Dyslexia Test (DAIPC)
Dyslexia Test (DAIPC)
CHILDREN (DAPIC)
Kuppuraj.S
MAY- 2009
CERTIFICATE
Indian children (DAPIC)” is the bonafide work submitted in part fulfillment for the
(Registration No.07SLP009). This has been carried out under the guidance of a
faculty of this institute and has not been submitted earlier to any other university for
Director
Mysore,
All India Institute of Speech and Hearing,
May, 2009
Naimisham Campus, Manasagangothri,
Indian children (DAPIC)” has been prepared under my supervision and guidance. It
is also certified that this has not been submitted earlier in any other university for the
Guide
Ms. Jayashree.C.Shanbal
India Institute of Speech and Hearing, Mysore, and has not been submitted earlier at
May, 2009
Acknowledgement
I would like to thank Dr.Vijayalakshmi Basavaraj, for permitting me to carry out this
study.
My thanks also to Dr.Prema.K.S (Mr. Research), for being the impetus of selecting this
field for this study.
This dissertation would not have reached its finishing line, with out Mr.Gopi Shankar’s
timely suggestions and support through out research writing.
I would like to thank our statistician Mrs.Vasanthalakshmi, for her reckless help in
statistics calculations.
I extend my sincere thanks to Jayakumar(Jaks), Arun BT(baba) foe being with me when
needed them for dissertation.
And my heartly thanks to all those kids participated in this study who asked nothing
but, ‘anna elli bari beku’.Thanks for the school authorities for the cooperation as well.
Who else could have given me a bright future none other than our AIISH?.I thank all the
teaching staffs for giving me the bits and pieces of their knowledge which would take me
a long way.
Finally here is the opportunity to thank all the cool dudes of AIISH(viv,amit,chandan
,Akshay,Mohandas,narendra)for being with me and you guys too are the reason for all my
smiles at AIISH, and I wounder , we ever had a reason to worry, and challenge the
jinx?(and also my class pals for bearing with me in class).
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Appendix A
Appendix B
List of Tables
Grades I to V.
Introduction
Reading is a complex cognitive process that involves multiple skills (Prema, 1997).
(Gayan & Olsen, 2003). Before one becomes completely literate, he or she must develop
early reading skills which comprised of steps like literacy awareness, syntactic awareness,
1997).
Reading process is the conversion of print into auditory equivalents and the
language. The reading process includes decoding of the print in to sound. Decoding begins
with the process of converting individual letters into sounds and proceeds to the decoding of
whole words and phrases. A person who cannot decode printed words cannot understand
what those words mean. Comprehension is the next step which is the ability to interpret and
understand the decoded words. Although adequate decoding is a prerequisite for reading it
is not sufficient, comprehension is also required. The ability to comprehend what one reads
is based on experience. Readers take what has been read and integrate it with previously
2003), the rules of orthographic knowledge (processing written language letters and letter
spelling is related to reading and written expression. Spelling is one of the most valued yet
difficult skills in written communication. Spelling requires matching the sounds of language
with the appropriate letters in order to accurately and reliably conveys messages. A child’s
ability to spell words correctly shows a sophisticated knowledge of letters, sounds and
syllable patterns (Bear & Templeton, 1998). There are several stages through which a child
travels before master spelling skill. Gentry (1982) proposes stages like precommunicative
(where the child uses symbols from the alphabet but shows no knowledge of letter-sound
correspondences), semi phonetic(sounds are assigned to letters), phonetic (The child uses a
letter or group of letters to represent every speech sound that they hear in a word),
transitional (the speller begins to assimilate the conventional alternative for representing
sounds),and correct stage(the speller knows the English orthographic system and its basic
rules). Frith (1985) also proposes parallel but different stages through which a child goes
through before he acquires complete knowledge of spelling. These stages are logographic,
Fletcher. Lyon, Fuchs, & Barnes (2006) have quoted that reading is not natural and
everyone is taught to read. Those children who lack the ability to learn literacy skills are
considered ‘dyslexic’. Despite years of research, mostly in the English language, there is still
no consensus as to the definition of dyslexia, nor its underlying cause. The current dominant
view (Masland, 1997) argues that the cause of the educational difficulties faced by dyslexics
centre around core difficulties in phonological awareness and word decoding skills.
(Stanovich, 2000) admits that it may be related to deficits in processing novel letter string.
Smythe and Everatt (2000) proposed the following definition which incorporates
many of the features identified by research as important in these diverse mono and
multilingual environments.
manifestation of dyslexia in any individual will depend upon not only individual
There are several causes proposed in the literature for dyslexia. They are heredity
information processing (Tallal, Miller & Fitch 1993), selective attention and attention deficit
disorder (Zentall, 1993), middle ear problems (Roberts & Medley, 1995), Cognitive rigidity
and learned helplessness (Clay, 1984). Akin causes of dyslexia, there are several types of
learning difficulty (dyslexia) exist. Theoretically there are as many types of dyslexics as
number of cases do, making the dyslexic group a heterogeneous one. Newcombe and
Marshall (1984) proposed a reading model which explains normal visual word reading. This
model composed of two routs namely lexical (direct route) through which words and
irregular words are read. Another route of the model is the sublexical route (indirect route)
through which non words are read. Castles and Coltheart (1993) broadly classifies dyslexia in
to two main types called phonological and surface dyslexics. He opined that with difficulties
in sub lexical skills shall come under phonological dyslexics and if the children with dyslexia
have difficulties in lexical skills, he/she may fall under surface dyslexia.
Edwards and Hogben (1999) included another type of dyslexia called mixed dyslexia,
dyslexia of this subtype tend to find difficulties in both lexical and sublexical route of reading ,
along with already proposed two types. There are several other subtypes of dyslexia
proposed by other investigators. Boder (1973) classifies dyslexics into dysphonetic (having
difficulties especially with grapheme–phoneme conversion, i.e. with the indirect route for
reading), dyseidetic (having difficulties with visual recognition of whole words, i.e. with the
direct route for reading) and mixed (having both types of difficulties) subtypes. Bakker (
1979, 1990) classifies dyslexia into P-types (relying on perceptual, analytical strategies for
reading, which turns out to be slow, fragmented and hesitating), L-types (relying on linguistic,
anticipatory strategies for reading, which allow for quicker reading but produce many,
usually plausible and context-based errors) and M-types (mixed types, showing both slow,
fragmented reading and many errors). Children with dyslexia and their subtypes need to be
identified with appropriate assessment measures so that the intervention for the same can
There are several western assessment tools available to assess dyslexia using
standard scores. For e.g., Stanford diagnostic reading test-4(SDRT 4) by Karlsen and Gardner
Woodcock Reading Mastery tests-Revised (WRMT-R) by Woodcock (1997), Oral and Written
language scales (OWLS) by Carrow & Woolfolk (1995) , Test of Written expression (TOWE)
by McGhee, Bryant, Larsen & Rivera (1995), Test of Written Language -3 (TOWL 3) by
Hammill and Larsen (1996),Test of Written Spelling-3 (TWS-3) by Larsen and Hammill (1994).
However, existence of successful assessment tool in Indian multilingual context has been
education plan (IEP) that clearly identifies the individual’s strengths and weaknesses, their
specific needs, and the timescale and resources required to implement the IEP. It should
attempt to match the teaching style to the preferred learning style of the individual in order
to maximize the amount learnt in a given time. The term dyslexia may be used to refer to
children presenting very different profiles of strengths and weaknesses, and therefore may
be of limited use in determining the IEP. What is required is greater specificity, such as a
child’s ability in phonological and non phonological tasks, which will enable us to classify
them under appropriate subtype, which will intern pave way for appropriate treatment plan.
Thus need to understand the cognitive profile and attainment measures become even more
Moreover, the estimated prevalence rate of learning disability have been found to
range from 3% to 10% (Snowling, 2000) that is approximately 1 in 59 or 1.69% or 4.6 million
people in USA. Prevalence rate in India ranges from 3% to 10 %( Ramaa, 2000). The high
prevalence rate of learning disability (3% to 10%, Ramaa, 1985) indicates the need for early
literature, it has been found that dyslexic population in vastly heterogeneous and this
heterogeneous nature in the dyslexic group poses the requirement to profile the learning
appropriately, it is important that the children with dyslexia are sub grouped under existing
II. To identify subtypes of dyslexia based on the profiles established on children with
dyslexia
CHAPTER 2
Review of literature
Literacy is defined as the minimal ability to read and write in a designated language,
as well as a mindset or way of thinking about the use of reading and writing in everyday life.
Literacy, requires an active, autonomous engagement with print and stresses the role of the
messages (Harris & Hodges, 1995).The components of literacy includes reading, writing, and
Before one becomes completely literate, he or she must develop early reading skills
which comprise of five steps (Strommen & Mates, 1997), in which each successive step is
grammatical structure.
