of a neuropsychological
assessment is a profile of the patient’s deficits and adequacies.
52
Neuropsychological assessment therefore has twin goals. Neuropsychology Test
Batteries
i) The first goal is to identify the disrupted psychological components/
processes/domains in an individual patient and arrive at a profile of
adequacies and deficits of psychological functions.
ii) The second goal is to identify the brain structures/ functional networks, which
are dysfunctional or damaged using the neuropsychological profile that has
previously been derived. Finally, this information is used to lateralise and
localise the bran lesion.
On the other hand, the psychometric approach takes a ‘here and now’ view. It
interprets objective scores with reference to normative data, without taking into
account previous history or current functioning in other areas.
At the same time, it is essential to have objective scores, in order to identify and
classify deficits in psychological components and processes. The need for
objective scores becomes greater when the deficits are mild or when minimal
levels of temporal improvement or deterioration are being tracked. Objective
scores can be obtained if the tests are constructed according to the psychometric
approach.
54
Neuropsychology Test
3) In what way neuropsychology helps in understanding the behvarioural Batteries
deficits?
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
4) What are the goals of neuropsychological assessment?
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
5) Discuss the nature of neuropsychological tests.
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
6) How would neuropsychological test identify a deficit?
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
The raw score for each scale is the sum of the 0, 1 and 2 item scores. Thus, the
higher the score, the poorer the performance. The scores for the individual items
may be based on speed, accuracy, or quality of response. In some cases, two
scores may be assigned to the same task, one for speed and the other for accuracy.
These two scores are counted as individual items. For example, one of the items
is a block counting task, with separate scores assigned for number of errors and
time to completion of the task. In case of time scores, blocks of seconds are
associated with the 0, 1 and 2 scores. When quality of response is scored, the
manual provides both rules for scoring and, in the case of copying tasks,
illustrations of figures representing 0, 1 and 2 scores.
The 269 items are divided into 11 content scales, each of which may be
administered individually. Since these scales contain varying number of items,
raw scale scores are converted to T score with a mean of 50 and a standard
deviation of 10. These T scores are displayed as a profile on a form prepared for
that purpose. In the alternate form of the battery, the names of the content scales
have been replaced by abbreviations. Thus, we have Motor, Rhythm, Tactile,
Visual, Receptive Speech, Expressive Speech, Writing, Reading, Arithmetic,
Memory, and Intellectual Processes scales, which are referred to as the C1 through
C11 scales in the alternate form.
In addition to these 11 content scales, there are three derived scales that appear
on the standard profile form: the Pathognomonic, Left Hemisphere scales. The
Pathognomonic scale contains from throughout the battery found to be particularly
sensitive to the presence or absence of brain damage. The left and right hemisphere
scales are derived from the Motor and Tactile scale items that involve comparisons
56
between the left and right sides of the body. They therefore reflect sensorimotor Neuropsychology Test
Batteries
asymmetries in the two sides of the body.
Several other scales have been developed by Golden and various collaborators,
all of which are based on different ways of scoring the same 269 items. These
special scales include new (empirically derived) right and left hemisphere scales,
a series of localisation scales a series of factor scales and double discrimination
scales. The new right and left hemisphere scales contain items from throughout
the battery and are based on actual comparisons among patients with right
hemisphere, left hemisphere, and diffuse brain damage.
The localisation scales are also empirically derived, being based on studies of
patients with localised brain lesions. There are frontal, sensorimotor, temporal,
and parieto-occipital scales for each hemisphere. The factor scales are based on
extensive factor analytical studies of the major content scales. The new right and
left hemisphere scales contain items from throughout the battery and are based
on actual comparisons among patients with right hemisphere, left hemisphere
and diffuse brain damage. The new right and left hemisphere, localisation factor
scales may all be expressed in T scores with a mean of 50. There are also two
scales that provide global indices of dysfunctions, and are meant as equivalents
to the Halstead impairment index. They are called the Profile Elevation and
Impairment Scales.
Therefore, our discussion of the theory underlying the Luria Nebraska battery
will be based on the assumption that the only connecting link between Luria and
that procedure is the set of Christensen items. In doing so, it becomes clear that
the basic theory underlying the development of Luria- Nebraska is based on a
philosophy of science that stresses empirical validity, quantification and
application of established psychometric procedures. Indeed, as pointed out
elsewhere, it is essentially the same epistemology that characterises the work of
the Reitan group.
