Goal Setting - Locke
Goal Setting - Locke
Abstract
C , 1
• •,16
ISCLAIMEI NOTICE
Edwin A. Locke
been overly concerned with theoretical issues, but the fact that occasional
studies fail to find significant KR effects on performance (e.g. , Chapanis,
1964) emphasizes the need to integrate and systematize findings in this area.
studies of KR or "feedback" have been of this type (e. g. , Bilodeau & Bilodeau,
1961). Given a constant motivational state, the more information given to S
about the task or about how to correct errors, the better his performance
level or learning.
-I-
Lr
Knowledge of score on simple motor twsks such as reaction time and weight
lifting also falls into this category, since there are no right or wrong
responses on such tasks. Any effects on performance of this type of KR may
Book and Norvell (1922), Crawley (1926), Mackworth (1950), and Ross (1927),
Ss given knowledge of score were usually told to try to improve their perform-
ance or were given explicit goals to reach, whereas the No KR Ss were told
not to think about trying to improve their scores or were told simply to "do
their best. " Although the KR groups performed better than the No KR groups
in these experiments, the effects of goal-setting were confounded with the
effects of knowledge of score, making it impossible to determine which was
the critical variable or the interdependence, if any, between them. Similar
criticisms can be made of recent studies, e.g. , Ch,'ch ano Camp (1965);
McElheran (1963); and Payne and Hauty (1955). In these studies goals were
In five other studies (Arps, 1920; Johanson, 1922, Mace, 1935, Manzer,
1935; Gibbs & Brown, 1955) positive results were obtained for KR without
explicit manipulation of Ss' goals; however, no attempt was made to deter-
mine if different goals were set by KR and No KR groups (although Mace
argued that the effect of KR was to suggest a performance standard to the KR
Ss). In contrast, Chapanis (1964), attempting to replicate the finding of Gibbs
and Brown (1955), did two things to eliminate "demand characteristics" (Orne,
1962) that may have affected previous results: 1) he "hired" Ss as employees
doing a job for pay (punching digits onto a tape) rather than as experimental
Ss; and 2) he ran Ss individually rather than in groups (as was the case in most
previous experiments in this area). In the absence of the implicit goal setting
-2-
demands (e.g. , "improvement") inherent in the typical experimental situa-
tion and the possible effects of implicit competition, Chapanis found no
effect of KR on performance. Further, Locke and Bryan (1966b) found no
overall effect of KR on performance on a complex c(mputation task. But
when Ss were re-grouped according to their a posteriori performance goal
descriptions, a significant relationship of goals to pcrformance was found,
suggesting that differences in Ss' goals had more effect on performance
The major premise underlying this hypothesis is that level of effort on a task
is determined largely by S's conscious performance goals. This emphasis
on a cognitive approach to motivation is supported by recent theoretical
developments (e. g. , Dulany, 1962; Miller, Galanter, & Pribram, 1960;
Ryan, 1958, 1964; Spielberger, 1965) and by the findings of a number of
recent studies (e.g., DeNike, 1965; Dulany, 1962; Locke, 1966a, 1966b;
Locke & Bryan, 1966a, 1966b; Spielberger, Berger. & Howard, 1963; Spiel-
berger, Bernstein, & Ratliff, 1966; Spielberger, Levin, & Shepard. 1962).
Method
Task. The task was simple addition. Each problem consisted of three
two-digit numbers and was presented on a separate 3x5 index card. The
cards were placed consecutively in boxes holding 720 cards each. The Ss
wrote their answers on sheets containing space for 90 answers. As each
answer sheet was completed, S was instructed to insert it in a slot underneath
a one-way mirror through which he was observed by E rduring the experin•nt.
-3-
The S worked 1'L" oMe. hour at the task, which was divided into five trials
separated by short rest pertods. The trials were alternately 10 and 15 min-
utes in length (i.e. , 10'; 15'; 10'; 15'; and 10'). All Ss were told the length
of each tria; in advance.
main effects.
The design was a 2xZ fixed model. The fixed variables were knowledge
of score (KR) and type of goal.
-4-
No KR Condition. The No KR Ss were not given their scores. Since
Ss had to hand in each answer sheet after completion and since they did not
know how many of their answers were wrong, these Ss could not easily keep
track of their score except in a very general way.
Do Best Goal. "Do Best" Ss were told to "do their best" on each trial.