The third area of reading development is word recognition. In this stage children are
develop across time and that various aspects of phonological sensitivity are
about the writing system and how letters and letter strings are used to represent
words. Evidences from literature reveals that has highly developed perceptions from
the time of birth. With in a few days, a baby is able to distinguish the mothers voice
from others voices and from less meaningful sounds. Thus the receptive faculty, the
ability to distinguish and understand what is heard, develops from before oral
language skill. Reception and expression of oral language are the child’s first and for
several years only means of communication; they are considered most critical in the
Though there are several prerequisites for later reading development, not all the
prereading skills are proven to be significantly influencing the later reading development.
Phonological awareness has been shown to be a primary factor underlying early reading
development (Ehri, Nunes, Willows, Schuster & Shanahan, 2001). Deficits in phonological
awareness have been linked to reading disabilities (Lyon, Shaywitz & Shaywitz, 2003).
Phonological awareness, when compared to many other predictors, was the most stable and
robust indicator of later reading in the group of children who were followed from late
preschool to kindergarten and first grade (Adams, 1990; Bryant, MacLean, Bradley &
Crossland, 1990; Lundberg, Frost, Cunningham, 1991; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 2001; Lonigan,
Burgess & Anthony, 2000). Manis, Seidenberg,Doi , McBride-Chang and Petersen (1996),
Pennington, Cardoso-Martins, Green and Lefly (2001) and Torgesen, Wagner, Rashotte,
Burgess and Hecht (1997) suggested after their longitudinal studies that phonological tasks
like alliteration and rapid naming(RN) were differentially related to reading ability.
Specifically it has been suggested that although alliteration is the most important for the
especially important for learning about the orthography of word. Naming objects rapidly has
been considered as a skill necessary for fluent, skilled reading. Indeed, RN has been
consistently found to be related to fluency of text reading even in the present study. The
individual’s writing ability can be a good predictor of reading thus spelling as a significant
predictor of reading (Read, 1971; Clarke, 1988; Dyson 2001; Richgels, 2001; Whitehurst &
Lonigan, 2001).
Once children begin reading however the best indicator of current and future
reading may simply be reading itself (Bell, McCallum & Cox, 2003). Wagner et al., (1997)
add to the prediction of reading once a measure of current word reading and vocabulary
was considered .The results revealed that the phonological awareness is less efficient in
prediction of reading as the child gets older. It is well accepted that reading consists of
two components, decoding and comprehension (Aaron, Joshi & Williams, 1999). Decoding is
the word recognition process that transforms print to words, whereas comprehension
assigns meanings to words, sentences and texts. Catts and Kamhi (1999) reported that word
larger discourse units requires syntactic, morphologic, semantic and discourse knowledge. A
logical consequence of the language basis of reading is that children who have deficiencies in
When the child enters school and turns to the task of reading, the visual
discrimination task becomes of key importance. This skill must already be highly developed
before letter recognition and the ability to identify the relatively small differences between
one letter and another will be possible. In addition, the social and emotional development of
the child will prove vital to all school experiences. These prerequisite skills flow in to and
influence the acquisition of the beginning reading skills. A child of average intelligence is
expected to learn to read if he has only reasonably good teaching. Learning to read, for
many children seems not to require much effort. They appear to attain this ability almost
incidentally. They quickly make associations between the printed symbol, the auditory
Meanwhile for the development of reading patterns, ordering skills or the skills of
The acquisition of phonic skills or the acquisition of the relationship between specific
sounds and specific symbols must be mastered in order for the children to succeed in their
reading journey. However the relationship between the grapheme and the phoneme in all
the language are not linear .The languages where there is only one sound for each letter, the
assigning phoneme to a grapheme step is relatively easy. But in languages like English the
association of various different letters sounds with a particular letter and various different
letters with a particular sound makes mastery of the sound symbol relationship more
difficult. Further to master in reading skill, the student must also be able to blend these
sounds together into a recognizable word and to development the reading patterns,
ordering skills or the skills of directionality are required. Additional abilities of word or the
ability to divide words into syllables and use of structural word analysis to distinguish
prefixes, suffixes and root meanings are also equally vital in reading process. Simultaneous
with the acquisition of the phonic skills is the assignment of meaning to words encountered
in print much of the initial meaning of these words is drawn from the children’s background
experiences in their environment. Comprehension skills develop and mature as the child
encounters words in print that appear in a new context. The child learns that words have
multiple meanings and that some words have implied or connotative meaning according to
the culture. As sentences combine into paragraphs and paragraphs into stories, the
meanings of words and ideas become more complicated. Both literal and interpretive skills
Further reading process can be explained through Dual Route Cascaded (DRC) model
of reading. According to this model there are two routs in translating print in to sound. A
rule system. The lexical route translates the pronunciation of a word based on word specific
knowledge. The route consists of three components they are semantic system, the
orthographic lexicon, and the phonological lexicon. The semantic system computes the
meaning of a word, whereas the lexicons compute the words’ orthographic and phonological
form. The non-lexical route differs from the lexical route in both the knowledge base and the
type of processing it employs. The non-lexical route generates the pronunciation of Letter
rules. The set of rules is encapsulated in the GPC module. Based on the two routes proposed,
disruption in any of the pathway may result in reading difficulties of different varieties
namely phonological and surface dyslexia. If the affected route is lexical route the patient is
said to be having surface dyslexia. The characteristics of surface dyslexia are inability to read
irregular words, while the patient has intact ability to read regular words and regular non
words. If the non-lexical GPC route is affected, the patient may show difficulties in reading
and the subsequent interpretation of those equivalents into meanings based on previously
learned language. The reading process includes decoding of the print in to sound. Decoding
begins with the process of converting individual letters into sounds and proceeds to the
decoding of whole words and phrases. A person who cannot decode printed words cannot
understand what those words mean. Comprehension is the next step which is the ability to
interpret and understand the decoded words. Although adequate decoding is a prerequisite
for reading it is not sufficient, comprehension is also required. The ability to comprehend
what one reads is based on experience. Readers take what has been read and integrate it
with previously learned language and experience. The next component of literacy is writing.
Berninger, Abbott, Vaughan & Vermaulen, 2003). Spelling development in normal children
can be derived from connectionist model (Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989) of alphabetic
spelling development. Loosemore, Brown and Watson (1991) studied the development of
alphabetical spelling system using the artificial connectionist model and concluded that
during the early course of development, they observed regularization errors and these
errors faded as the model was exposed to more alphabetical spelling system.
development like precommunicative (where the child uses symbols from the alphabet but
to letters), phonetic (The child uses a letter or group of letters to represent every speech
sound that they hear in a word), transitional (the speller begins to assimilate the
conventional alternative for representing sounds),and correct stage(the speller knows the
increased proficiency in each ability. He proposed of three stages through which a child goes
through while acquiring spelling and reading like logographic, alphabetic, and orthographic.
Literacy development begins with logographic reading where child acquires a small sight
vocabulary of written words and word recognition visually based but becomes increasing
less efficient with development. E.G.’ yellow' recognized by the “two sticks in the middle of
the word” 'follow' read as “yellow” due to the double ’l' shared by both words. In the
child’s wish to write brings about change from logographic stage to alphabetic stage. Here
by practicing spelling child learns that spoken words can be broken down into speech sounds
(phonemes) that map onto letters. Even though child applies letter-sound rules in spelling
but rely on visual cues for reading. In the orthographic stage through considerable practice
at reading using an alphabetic strategy child learns to recognize words as orthographic units.
Further in this stage, word recognition occurs by accessing stored internal representations of
are precise enough to be transferred to spelling. Thus, orthographic reading drives the
with Gentry’s stages which says that spelling shifts from phonetic, to transitional, to correct
spellings.
Writing is defined as a complex process linking language, thought and motor skills.
Early writing is more closely linked to early spelling than to early reading. When children
correspondence, whereas early readers rely on a visual approach to word recognition (they
tend to use contextual cues). Marsh, Friedman, Welch & Desberg (1980) investigated and
reported that at a later stage, most children seem to learn to alternate between the two
strategies. However this may not happen for children who have difficulty in learning to read.