Thus, research done with the Luria Nebraska battery determined
1) whether it discriminates between brain damaged patients in general and
normal controls;
2) whether it discriminates between patients with structural brain damage and
those with schizophrenia;
3) whether the procedure has the capacity to lateralise and regionally localise
brain damage; and
4) whether there are performance patterns specific to particular neurological
disorders, such as alcoholic dementia or multiple sclerosis.
Since this research was accomplished in recent years, it was able to benefit from
the new brain imaging technology, notably the CT scan, and the application of
high speed computer technologies, allowing for extensive use of powerful
multivariate statistical methods. With regard to methods of clinical inference,
the same method suggested by Reitan that is level of performance, pattern of
performance, pathognomonic signs, and right left comparisons etc., are used
with the Luria Nebraska battery.
Adhering to our assumption that the Luria Nebraska bears little resemblance to
Luria’s methods and theories, there seems little point in examining the theoretical
basis for the substance of the Luria Nebraska battery. For example, there is little
point in examining the theory of language that underlies the Receptive Speech
and Expressive Speech scales or the theory of memory that provides the basis
for the Memory scale. We believe that the Luria Nebraska battery is not a means
of using Luria’s theory and methods in English speaking countries, but rather a
standardised psychometric instrument with established validity for certain
purposes and reliability.
The test manual reports reliability data. Test-retest reliabilities for the 13 major
scales range from .78 to .96. The problem of interjudge reliability is generally
not a major one for neuropsychological assessment, since most of the test used is
quite objective and have quantitative scoring systems. However, there could be
a problem with the Luria-Nebraska, since the assignment of 0, 1, and 2 scores
sometimes requires a judgement by the examiner.
During the preliminary screening stage in the development of the battery, items
in the original pool that did not attain satisfactory interjudge reliability were
dropped. A 95% inter-rater agreement level was reported by the test constructors
for the 282 items used in an early version of the battery developed after dropping
those items. The manual contains means and standard deviations for each item
based on samples of control, neurologically impaired, and schizophrenic subjects.
An alternate form of the battery is available. To the best of our knowledge, there
have been no predictive validity studies. It is unclear whether or not there have
been studies that address the issue of construct validity.
60
Reitan adopted Halstead’s methods and various test procedures and with them Neuropsychology Test
Batteries
established a laboratory at the University of Indiana. He supplemented these
tests with a number of additional procedures in order to obtain greater
comprehensiveness and initiated a clinical research program that is ongoing.
The program began with cross validation of the battery and expanded into
numerous areas, including validation of new tests added to the battery (e.g. the
Trail Marking Test), lateralisation and localisation of function, aging, and
neuropsychological aspects of a wide variety of disorders such as alcoholism,
hypertension, disorders of children, and mental retardation.
The Halsted Reitan battery, as the procedure came to be known over the years,
also has a history. It has been described as a fixed battery, but the sets of tests are
grown by accretion and revision and continues to be revised. The tests that
survived a long research history include the Category Test, The Tactual
Performance Test, The Speech Perception Test, The Seashore Rhythm Test, and
Finger Tapping.
There have been numerous additions, including the various Wechsler Intelligence
scales, the Trail Making test, a sub-battery of perceptual tests the Reitan aphasia
Screening Test, the Klove Grooved Pegboard, and other tests that are used in
some laboratories but not in others.
The Halstead Reitan battery continues to be widely used as a clinical and research
procedure. Numerous investigators use it in their research, and there have been
several successful cross validations done in settings other than Reitan’s laboratory.
In addition to the continuation of factor analytic work with the battery, several
investigators have applied other forms of multivariate analysis to it in various
research applications.
Some of this research has been conducted relative to objectifying and even
computerising interpretation of the battery; the most well-known efforts are the
Selz Reitan rules for classification of brain function in older children and the
Russel, Neuringer, and Goldstein “neurological keys”.
The issue of reliability of the battery has been addressed, with reasonably
successful results. Clinical interpretation of the battery continues to be taught at
workshops and in numerous programs engaged in the training of professional
psychologists.
61
Brain Behaviour 3.8.2 Structure and Content
Inter-relationship
Although there are several versions of the Halsted Reitan battery, the differences
tend to be minor, and there appears to be a core set procedures that essentially all
versions of the battery must be administered in a laboratory containing specific
equipment. It is probably best to plan on about 6 to 8 hours of patient time. Each
test of the battery is independent and may be administered separately from the
other tests. However, it is generally assumed that a certain number of the tests
must be administered in order to compute an impairment index.