(This was the goal typically given to NoKR Ss in the experiments discussed
earlier.) Do Bez.t Ss in the KR condition could not easily use their scores
to set specific personal goals due to the alternating trial lengths. In addition,
Hard Goal. Goals were set for the Hard Goal Ss on the basis of the
scores attained by the matched "Do Best" Ss. On the first trial a given S's
goal was set about 10 percent higher than the score achieved on the same
trial by a matched Do Best S. Then E adjusted S's goal before each succeeding
trial depending upon how well S had done on the previous trial. If S did not
get near the goal, the next goal was lowered slightly; if S reached or exceeded
the goal, the succeeding goal was raised.
The goals were marked by means of a colored 3x5 index card placed
vertically at the appropriate point in the box of problem cards. This card
represented the point they had to reach by the end of the trial in order to
reach their goal (if they got all problems correct). The Ss were told to try
and surpass this point since they were bound to get sorvie wrong. They were
told at the end of each trial whether or not they had blawil) the ga-l but were'
not given their actual scores unless they were in the KR condition.
On the average the goals of the Hard Goal Ss were set from 0% to 32%
(mean and median = 11%) above the scores attained by the matched Do Best
Ss. Hard Goal Ss wereable to reach or beat their goals on 16% oi the trials.
each of which described a possible goal they could have had (e. g. , "I tried
to do my best"; "I tried for the assigned goal") and were asked to pick out
the card that best represented their performance goal during the experiment.
Results
period. On the other hand, the KR and No KR curves did not diverge except
for the Hard Goal group in the last 10-minute period.
Goal group bcing superior to the Do Best group in both catses. A mantched
-6-
groups test comparing the Hard Goal and D)o B~est groups (performed by
matching each Hard Goal S with a Do Dest S of equal initial ability) yielded
a t-ratio for mean deterioraticn of 2.80 (17, p ( .02), and a similar test
There were no significant main effects nor any interaction for the
percent error criterion, indicating that the higher performance level attained
by the Hard Goal group was not achieved at the expense of a relatively greater
number of errors in relation to problems attempted. Both the Hard Goal and
Do Best groups averaged about 5.6% errors during the experimental trials.
Discussion
The results of the present experiment support the hypothesis that moti-
vational effects previously attributed to differential knowledge of results were
actually a function of differential performance goals associated with the KR
conditions. When differential goal setting by KR and No KR Ss was controlled,
no overall effect of KR on performance was found. Howc\.er, when the effccts
-7-
specifi Hard GoQ'% versus L, Besýj were compared, a significant goal effect
was found. These results were consistent with previous findings by Locke
and Bryan (1966b) where goals were measured by post-experimental inter-
Although the findings of the present study provide support for the initial
hypothesis, there are a number of issues still open. It may be observed from
Figure 1 that there was an effect of KR condition between trials 4 and 5. The
KR groups increased their performance rate on trial 5, whereas the No KR
groups decreased. The F for the difference scores (1, 32) was 8.83 (p < .01).
It would be useful to determine whether this effect was due to implicit goal-
setting on the part of the KR Ss, who by this time may have been able to get
some idea of their rate, or to some other effect.
Only two goal classes were used in the present study. In one sense the
Hard Goals in the present experiment were harder than the Do Best goals,
since the Hard Goals were set above the performance level of the Do Best Ss.
However, just "how much" harder is not known. A greater variety of per-
formance goals should be utilized in subsequent studies and some attempts
made to scale the goals as to difficulty or motivational level.
In more general terms, the results of the present study suggest one
mechanism by which incentives of all types might work (e. g. , KR, money,
instructions, participation, praise, reproof, verbal "reinforcement, " etc.);
it is possible that such incentives are effective only to the degree that they
-8-
The present results suggest that in order to predict the effect of
-9-
Footnotes
- 9a -
References
-11-
Orne, M. T. On the social psychology of the psychological experiment with
particular reference to demand characteristics. American Psychologist,
1962, 17, 776-783.
-12-
Table I
Deterioration
Source df F
Goals 1 4.83*
KR 1 ( 1
GoalsxKR I CI
Within (mean square) 3z (8,695.28)
Goals I (1
KR 1 1. 32
GoalsxKR 1 41
Within (mean square) 32 (9. 29)
*p - .05
S*p K .01
-13-
01. .
Isz
-z Ix
0 Z-Ze
z a,
0 00
o~4 4%) s)
% /
0 -%n
4.4
4 F- -0
C:4 E-)
0
00
C-)
0 00 00
04fluiW, J~d 4:)OJJO:) swalq0Jdj 10401