As a result spelling is related to reading and written expression. Spelling is one of the
most valued yet difficult skills in written communication. Spelling requires matching the
sounds of language with the appropriate letters in order to accurately and reliably conveys
letters, sounds and syllable patterns (Bear & Templeton, 1998). In children with dyslexia
(CWD), damage to the orthographic input lexicon leading to inability to recognize words
lexically, so they would not spell lexically and so will be regularizing the irregular words (Bub,
exposure to print and word knowledge which emerges with appropriate teaching. Fletcher
et al (2003) has quoted that reading is not natural and everyone is taught to read. Those
children who lack the ability to learn literacy skills are considered ‘dyslexic’. Despite years of
research, mostly in the English language, there is still no consensus as to the definition of
dyslexia, nor its underlying cause. The current dominant view argues that the cause of the
awareness and word decoding skills. Smythe and Everatt (2000) define that
Lexical confusions and speed of processing difficulties may also be present. The
manifestation of dyslexia in any individual will depend upon not only individual
speaking, reading and writing, reasoning a or mathematical abilities. These disorders are
intrinsic to the individual and presumed to be due to central nervous system dysfunction
even though a learning disability may occur concomitantly with other handicapping
conditions(i.e. sensory impairment and mental retardation), social and economic
appropriate instruction, psychogenic factors), it is not the direct result of those conditions or
One of the evidences is provided by Scarborough (1990), who after examining the
very early language deficits in 52 children with dyslexia, grouped 30 month old subjects into
three sets. The first group had 20 children from families with an incidence of dyslexia who
subsequently became disabled readers; the second group had 12 children from families with
incidence of dyslexia became normal readers and the third group had 20 children who and
no history of dyslexia (i.e. they were normally achieving). Scarborough (1990) found that
children who demonstrated dyslexia usually experienced difficulty with three emergent
literacy skills during the preschool period. The results showed that, at 2.5 years they
produced shorter, syntactically simpler sentences with less accurate word pronunciations
than other 2 year old while demonstrating normal lexical or speech discrimination skills. At
three years of age, they began to demonstrate deficits in receptive vocabulary and object-
naming abilities. At 5 years of age, they exhibited problems in object naming, poor rhyme
recitation abilities, poor letter sound knowledge, and phonemic awareness deficits.
language and can be found in performance on a range of tasks that require phonological
processing (Gallagher, Frith, Snowling, 2000; Vellutino, Fletcher, Snowling, & Scanlon, 2004).
the sound structure of words separately from their meaning, grammar or spelling and is
heterogeneous in its genetic mechanisms. At least some forms of familial dyslexia appear to
be autosomal dominant, with linkage studies supporting both major locus on chromosome
gene(s) for dyslexia, sex hormones, and possible even concomitantly caused immunologic
responses in the development of brain in dyslexia. Findings from linkage studies suggest that
there are genes that lead to dyslexia associated with chromosomes 15 and 6.
Brain differences: The overabundance of tree like connections produced during the
(1991) found that the right hemisphere in brains of dyslexics have too many brain cells,
suggesting that something has interfered with the normal pruning process”. He reported
that the left hemisphere planum temporale was larger in the brains of reading disabled
neuronal substrates. According to him an optimal match is needed between the number of
neurons and their connections in a neural net so that a particular behavior can be achieved.
Too many or too few neuron match ups can be deleterious for the developing skill. He also
hypothesized that the neurons in questions ate not only misplaced, but the affected cortex is
different in terms of its cellular and connectional architecture, hence its functional architecture
as well.
Miller & Fitch, 1993) maintained that temporal mechanisms in the nervous system play a
central role in aspects of information processing and production, and may be especially
critical foe the normal production and maintenance of sensory motor integration systems as
well as phonological systems. Findings from a series of studies beginning in the 1970s, led
Tallal and colleagues to conclude that some students with developmental language and
components of information that enter the nervous system in rapid succession, and a
concomitant motor deficit in organizing rapid sequential motor output (Tallal et al.,
range.
Selective attention and attention deficit disorder: The primary purpose of reading is
to obtain the author’s intended meaning. To do this one, must proceed through a series of
reading skills. The most fundamental of these is the ability to attend selectively to relevant
features while ignoring irrelevant stimuli. Selective attention develops with maturation and
learning. A lag in its development can contribute to difficulties in reading, writing and
spelling. When more then one learning disability co-exists with dyslexia, the co morbid
specific reading disability and in many cases, co-occurs with it. Zentall (1993) found that
students with ADHD are more likely to receive lower grades in academic subjects and on
standardized reading and math tests that their normally achieving peers.
Middle ear problems: Roberts & Medley (1995) hypothesized that hearing loss
associated with persistent and /or recurrent OME causes the child to receive a partial or
inconsistent auditory signal. The child consequently encodes information incompletely and
inaccurately into the database from which language develops. A child may then be at a
disadvantage for learning speech and language. This, in turn may negatively influence later
Additionally, a child who hears a decreased or inconsistent auditory signal may tune sound
Cognitive rigidity and learned helplessness: Possibly as a result of one or more of the
above factors, many students with specific reading disabilities display cognitive rigidity that
slows information retrieval. Clay (1984) suggested that students with reading disabilities
have stopped making appropriate responses, and instead, rigidly specialize unparticular
Morton-Frith (1993). According to the model, four factors namely biological, cognitive,
behavioral and environmental influences are essential for normal reading development.
to biological factors, which is internal and exhibited through reading problems. Cognitive
of processing and lexical access too contribute to the successful reading .Other external
environmental factors like language abilities of the child, teaching at school, socio economic
status of the child and culture in which the child is being raised are assumed to be
influencing the reading development of the child. Because any of the causal and contributing
factors previously presented may interfere with the normal, developmental progression of
reading acquisition, a child may enter school unprepared for the materials and approaches
used in the regular curriculum. Smythe (1999) reports, dyslexia may be caused by a
combination of phonological, visual and auditory processing deficits. He adds that Word
retrieval and speed of processing difficulties may also be present. A number of possible
underlying biological causes of these cognitive deficits have been identified, and it is
probable that in any one individual there may be several causes .In addition dyslexia may
also be related to a number of problems which may include some or all of the following:
Making errors with numbers (telephone numbers, reversing bus number etc.)
While the dyslexics may experience difficulties in the acquisition of reading, writing
and spelling, they can be taught to find strategies and alternative learning methods to
overcome most of these and other difficulties. However some problems, such as poor
spelling, may persist into adulthood. He also admits that every dyslexic is different, and
should be treated as an individual. Werker & Tees (1987), revealed auditory processing
deficit in dyslexics. Study by Tallal, Miller & Fitch (1993) from their study after extensive
research about the processes those are affected in dyslexics concluded that temporal aspect
in auditory and visual processing are deficient in dyslexics. They explain that the acquisition
of higher level speech processing of basic sensory information entering the nervous system
in rapid succession, ‘within milliseconds’. Thus there is a basic temporal impairment, a tenth
of millisecond delay in the time information from peripheral sensory apparatus is relayed to
the central nervous system .The delay in temporal integration causes a cascade effects,
starting with the normal development of efficient phonological abilities, and in turn resulting
in subsequent failure to learn to speak and read normally. Normal temporal sequencing is
the stream of speech and combined to form words (Tallal, Merzenich, Miller & Jenkns,
1998).
The deficit in temporal sequencing however is not limited to the auditory area. The
magnocellular visual pathway that integrates rapid reception and rapid processing of visual
information has been described as poorly functioning in dyslexia compared with the slower
processing component of the visual system, the parvocellular system, the component
Cognitive neuropsychological dual route models (see Figure 1) propose that a skilled
reader uses two main procedures for converting print into speech which are referred to as
the lexical and sublexical processing routes. The lexical reading route relies on visual word
recognition through access to an internal store of learned familiar words. This route allows
successful and efficient processing of familiar words (both regular and irregular), but not
novel words or pronounceable nonwords, thus the need for an alternative sublexical reading
conversion and allows the skilled reader to “sound-out” unfamiliar words and nonwords.
The development of cognitive models of reading has largely been based on research
investigating normal skilled reading and acquired reading disorders of adulthood. However,
the suitability of applying such models to developmental disorders has been debated
( Bishop, 1997; Frith, 1985; Seymour, 1987; Seymour & Bunce, 1994) due to their failure to
specify explicitly the components and contents of partially formed systems and the
principles by which an “illiterate” cognitive system changes or develops into a fully skilled
adult system (Seymour, 1990). Developmental stage models have been proposed as an
developmental aspects of learning to read and stress that different reading and spelling
processes are favored at various stages of normal reading development. They stress the
lies in the formulation of a “static” model that can successfully represent a fluid (i.e.,
early reading, as well as the possibility that the manner in which children acquire reading
skills depends on teaching strategies employed in the classroom (Mann, 1986; Seymour,
1987; Seymour & Elder, 1986; Seymour & Evans, 1992; Stuart, 1995; Stuart, Masterson, &
Dixon, 2000).
There is now growing evidence that current dual route models of adult skilled
reading can be applied to developmental reading disorders (e.g., Castles & Coltheart, 1993;
Coltheart, Bates & Castles, 1994). More specifically, one can propose that developmental
dyslexia reflects a selective failure or delay in the acquisition of specific components of the
model (Coltheart et al., 1994). A growing number of case studies of developmental dyslexia
as well as studies of normal reading development are supporting this proposal (e.g., Castles
& Coltheart, 1993; Coltheart et al., 1983; Edwards & Hogben, 1999; Hanley, Hastie & Kay,
1992; Temple & Marshall, 1983). It has been hypothesized that the representations of
phonological dyslexics (Elbro, 1996; Hulme & Snowling, 1992 and Snowling, 2000). In surface
dyslexics the disconnection is in between the semantics and phonological functions leaving
only the sub lexical route in operation (see Figure.1). So, the reading of non words is
relatively intact in this population. Most studies in literature have found that surface
dyslexics had weaker phonological deficits than phonological dyslexics (Manis, McBride-
Chang, Seidenberg, Keating, Doi & Munson, 1997; Stanovich, Siegel & Gottardo, 1997).