Scoring for the Halsted Reitan varies with the particular test, such that individual
scores may be expressed in time to completion, errors, number correct, or some
form of derived score. These scores are often converted to standard scores or
ratings so that they may be profiled. All of the tests contributing to the impairment
index on a 6-point scale, the data being displayed as a profile of the ratings. They
have also provided quantitative scoring systems for the Reitan Aphasia Test and
for the drawing of a Greek cross that is part of that test. However, some clinicians
do not quantify those procedures, except in the form of counting the number of
Aphasic symptoms elicited.
Theoretical Foundation: There are really two theoretical bases for the Halsted
Reitan battery, one contained in brain and intelligence and related writings of
Halstead. The other are found in numerous papers and chapters written by Reitan
and various collaborators. Halstead was really the first to establish a human
neuropsychology laboratory in which patients were administered objective tests,
some of which are semi automated, utilising standard procedures and sets of
instructions. His Chicago laboratory may have been the stimulus for the now
common practice of administration of neuropsychological tests by trained
technicians. Halstead was also the first to use sophisticated, multivariate statistics
in the analysis of neuropsychological test data.
One could say that Reitan’s great concern has always been with the empirical
validity of test procedures. Such validity can only be established through the
collection of large amounts of data obtained from patients with reasonably
complete documentation of their medical\neurological conditions. Both presence
and absence of brain damage had to be well documented, and if present, findings
related to site and type of lesion had to be established. He described his work
62
informally as one large experiment, necessitating maximal consistency in the Neuropsychology Test
Batteries
procedures used, and to some extent, in the methods of analysing the data. Reitan
and his various collaborators represent the group that was primarily responsible
for the introduction of standard battery approach to clinical neuropsychology. It
is clear from reviewing the Reitan group’s work that there is substantial emphasis
on performing controlled studies with samples sufficiently large to allow for the
application of conventional statistical procedures.
It would probably be fair to say that the major thrust of Reitan’s research and
writings has not been espousal of some particular theory of brain function, but
rather an extended examination of the inferences that can be made from
behavioural indices relative to the condition of the brain. There is a great emphasis
on methods of drawing such inferences in case of the individual patient. Thus,
this group’s work has always involved empirical research and clinical
interpretation, with one feeding into the other. In this regard, there has been a
formulation of inferential methods used in neuropsychology that provides a
framework for clinical interpretation. Four methods are outlined: level of
performance, pattern of performance, specific behavioural deficits
(pathognomonic signs), and right-left comparisons. In other words, one examines
whether the patient’s general level of adaptive function is comparable to that of
normal individuals, whether there is some characteristics performance profile
that suggests impairment even though the average score may be within normal
limits, whether there are unequivocal individual signs of deficits, and whether
there is a marked discrepancy in functioning between the two sides of the body.
Halstead’s Biological Intelligence Tests: There are five subtests in this section
of Halsted-Reitan battery developed by Halstead.
The point of the test is to see how well the subject can learn the concept, idea, or
principle that connects the pictures. If the correct switch is pressed, the subject
will hear a pleasant chime, while wrong answers are associated with a rasping
buzzer. The conventionally used score is the total number of errors for the seven
groups of stimuli that forms the test. Booklet forms (Adams & Trenton, 1981;
DeFillippis, McCampbell & Rogers, 1979) and abbreviated forms (Calsyn,
O’Leary, & Chaney, 1980; Russel & Levy, 1987; Sherril, 1987) of this test have
been developed.
The Halstead Tactual Performance Test: This procedure used a version of the
Seguin-Goddard Form board, but it is done blindfold. The subject’s task is to
place all the 10 blocks into the board, using only the sense of touch. The task is
63
Brain Behaviour repeated three times, once with the preferred hand, once with the non preferred
Inter-relationship
hand, and once with both hands, after which the board is removed. After removing
the blindfold, the subject is asked to draw a picture of the board, filling in all of
the blocks he or she remembers in their proper locations on the board. Scores
from this test include time to complete the task for each of the three trials, total
time, number of blocks correctly drawn, and number of blocks correctly drawn
in their proper locations on the board.
The Seashore Rhythm Test: This test consists of 30 pairs of rhythmic patterns.
The task is to judge whether the two members of each pair are the same or
different and to record the response by writing an S or a D on an answer sheet.
The score is either the number correct or the number of errors.
Finger Tapping: The subject is asked to tap his or her extended index finger on
a typewriter key attached to a mechanical counter. Several series of 10-second
trials are run, with both the right and the left hand. The scores are the average
number of taps, generally over five trials, for the right and left hand.
Tests added to the battery by Reitan. Reitan added four components to the battery
and these are given below:
The Trail Making Test: In part A of this procedure the subject must connect in
order a series of circled numbers randomly scattered over a sheet of 81\2 X 11
paper. In part B, there are circled numbers and letters and the subject’s task
involves alternating between numbers and letters in serial order. The score is
time to completion expressed in seconds for each part.