Castles and Coltheart (1993) propose that children with developmental dyslexia can
be broadly classified into two main subtypes, those with a relative delay in the development
of lexical or sublexical reading skills, commonly referred to as surface and phonological
dyslexia, respectively. Current classification systems such as this readily highlight children
with specific reading disorders (e.g., surface or phonological dyslexia). However, there is a
significant proportion of children who present with a “mixed” picture of reading difficulties
incorporating characteristics of both phonological and surface dyslexia subtypes (Edwards &
Hogben, 1999), the mixed dyslexia subtype. Cognitive neuropsychologist s have paid little
attention to this subgroup and it has been suggested that the cognitive neuropsychological
approach “may not be very useful” for them in clinical settings (Coltheart et al., 1994).
Although a child with mixed dyslexia has deficits in both the lexical and sublexical
processing routes, both have numerous sub-components, any one (or more) of which could
processing routes the nature of impairment and general treatment focus is difficult to
determine.
There are several subtypes of dyslexia reported in literature, in some cases, Dyslexia
Among the well-known classification systems for subtypes of dyslexia is Boder’s classification
(Boder, 1973). It classifies dyslexics into dysphonetic (having difficulties especially with
grapheme–phoneme conversion, i.e. with the indirect route for reading), dyseidetic (having
difficulties with visual recognition of whole words, i.e. with the direct route for reading) and
mixed (having both types of difficulties) subtypes. Another sub typing system is Bakker’s
classification (Bakker, 1979, 1990) into P-types (relying on perceptual, analytical strategies
for reading, which turns out to be slow, fragmented and hesitating), L-types (relying on
linguistic, anticipatory strategies for reading, which allow for quicker reading but produce
many, usually plausible and context-based errors) and M-types (mixed types, showing both
slow, fragmented reading and many errors). In Bakker’s model, the two subtypes are
reading process, the right hemisphere being more activated in reading for P-types, the left
one for L-types. Both classification systems, therefore, suggest that visual functions can be
reading aloud: the dual route cascaded model (DRC) (Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, &
Ziegler, 2001). The authors designed four tasks to assess each representational level of the
DRC.i.e, letter level, orthographic lexicon, phonological lexicon, and phoneme system. The
data showed no single cause of dyslexia, but rather a complex pattern of phonological,
phonemic, and letter processing deficits. Importantly, most dyslexics had deficits in more
than one domain. Sub typing analyses also suggested that both the phonological and surface
dyslexics almost always had more than a single underlying deficit. Manis, Seidenberg, Doi,
McBride-Chang and Petersen (1996) in their study reported of two subgroups that formed
dyslexics from their sample of 68 reading disabled 3rd grade children by comparing them to
chronological age controls on exception word and pseudo word reading. However, when the
dyslexic subtypes were defined by referring to Reading level controls, 17 phonological and
only one surface dyslexic were identified. When the chronological age defined subtypes
were compared to reading level controls, the phonological dyslexics displayed superior
displayed a cognitive profile remarkably similar to that of the reading level controls.
The cognitive mechanism underlying subtypes Dyslexia are still a matter of debate.
Numerous theoretical approaches have identified different potential causes of dyslexia. The
phonological theory (Snowling, 2000) which is the most influential account for reading
Foorman (1997) and Adams (1990) also suggests that children who learn to read
alphabetical language system such as English proven to be having poor phonemic awareness
tasks like rhyming and letter identification. In contrast, the auditory processing deficit theory
(Tallal, 1980) assumes that dyslexics have deficit in (rapid) auditory processing .According to
this theory; phonological problems are only secondary to the auditory deficits. Yet other
researchers conceptualize dyslexia as a visual processing deficit arising from the impairment
of the visual magnocellular system in the brain (Stein & Walsh, 1997).The role of attentional
deficits for the development of dyslexia is also discussed (Hari & Renvall 2001).Attentional
deficits are thought to interfere with the encoding of a sequence of letters, resulting in the
confusion of letters and visual word forms. Interestingly, attentional deficits can be
dissociated from phonological deficits, and both types of deficits are valid predictors for
Finally, the cerebellar theory (Nicolson, Fawcett, Berry, Jenkins, Deen & Brooks,
1999; Nicolson, Fawcett & Dean, 2001) argues that reading disabilities are a consequence of
the impaired ability to automatize processes. It is assumed that the cerebellum supports the
automatisation of basic articulatory and auditory abilities which are relevant for the
evidence. Interestingly , however, not all dyslexics suffer from deficits in all cognitive
domains or profit equally from all remediation techniques (Ramus,2 003).Thus, it is possible
that distinguishable phenotypes of dyslexia exist on the cognitive level (Ho, Chan, Lee, Tsang
& Luan, 2000;& Ramus, 2004) for which universal or distinct genetic (Olson, 2002) and
specifically targeted and thus more efficient remediation strategies .Likewise there can be a
group. So the present study aims at sub grouping based on profile that will be obtained.
Several assessment tools are available for the assessment of dyslexia. Those can be
broadly divided in to tests for reading and tests for writing. The assessment tools those are
In the cloze procedure, the student must read the passage and provide the missing
words by analyzing the content and its structure. This procedure measures the reader’s
ability to interpret written passages, and it requires the student to use both comprehension
skills and knowledge of linguistic structure. The percentage of correct responses can be
determined and can serve as a rough indication of the reading level of the student. Usually
every fifth or sixth word is deleted called fixed ratio approach, however Farhady and
Keramati (1996) found that using this method versus other close methods resulted in
knowledge of linguistic structure in the maze procedure .This procedure is similar to the
cloze method, except that vertically presented choices are given instead of blanks. Parker,
Asbrouck, and Tindal (1992) reviewed twenty years of research on the maze procedure and
reported overall support of the technique of the maze procedure. Both the cloze and the
assessment devices.
Diagnostic assessment tools helps screening the patient for the problem, identifying
the problem and helps in informal determination of objectives and teaching strategies, and
also in documentation of educational needs, and establishment of IEP goals. Gray oral
administered set of reading tasks designed for children ages 51/2 through 12. It assesses the
contextual analysis, word ordering abilities. This test covers the main areas of oral reading
and comprehension, although it contains a large number of structural analysis tasks as well.
strengths and weaknesses in reading. It is used with students from the middle first grade
through grade 12.The authors suggest that the results from the SDRT4 can be used to help in
grouping students and in developing appropriate instructional strategies. The SDRT4 places
special emphasis on the low achievement student, and it contains more easy items than
many reading achievement test. This measures four major components of reading:
Vocabulary, phonetic analysis, comprehension and scanning. It has got six levels each
Green level-Used with students at the middle of grade 3 to the middle of grade 4
Brown level-Used with students from 6.5 to 8.9 and with low achieving high school
students.
Blue level-Designed for use with students from grades 9 through 12.
For each level, several tests are given that measure performance in such areas as
and structural analysis. There is one form for the first three levels and two forms for the last
three. The SDRT4 is a well constructed diagnostic reading test. Its standardization sample
was large and representative and the reliability and the validity good. It can therefore be
scanning, particularly for screening purposes. It gives more normative information but less
informal information like error analysis than other diagnostic reading tests.
is an individually administered instrument designed for students between the ages of 7 and
18.It can also be used with small groups of students. The test measures the comprehension
of silent reading; therefore, individuals who have articulation or other oral reading problems
are not penalized.8 subtests are included in the TORC-3.Subtests 1 to 4 are called the
reading. The TORC3 is theoretically a measure of silent reading comprehension .Only a small
part of the test actually focuses on traditional comprehension measures (that is, on reading
includes six individually administered tests and a two-part supplementary checklist designed
for individuals ages 5 to 75 and older. This test can be used for a variety of reasons including
diagnosis of reading problems, program planning, and program evaluation. The subtests
include, Visual –Auditory learning, letter identification, Word identification, Word attack,
is probably the most popular norm- referenced diagnostic reading test used in special
education. The tests are easy to administer and cover a number of abilities. It is however
very tedious to score, so it is recommended the scores uses computer version of scoring. It
appears that WRMT-R has improved many of the shortcomings of the original test. Although
the norms are relatively new, the test itself, including the format and items are over 10 years
old. Thus, there may be some questions about the items being consistent with today’s
curriculum.
Oral and Written language scales (OWLS) Carrow and Woolfolk (1995). It is a Written
expression scale designed for individuals ages 5 through 21(the oral scales are for ages 3
through 21).It measures the individual’s ability to communicate using written linguistic
forms. The test measures written expression in three areas using two different methods. The
complex sentences, different verb forms)and content(e.g. appropriate word choice ,subject
matter).These areas are measured through indirect writing tasks such as writing dictated
sentences or combining sentences, and direct writing tasks. This test provides a potential
valuable measure of both the mechanics and fluency of writing. A wide variety of derived
scores are available for comparison purposes .One disadvantage is that the Written
Expression Scale is packed and sold separately from the Oral Scales of the OWLS. It would
seem that the advantage of the OWLS is in giving an overall picture of language using the
same comparison group. Having to use two separate tests tends to discourage that purpose.
Test of Written expression (TOWE) by McGhee, Bryant, Larsen and Rivera, (1995). It
semantics, syntax, spelling, capitalization, and punctuation. The TOWE is designed for use
with students from ages 6 1/5 years through 14 years, 11 months. According to the TOWE
authors, the test can be used to identify students who have writing problems and to
discover the writing strength and weaknesses. It can also be used to help evaluate a
student’s progress and can be used by researchers who are interested in studying the area
of written expression. The major sections of this test are items section and essay section.
Punctuation, and Spelling. The essay component of TOWE requires the student to write an
developed to identify students who have problems with written expression, to indicate
strengths and weaknesses in writing language skills, to document progress, and to aid in
research studies related to the writing process. OWL 3 can be either individually or group
administered and is designed to be used with students ages 7 through 17.The authors divide
the area of written language into three components :conventional, linguistic and cognitive.
They also refer to two formants or methods of eliciting writing samples: contrived and
spontaneous. There are eight subtests on the TOWL-3.The first five are considered
contrived, and the second three are considered spontaneous. The capacity to use written
developed to offer the special educator a measure in that area. The TOWL 3 measures
several components of written language and appears to be a considerable improvement
administered or group administered test designed to pinpoint a child’s written spelling level
and to specify the types of words with which a child is having problem. Its use as a group
administered instrument is somewhat limited, however it is designed for use with children
ages 6 to 18.the TWS 3 comprises one hundred spelling words divided in to two categories:
Predictable and Unpredictable. The predictable subtest includes fifty words; these conform
to usual spelling rules and generalizations .the unpredictable subtest also includes fifty
words; these words do not follow the usual rules and therefore, primarily have to be
memorized. Although the TWS-3 yields normative data, its real value lies in the informal use
of the word lists. The idea of having predictable and unpredictable word lists is a good one.
The reliability of the newest version has improved over the earlier version, however which
However well standardized the assessment tools are the heterogeneity in the
dyslexic group poses the requirement to profile the learning disabilities on individual based
education plan (IEP) that clearly identifies the individual’s strengths and weaknesses, their
specific needs, and the timescale and resources required to implement the IEP. It should
attempt to match the teaching style to the preferred learning style of the individual in order
to maximize the amount learnt in a given time. The term dyslexia may be used to refer to
children presenting very different profiles of strengths and weaknesses, and therefore may
be of limited use in determining the IEP. What is required is greater specificity, such as a
child’s ability in phonological discrimination and rhyming tasks, which may inform teaching
practice. This need to understand the cognitive profile and attainment measures becomes
even more important with the assessment of the multilingual individual, since behavioral
outcomes, such as literacy difficulties, can manifest in very different ways across different
language contexts. Profiling means a lot especially in the Indian multilingual contexts as it is
difficult to generalize about the assessment and remediation of the multilingual dyslexic
individual since the context of language, culture and learning environment, can be very
diverse.
developing or applying specifically targeted and thus more efficient remediation strategies .
Hence, the skills based on which they can be profiled should be assessed and a standard
scores should be developed based on which the dyslexic group can be compared.
Meanwhile, the high prevalence rate of learning disability (3% to 10%, Ramaa, 2000)
indicates the need for early identification and intervention based on individual
performances. The lack of Indian tools for assessment of dyslexia and the heterogeneity of
dyslexic group urges to develop a tool to profile dyslexics based on individual characteristics.
CHAPTER 3
Method
3.1. Participants
The participants of the present study included two groups. One group consisted of
60 school going normal children from grades I, II, III, IV and V. Each grade consisted of twelve
children. The other group consisted of 16 children with dyslexia (CWD) who were identified
as dyslexics at the institute using Test for Early Reading Skills (ERS) (Loomba, 1995) in the
clinical set up. All the participants were native Kannada speakers with English as the medium
of instruction.
Prabhjot and Kumar, 2007 ) and Developmental Screening Test (Bharathraj, 1972) was used
to screen for normal children in terms of hearing, intelligence, motor and other factors like
school performance, emotional or behavioral factors. The participants either had normal or
corrected-to-normal vision. Written consent was obtained from all the participants prior to
testing. This study was conducted with the understanding and consent of the participants
and their parents. They were provided information in their language he/she was capable of
understanding and were explained about the aims, method of the research and approximate
duration of testing.
3.2. Procedure
The test was adopted from International Dyslexia Test (IDT) developed by Ian
Smythe (2000). IDT was earlier called as International cognitive profile test (ICPT), which
included vast domains of cognitive skills for assessment of literacy skills. The International
Dyslexia Test was born out of a research project to study the role of Orthography and
phonology in dyslexia, and was first publicized at the International Dyslexia Conference in
York in April 1997. To date (November 1999) the test has been “translated” into a number of
languages including Russian, Hungarian and Chinese, with a number of others currently in
progress. The authors of the test do not claim to answer the entire question, but the test can
lead to a greater understanding of dyslexia, and lead towards improved diagnosis and
remediation of the individual with dyslexia with Tasks that have been found to be crucial for
reading and writing skill in children have been adapted for the present study and the
rationale behind assessing them has been justified in the following sections. The final test
that was derived out of the IDT after revisions was called the Dyslexia assessment profile in
Alphabet
Shape copying
Spelling
Rapid naming
Sound discrimination
The first stage of decoding stage is to learn that reading involves use of codes.
Words are not written in arbitrary ways but according to an alphabetic principle by which
letters have a regular and predictable relationship with sounds. Children come to
understand that the alphabetic principle simplifies the reading process and that it is crucial
that they attend to all of the letters to read accurately .The reader must also know the
specific correspondence between letters and letter patterns and sounds. Acquiring this
knowledge is primary task of decoding stage (Loomba, 1995). The alphabet subtest will help
It is a test of visual perception and visual motor integration. This involves demonstrating
geometric forms from memory, tracing and copying (Crandall , Hammill, Witkowski &
Barkovich (1968).Shape copying will help us further to know whether the child’s fine motor
skills are ready and are prepared to be used for writing skills.
Spelling
Normal spelling needs auditory and visual reception, auditory and visual memory,
auditory and visual discrimination, association of auditory and visual stimuli, motor
expression and vocal expression. Ability to spell enables the writer to concentrate on the
ideas he wishes to convey rather than on the mechanics of writing. (Frostig and Maslow,
1967).
Reversals, Omissions and poor spacing are a characteristic of a young child’s writing.
It is the persistence of such errors over a long period of time that is indicative of writing
difficulty. Johnson and Myklebust (1967), suggest that such difficulties are associated with
deficiencies in visual motor integration (dysgraphia), revisualization (memory), and
formulation (syntax).
The rationale for assessing person’s reading rate lies in the centrality of automaticity.
Extremely slow reading stimulates the decoding of a student with Learning disability.
Decoding problems consume his/her cognitive resources, leaving little of them for
decode non-words highlights the grapheme phoneme translation difficulties may lie at the
heart of an individual’s specific learning difficulties. It also establishes the subject’s degree of
different meanings. It has been demonstrated that this prepares them well for learning
about orthographic links between words once they begin the formal construction of linking
phonemes to graphemes, (Maclean, Bryant & Bradley, 1987). The phonological awareness
like rhyming and alliteration skills have been found to become predictors of reading skill in
Word repetition in very young children have been found to be related to the vocabulary
size of the child ,(Gathercole & Baddeley ,1989 ; Hoff, Cotre & Bridges 2008) .The evidence
for deficits in a core phonological domain predicts that children with below average reading
ability will experience particular problems in print related decoding tasks such as pseudo
word reading .
Word and non word repetition may become measure of phonological working
memory capacity in spite of the fact that researchers have consistently acknowledged that
the task taps many language processes, including speech perception, phonological encoding,
lexical and sub lexical properties. The Non word repetition can be used to examine the
structural properties of the lexicon in both children with normal language and with specific
Rapid naming
Naming speed task assess the rate at which a verbal label for high frequency visual
stimuli is produced. If an individual takes much longer than average to name all the stimuli,
that individual is said have a naming speed deficit. Bowers and Wolf (1993) stated that slow
naming speed is implicated in failure to learn to recognize words quickly. Wolf, Bowers and
Biddle (2000) stated that naming speed (particularly serial naming speed) provides an early,
simpler approximation of the reading process. They believed that naming speed is
phonological, semantic and motoric sub processes that place heavy emphasis on precise
Sound discrimination
Phoneme discrimination is the auditory processing skill, where a few Children with
dyslexia have been reported to have difficulties in discriminating between two similar
The tasks of the material have been divided under phonological and non phonological
depending on the rationale those they assess phonological skills specifically and non-
phonological skills in general. The tasks those are considered as phonological are Alphabet,
SDis, NWreading, NWrep, alliteration and rhyming. And the tasks those are considered as
non phonological are Wrep, Wreading, HQ, SC, RN and spelling. The following Table shows
Testing environment
The test was administered in a quiet, noise free set up using a paper and pencil only.
Table 1 below shows the subtests for the present study and the instructions that were given
o the children prior to testing each of the tasks. The scoring was done as given in the Table 1.