The Reitan Aphasia Screening Test: This test serves two purposes in that it
contains both copying and language-related tasks. As an Aphasia screening
procedure, it provides a brief survey of the major language functions: naming,
repetition, spelling, reading, writing, calculation, narrative speech, and right-left
orientation. The copying task involves having the subject copy a square, Greek
cross, triangle, and key. The first three items must each be drawn in one continuous
line. The language section may be scored by listing the number of aphasic
symptoms or by using the quantitative system developed by Russel and co-
workers. The drawings are either not formally scored are rated through a matching
to model system also provided by Russel and Colleagues.
Perceptual Disorders: The procedure actually constitute a sub-battery and
include tests of the subject’s ability to recognise shapes by touch and identifies
numbers written on the fingertips, as well as tests of finger discrimination and
visual, auditory, and tactile neglect. The number of errors is the score for all
64 these procedures.
Other Tests: The Halsted Reitan battery was expanded further by other Neuropsychology Test
Batteries
researchers to include more tests.
The Klove Grooved Pegboard Test: The subject must place pegs shaped like
keys into a board containing recesses that are oriented in randomly varying
directions. The test is administered twice, once with the right and once with the
left hand. Sores are the time to completion in seconds in each hand and errors for
each hand, defined as the number of pegs dropped during performance of the
task.
The Klove roughness Discrimination Test: The subject must order four blocks
covered with varying grades of sandpaper presented behind a blind with regards
to degree of roughness. Time and error scores are recorded for each hand.
Visual Field Examination: Russel et. al include a formal visual field examination
using a parameter as part of their assessment procedure.
Tests in the expanded version include the Wisconnin card Sorting, Thurstone
word Fluency, Story Memory, Figural Memory, Seashore Tonal Memory, Digit
Vigilance, Peabody Individual Achievement, and Boston naming Tests, plus a
part of Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination.
It would appear from one impressive study that valid inferences concerning
prediction at this level must be clinically, and one cannot call upon the standard
univariate statistical procedures to make the necessary discriminations. The study
65
Brain Behaviour provides the major impetus for Russel and co-workers’ neuropsychological key
Inter-relationship
approach, which was an essence an attempt to objectify higher-order inferences.
With regard to the first aspect, Heaton and Pendleton (1981) document lack of
predictive validity studies using extensive batteries of the Halsted Reitan type.
However they do not report one study in which Halsted Reitan successfully
predicted employment status on 6-month follow-up. With regard to prediction
of course of illness, there appears to be a good deal of clinical expertise, but no
major formal studies in which the battery’s capacity to predict whether the patient
will get better, worse, or stay the same is evaluated. This matter is of particular
significance in such conditions as head injury and stroke, since outcome tends to
be quite variable in these conditions. The changes that occur during the early
stages of these disorders are often the most significant ones related to prognosis.
In general, there has not been a great deal of emphasis on studies involving the
reliability of the Halsted Reitan battery, probably because of nature of the tests
themselves, particularly with regard to the practice effect problem, and because
of the changing nature of those patients from whom the battery was developed.
Golstein and Watson (1989) provided a review of Halsted Reitan battery reliability
studies, as well as a test-retest study of their own, concluding that reliability
levels were satisfactory in a number of different clinical groups.
The category test can have its reliability assessed through the split-half method.
Self Assessment Questions
1) Discuss in detail the Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological battery.
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
66
Neuropsychology Test
2) Trace the history of how the Halstead-Reitan battery was devised. Batteries
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
3) What are the structure and content in the Halstead Reitan
Neuropsychological battery?
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
4) Discuss the theoretical foundation on which Halstead Reitan battery is
devised.
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
5) What are the validity and reliability of this test battery?
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
Tests of Attention:
3) Focused attention-Colour trails test
4) Sustained attention- digit vigilance test
5) Divided attention- the triads test
Working memory:
9) N back test (Verbal working memory and Visual working memory)
10) Self ordered pointing test
Planning
11) Tower of London test
Set shifting
12) Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST)
Response inhibition
13. Stroop test-NIMHANS version
Verbal comprehension
14) Token test
Tests of verbal Learning and memory:
15) Rey’s Auditory verbal learning test
16) Logical memory test
70
Neuropsychology Test
Batteries
References
Sherrill, R. E. jr. (1987). options for shortening Halstead’s category test for adults.
I Archives of clinical neuropsychology,2,343-352.
73