1- Least clear
(The shape that is displayed supposedly shape.
the most complicated) Score:7
Score:3
Maximum score:10
1- Bad handwriting.
Total score: 5
4. Reading Has to read the words that are 1- Correct
word(Wreading) given(list consists of few irregular
words) 0- Incorrect
Total score: 70
5. Reading non Has to read the non words that are 1- Correct
word(NWreading) given
0- Incorrect
Total score: 10
Total score: 7
Total score: 8
8. Rhyming and Has to find out the word which are in 1- Correct
Alliteration rhyme
0- Incorrect
E.g. Bat, Mat, Wall- Here “Bat” and
“Mat” are in rhyme with each other Total score: 30
where as the ‘Wall” is not Rhyming:20
He has to find out the words which are Alliteration:10
in alliteration with other words.
9. Rapid naming(RN) Has to name the pictures those are Time taken to
given. completely name
all the pictures.
timed test. On the other tasks the performance was scored for correct and incorrect
responses. The responses were coded .The data was subjected to quantitative (details of
statistical procedure are mentioned in the results section) analysis and further a qualitative
analysis was also done. The results of this study have been discussed in the following
sections.
CHAPTER 4
Results
II. To identify subtypes of dyslexia based on the profiles established on children with
dyslexia
The following statistical techniques were used to analyze the data obtained from the
study:
across grades.
b) A post-hoc Duncan analysis was done to explore which grades are significantly
c) Kruskal-Wallis, test was used to check for performance of children with Dyslexia
on various tasks
d) Bonferroni test was carried out to explore the performance of children across tasks
and if they were different across grades in Children with dyslexia (CWD) group.
d)e)A multiple hierarchical regression analysis was carried out on normal group to find
out the best predictors of reading skill among all the tasks
e)f) Cluster analysis was done to derive the different clusters /subtypes among children
The results of the study are described under the following sections,
4.2 .Comparison of performance of normal children and children with dyslexia (CWD).
Participants for the normal group consisted of 60 children from grades I to V. Each
grade consisted of 12 children. All the 12 tasks were administered on all the subjects and
variable and task as dependent variable. Table 2 shows mean and standard deviation (SD)
The Table 2 shows that the performance of normal children improved from lower
grades to higher grades on tasks like HQ, SC, spelling and Wreading. Thus, showing a
developmental trend on these tasks. It is evident from the Table, on the alphabet subset, all
children in the grade I itself were able to score the maximum .This indicates that the
Grades
Tasks I II III IV V
Alphabet 9 0 9 0 9 0 9 0 9 0
HQ 3.83 0.94 4.00 0.74 4.17 0.72 4.67 0.89 4.83 0.39
RN 37.67 4.12 40.92 7.66 39.50 5.27 31.50 4.96 25.75 4.00
SC 8.08 2.07 8.75 1.22 8.92 1.24 9.42 .90 9.92 0.29
NW reading 5.17 1.75 5.08 2.78 7.00 2.26 7.33 1.83 7.58 1.97
Alliteration 2.92 2.27 3.50 2.71 6.42 2.15 6.17 2.40 9.08 1.24
Rhyming 3.92 3.39 0.42 0.99 7.33 2.35 10.33 4.70 14.50 3.00
Wrep 4.17 1.11 3.75 1.06 4.92 0.90 5.58 0.99 5.25 0.96
NWrep 4.33 0.89 4.17 1.53 5.17 1.11 6.17 0.72 6.50 0.67
SDis 17.42 1.88 17.08 1.56 18.67 1.15 19.58 1.44 20.00 0
Spelling 8.67 3.85 8.75 2.14 16.00 3.36 18.42 7.09 29.25 3.16
Wreading 15.58 7.23 20.08 4.17 26.67 4.03 31.25 10.97 54.67 6.36
[Handwriting Quality(HQ), Rapid Naming(RN), Shape Copying(SC), Non Word Reading (NWreading),
reading)]
On the handwriting quality (HQ) task, results showed that children in the higher
grades showed better performance than the lower grades (see Table 2) and significant
difference was noticed at F (4, 55) =3.88, p< 0.01. Post –Hoc Duncan results showed no
significant difference among grades I through III, however children in grade V performed
significantly better than Grade IV. On the shape copying (SC) task, results revealed that
children in the higher grades showed better performance than the lower grades (see Table
2) and significant difference was noticed at F (4, 55) =3.53, p<0.05 . Post –Hoc Duncan
results showed no significant difference among grades I through III, however grade V
participants performed significantly better than grade IV. On both spelling and reading tasks,
results revealed that children in the higher grades showed better performance than the
lower grades (see table 2). For the spelling task, significant difference was found at F (4, 55)
=47.44, p<0.001. For the reading task, significant difference was found at F (4, 55) =56.35,
p<0.001. The Post-hoc Duncan analysis revealed that grades I and II were not significantly
different from each other, while grade III performed significantly better than grade II. The
results also showed that grade III and IV were not significantly different from each other;
Overall, there was a developmental pattern observed on tasks like handwriting quality (HQ),
NWreading, rhyming, Wrep, NWrep and SDis, showed that children in grade I performed
better than grade II, this was not found to be statistically significant. However, the
performances of children improved from grade III to grade V on these tasks significantly. On
word repetition (Wrep) task, results showed that the performance of children improved from
grade III to grade V and this was found to be significant at F (4, 55) =6.81, p<0.001. On the
non word repetition (NWrep) task , results showed that the performance of children
improved from grade III to grade V and this was found to be significant at F (4, 55) =12.44,
p<0.001 level of significance. On the sound discrimination (SDis) task results revealed that
the performance of children improved from grade III to grade V and this was found to be
significant at F (4, 55) =10.56, p<0.001. For all of these three tasks, Post-hoc Duncan analysis
revealed no significant difference between grade I and II, while it showed that grade III was
significantly different than grade I and II . There was no significant difference between grade
IV and V on this task. Performance of children in grade IV and V was found to be better than
grade III.
On the non word reading (NWreading) task results, showed that grade I performed
better than grade II, and the performance of children improved from grade III to grade
V(see Table 2) and this was found to be significant at F (4, 55) =3.82, p <0.01 level of
significance. Post-hoc Duncan analysis revealed that there was no significant difference
across grade I and grade II. There was no significant difference across grades III, IV and V.
On rhyming task, significant at F (4, 55) =36.62, p<0.001 level of significance. Surprisingly,
grade II children performed significantly poorer than grade I children; however from grade III
through V the performance improved. Post-hoc Duncan analysis revealed that all the grades
performed significantly different from each other on this task and an increasing trend was
observed for d V were in increasing order (see Figure 2). Also to note, a high standard
deviation score revealed a high variability of performance amongst children on rhyming task.
On the alliteration task the performance of normal improved as the grade increased,
the pattern was observed only till grade III, while the children in grade IV students
performed better than grade V (see Table 2). For the alliteration task significant difference
was noticed at F (4, 55) =15.15, p< 0.001 level of significance. Post-hoc Duncan analysis did
not show any significant difference between grades I and II, and between grade IV and grade
III, grade V performed significantly better than grade III and IV. On the rapid naming (RN)
task, grade I took more time compared to grade III. Grade V took lesser time compared to
grade IV .Grade II took more time than grade I (see Table 2). For the RN task significant
difference was noticed at F (4, 55) =16.69, p< 0.01 level of significance. Post –Hoc Duncan
results showed no significant difference among grades I through III, however grade V
grades.
As shown in Figure 2 and 3 describes, the performances on tasks has been found to
be increasing grades, on rhyming task where the grade II have performed worse than grade
I. The trend is not exactly in increasing pattern on tasks of Wrep and SDis as well.
HQ,SC,alphabet,spelling,Wreading,NWreading,alliteration,rhyming,Wrep,NWrep,
RN,SDis.The Figures shows that there is a developmental trend across all the tasks however,
Figure: 3.Performence of normal on tasks (rhyming, Wrep, NWrep, SDis, spelling and
regression equation which was significant at F (9, 50) =44.73, p< 0.01.Combined reading task
which included word reading (Wreading) and nonword reading (NWreading) was considered
to arrive at the regression equation. The multiple regression for the entire sample revealed
that rapid naming, alliteration, sound discrimination and spelling were the four significant
predictors for the reading score (r2=0.89; Rapid Naming β =-.469; p<0.05; Alliteration β
=1.016; p<0.05; Sound Discrimination β =-1.54; p<0.05; Spelling: β=1.3 03; p<0.001.
The CWD group consisted of sixteen children with dyslexia (CWD) who participated
in the study. There were three CWD in each group of grade I, grade II, grade III and grade V.
Only grade IV consisted of four CWD. An individual profile of each child with dyslexia was
The data was further analyzed using Kruskal-Wallis non parametric test to check for
performance of children with dyslexia on various tasks. Results showed that the
performance of children with dyslexia on tasks of alphabet, SC, NWreading, and Wrep were
deviation (SD) to compare normal children and CWD, since they are two different groups,
following Table 3 displays the mean and SD for overall performance of normal children and
CWD.
Table 3 displays the mean and standard deviation of two groups of the study.In
order to explore the tasks those are significantly different among normal and CWD groups,
Mann-Whitney U test was administered, and results revealed that they are significant at p<
0.05. Results showed that normal group were significantly better than CWD group on most
of the tasks but CWD performed as good as normal children on phonological awareness
tasks like alliteration, rhyming and on SDis. Figure 4 shows compare the performances of
normal and CWD groups across all the tasks across grades I to V.
Groups
Subtests Normal CWD
N Mean SD N Mean SD
HQ 60 4.30 .83 16 2.44 1.26
RN 60 35.07 7.71 16 50.63 20.30
Alphabet 60 9.00 .00 16 7.94 1.91
SC 60 9.017 1.38 16 6.56 2.37
NWreading 60 6.43 2.35 16 2.75 1.39
Alliteration 60 5.62 3.10 16 3.94 3.13
Rhyme 60 7.30 5.79 16 4.12 4.21
Wrep 60 4.73 1.19 16 4.00 1.09
NWrep 60 5.267 1.38 16 4.12 1.26
SD 60 18.55 1.76 16 16.94 5.94
[Handwriting Quality(HQ), Rapid Naming(RN), Shape Copying(SC), Non Word Reading (NWreading),
reading)]
As it can be evidently observed from the Figure, CWD were found to perform poorly
than the normal children on all tasks. Mann-Whitney U test was used to compare between
grades of normal and CWD group as well. The results are described below,
[HQ-Handwriting Quality, RN-Rapid Naming, SC-Shape Copying, NW reading-Non Word reading,
reading]
Figure: 4. Mean scores for normal children and children with dyslexia across tasks.
On the alphabet task CWD in lower grades performed poorer than normal, but it
was not statistically significant. In the grade I the tasks on those where CWD performed
similar to normal are alliteration, rhyming and Wrep. In the grade II, like grade I CWD
performed similar to normal, in tasks like alliteration, rhyming and Wrep. But unlike grade I
alphabet, SC, NWreading, alliteration, rhyming, spelling and on Wreading. Where as CWD
could match the performance of normal children on tasks of RN, Wrep, NWrep, and SDis.
Grade IV CWD performed significantly poorer only on tasks of HQ, RN, NWreading,
Wrep, and NWrep. On all the other tasks they were able to match the normal children
performance in this grade. Normal children in grade V and CWD was compared, the results
show that normal children of grade V performed better than CWD on most of the tasks, but
performed similar to CWDs on tasks of RN, alliteration, Wrep, NWrep and SDis.
On qualitative analysis it was found that, on spelling and handwriting quality tasks
words were seen in CWD compared to normal children. When the CWD were asked
to write the alphabets, they seemed to produce the mirror images of the alphabet
they were attempting to write (e.g. writing 9 for p), though these errors were
The scores of Shape Copying task was reflected in Handwriting Quality in both
Normal and CWD group. In other words children who performed better in Shape
Copying were found to have better handwriting quality and vice versa.
On the task of alliteration and rhyming, the CWD were not able to keep the
presented three words of one stimulus in their working memory in order to identify
the words those have the same first letter or words those are in rhyme with each
other. They also showed difficulty in understanding the instruction for these tasks,
and most of the times tried to identify the first two of three presented stimuli as the
appropriate response, which might have been due to their working memory deficit
rather than a deficit in alliteration and rhyming task itself. Though these similar
errors were seen in normal children as well, however, the errors found were lesser
and more errors were found in the earlier grades in normal children.
On the Wreading task, it was evident that CWD performed much poorer than the
normal children in reading words as well as non-words. A few CWD were able to
read non words with difficulty, however most of them (CWD) performed poorly on
this task. CWD failed to use the strategy of reading the word as whole there by
implementing lesser lexical cue compared to normal. When it came to reading non
On the repetition (words and nonwords), compared to normal children, the CWD
showed difficulty in repetition when the length of the stimuli increased from
monosyllabic to disyllabic words .Showing that the errors made are predominantly
On rapid naming task, one salient feature observed among the subjects was that
they tend to skip a whole series while naming given series of items. Otherwise there
children. CWD children took relatively more time in comparison to normal children.
On sound discrimination task, the quality of error observed in CWD was similar to
normal.
Cluster analysis was carried out as part of quantitative analysis for the CWD group in
various tasks. The tasks those were considered to arrive at overall Dendrogram were SDis,
Wrep, NWrep, Wreading, NWreading, alliteration, rhyming, RN and spelling. The results are
Following Figure 5 shows the Dendrogram obtained using phonological and non
phonological tasks.
Figure: 5 Dendrogram depicting all the clusters in the CWD group
cluster (Cluster I) themselves(see Appendix B). The typical characteristics of these clusters
were,
Subject 1 (Ia) also performed similar to this cluster but it fell slightly apart, since this subject
Subject 4(Ib) Having taken maximum time for performing the Rapid Naming task
falls slightly apart from Cluster the cluster I and Ia (see Appendix B).
Subjects 7 and 9 formed the cluster II. The typical features of this cluster were,
In the Wrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had four
words in it
In the NWrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had three
non words in it
Subjects scored just over ten in Wreading task, and most of the stimulus words read
In the NWreading task only single syllable words were read correctly by subjects in
Cluster III was formed by the subjects 8, 12 &13.The common features of this cluster were,
The time taken for RN was almost same duration (38 and 37 seconds respectively)
Subject 11 (IIIa) was slightly different from the cluster III, since its performance on spelling
Subjects 14, 15 and 16 fall in a same region of the Dendrogram forming its own cluster
On the Wrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had four
words in it
On the NWrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had three
non words in it
In the NWreading task only single syllable words were read correctly by subjects in
this cluster
Ultimately, lonely subject that didn’t seem to fall near any of the cluster was the 10 th
(X) subject (see Appendix B). The reason for being solace in the Dendrogram was that this
was the only subject who read even one of the bisyllabic words in NWreading
This was the only subject who read half of the words correctly in reading task (i.e.35 words),
and scored maximum in this task compared to all the other tasks.
Ia-1
Ib-4
Cluster II-7&9
IIIa-11
In order to classify the CWD into phonological and non phonological group the data
was further analyzed considering phonological and non phonological tasks separately. The
phonological tasks considered were SDis, NWrep, NWreading, alliteration and rhyming (see
Figure 6). The nonphonological tasks considered were Wreading, NWreading, RN and
The following Figure 6 shows the Dendrogram acquired for clusters among phonological
tasks.
In the NWrep task the performance was poor ,managed to repeat stimulus series
Subjects 1 and 4 formed the cluster b, the common phonological features they
In the NWrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had one
Cluster c was formed by subjects 7 and 9. The common phonological features of this
group were,
In the NWrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had three
In the NWreading task only single syllable words were read correctly by subjects in
Cluster d was formed by the subjects 8, 11 &13. The common phonological features of
Subject 12 formed the Cluster d1, since this subject performed fair in rhyming task.
Cluster e was formed by the subject 14 since this subject had salient feature of very
Cluster f was formed by the subject 15, the salient phonological feature it had from
other clusters was that it had very good performance on alliteration and fair performance
on rhyming task.
Cluster g is formed by subjects 10 and 16 and the common features these subjects
shared were that all the phonological tasks were performed fair to good. Even very good
Cluster a – 2, 3, 5 and 6
Cluster b- 1 and 4
Cluster c- 7 and 9
Cluster d- 8, 11 and 13
Cluster d1- 12
Cluster e- 14
Cluster f-15
The following Figure 7 shows the Dendrogram acquired for clusters among non phonological
tasks.
of this group were, that all the subjects performed poorly on Spelling, WReading and Wrep
Subject 7 and 9 forms the cluster (b), the common non phonological features they
shared were that they had good performance on Wrep and the correct responses in
very poor on Wrep thus forming a different cluster (see Appendix B).
Cluster (c) formed by 15 and 16, the features of this cluster were their fair
performance on Wrep and Spelling tasks and Very poor to poor performance in Wreading
Subject 10 formed the cluster (d) performed good on Wreading and spelling and
poor on Wrep.
Subject 14 formed the cluster (d1), since they had very poor performance in
Wreading and spelling. These clusters also had fair performance in RN and Wrep.
Subjects 2, 3, 5 and 6 forms the Cluster (e) the common non phonological features
were, They performed poorly on spelling and Wreading but fair performance on Wrep task.
Subject 1(e1) Spelled none of the words and read none of the words. The subject
repeated repeat only the first series of stimulus which had only two words in it.
Subject 4 formed the cluster (f) since they showed Very poor performance on
spelling and Wreading and fair performance on Wrep task (see Appendix B).
Cluster a- 8, 12 and 13
Cluster c- 15 and 16
Cluster d-10
Cluster d1-14
Cluster e- 2, 3, 5, and 6
Cluster e1-1
Cluster f-4
Table 4. Sub grouping of CWD under phonological, non phonological and mixed type.
Phonological Surface Mixed
Subjects were classified as phonological dyslexics {1, 4, 16, 14, 10} if they showed
difficulties on tasks of NWreading, NWrep, alliteration and rhyming. Subjects were classified
as surface dyslexics {15} if they find difficulties in tasks of Wreading, Wrep, RN and Spelling.
Subjects were classified as Mixed {2, 3, 5, 6, 8, 9, 11, 12, 13} if they show difficulty in both
To summarize, the quantitative clusters and the qualitative analysis reveal that the
group of CWD children which seem to have been a part of the study are of mostly the
phonological(7)and the mixed dyslexia subtypes(9), however one of the surface dyslexics(1)
also identified. More number of mixed groups was found in the study compared to the
phonological types. The data again shows the existence heterogeneity in children with
dyslexia.
CHAPTER 5
Discussion
Development of reading and writing skills based on the adopted IDT (Smythe, 2000)
in normal children from grade I through grade V. This section will also include
5.3 Development of reading and writing skills in normal children and performance of
CWD
Discussion for normal development and comparison of CWD with normal will be
discussed for tasks of alphabet, HQ, SC, spelling, Wreading and NWreading, Wrep and
with dyslexia in lower grades (I and II) showed errors in alphabet writing which resembled
mirror images of target letter (9 for p). The mirror writing feature observed in the lower
grades (I, II and III) of dyslexics could be due to directional confusion nature for forms of
CWD Directional confusion is the reason for reversing of letters, Directional confusion also
tend to occur in handedness (right or left) Hornsby (1999). However this mirror writing
features in this population disappeared as grades improved it was appeared in lower grades.
Visual errors have also been reported by Terepocki, Kruk, and Willows (2002) who compared
10-year-old average readers and children with reading disability. The children with reading
disability made more orientation errors than average readers on computer-based reversal
detection tasks (numbers, letters, letter strings, words), and more reversal errors on
controlled writing tasks. The authors suggest that the difficulties of reading disabled group in
letters. They concluded that although reversal errors are likely to disappear in children with
It is evident from the results of the present study performance of normal children
improved from grade I to grade V on both the visuo-motor tasks such as handwriting quality
and shape copying. This may be attributed to better fine motor control over this skill with
development. It was found that performance on both the motor tasks (handwriting quality
and shape copying) have increased parallelly from lower grades to higher grades suggesting
the interdependence of these two skills for writing skill. Qualitative analysis of their
handwriting revealed that children in the lower grade wrote large letters, poorly spaced and
offline writing. Whereas, those children in the higher grades like grade IV and grade V had
improved writing in terms of better spacing, smaller and more uniformity compared to the
lower grades. Gessel and Amatruda (1947) reported that around 5-6 years the child
improves in his/her grasping and co ordination and at about 6 years copies capital letters.
The authors also reported that they can write, but the writing is large, awkward, uneven,
and irregular in size and position at around 7 years. They added that penmanship becomes
smaller and more uniform at around 9 years. These findings of the present study evidenced
that these skills are acquired much earlier that they were ought to be as mentioned in the
With respect to children with dyslexia, the results of the present study showed that
CWD children were found to have poorer hand writing quality and shape copying in
dyslexia have been found to have poor fine motor abilities and often poor writing skills
(Denckla, 1985). Motor problems are frequently observed in dyslexic children (Snowling,
2000, 2005). It is estimated that about 60% of children with reading disability have a
developmental coordination disorder or some other problems with motor skills (Viholainen
characterized by difficulties with gross and fine motor movements (Snowling, 2000) in
5.1.3. Spelling
Results of the present study also showed that the performance on spelling task
increased from lower to higher grades. It was found that children in the earlier grades used
symbols from the alphabet but showed no knowledge of letter-sound correspondences
In the later grades children were found to make better letter-sound correspondences and
further higher grades (grade IV and grade V), children were able to understand the basic
spelling rules of English and use them appropriately for reading purpose. The results also
support Frith’s developmental model for reading (Frith, 1985) in which she proposed three
stages which a child goes through while acquiring spelling and reading like logographic,
alphabetic and orthographic stages. Logographic stage is a more primitive stage of learning
to read whereas orthographic stage is a much developed stage of reading and the alphabetic
stage is a transition from logographic to orthographic stage. From the present study, it is
evident that the errors made by normal children (e.g., writing ‘bot’ for ‘boat’) in the lower
grades (grade I and II) could be due to the transition period from alphabetic to orthographic
stage. Empirical support for the present findings has been drawn from other studies in
literature which uses connectionist model (Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989) of alphabetic
spelling development. Loosemore, Brown and Watson (1991), studied the development of
alphabetical spelling system using the artificial connectionist model and concluded that
during the early course of development, they observed regularization errors and this errors
faded as the model was exposed to more alphabetical spelling system. The findings of
present study are tallying with the findings of Richard et al., (1991), since normal children of
lower grades performed poorly on irregular words from the stimuli. They used regular
spellings for irregular words. (e.g. ‘hart’ for ‘heart’). As the grades increased, the normal
children of the present study performed better on irregular words suggesting the
construction of orthographic lexicon in them. This was evident when the data was
qualitatively analyzed. Thus, the present study supports various other studies suggesting
significant predictor of reading skill with the significant value of β=1.3 03; p<0.001.The
findings of the present study is in consonance with Read (1971), Clarke (1988), Dyson (2001),
Richgels (2001), Whitehurst and Lonigan, (2001) who also found spelling as a significant
predictor of reading in their study. This was evident when the data was qualitatively
analyzed. Thus, the present study supports various other studies suggesting different stages
On spelling tasks, CWD were found to perform poorer than the normal children in
the present study. Qualitative analysis revealed that CWD even in the higher grades showed
more errors in comparison to young normal children. The errors revealed that CWD in higher
grades who were supposed to have reached the orthographic stage of spelling (Frith, 1985)
stage. For e.g., some CWD wrote ‘butter’ as ‘better’, which means that these children are still
using the logographic strategy as the two words are visually similar. Other errors like
regularization errors were again found to be more in CWD compared to normal children.
orthographic lexicon. A study by Bub, Cancelliere and Kertesz, 1985, McCarthy and
Warrington, 1986 who discussed this as damage to the orthographic input lexicon because
of which CWD would not recognize words lexically, so they would not spell lexically and so
will be regularizing the irregular words. We can also hypothesize from Bub et al., 1985, and
McCarthy and Warrington, 1986) that CWD of higher grades have not completely acquired
the orthographic lexicon thus committing regularization errors (e.g. ‘cattel’ for ‘cattle’).
5.1.4. Word reading (Wreading) and Non word reading (NWreading)
The present study showed improved performance on word reading and non word
reading tasks from lower to higher grades in normal children. However, the performance of
word reading was comparatively better than the non word reading task in all the grades. The
beginners (grade I & II) seemed to read both the word and non word not analytically, but as
a whole (e.g. reading ‘yoll’ as ‘doll’). These findings are in consonance with Frith (1985)
whose model can be used to explain word reading. This could be attributed to lack of
other words, children in their lower grades treat a visual word as a visual object. It can be
hypothesize that these lower grades normal children are still in logographic to alphabetic
stage of Frith’s model. Typically, according to Frith this alphabetical stage is followed by
orthographic stage. This refers to the mastery of the alphabetic principle of phonography
according to which written words may be segregated into left to right series of letters, each
of which can be decoded as standing for as segment of speech. These segments correspond
to the linguistic abstractions, the phonemes, by which the set of vowels and consonants
The features observed in the present study can be contradicted with Beers (2007)
where he suggests that the first two stages of reading development (discusses only word
reading where as this stages does not explain non word reading). Stage I in her study
included decoding Stage (grades 1-2; Ages 6-7) where the student's central task is learning
arbitrary letters and associating them with corresponding parts of spoken words. The
children in grades I and II of the present study didn’t seem to use decoding strategy (letter
by letter reading) to read the given word (where they seemed to use logographic skills). The
next stage according to her is confirmation, fluency, ungluing from print, and automaticity
stage (grades 2-3; Ages 7-8) where readers do experiments with their phonic skill. This stage
is also a consolidation of what was learned in Stage 1. The children in present study of these
grades (grade III) tried experimenting with their phonic skills; this was evident from their
inquisitiveness to read the new words with their phonic skills, which enabled them to
improve in their reading scores (as you can see the increase in reading scores gradually). It
was also evident that children in these stage of the present study read words correctly even
tough they had not encountered those words in past (lack of prior word knowledge). She
calls the third stage as Reading for Learning the New Stage (Grades 4-8; ages 9-13) where
the readers need to bring prior knowledge to their readings which enable them to acquire
facts. Grade IV and V of our present study performed better compared to lower grades since
Poor performance of normal children lower grades on non word reading task can be
explained using the developmental stage models of reading like the dual route models
suggested in literature (Newcombe & Marshall, 1984; Castles & Coltheart, 1993). The
performance of normal children in present study improved from lower grade through higher
grades. To read a non word, it is the sub lexical processing which is important. It involves
rule-based grapheme-to phoneme conversion and allows the skilled reader to “sound-out”
unfamiliar words and nonwords. Results of the present study are indicative that probably in
the lower grades, children have still not developed the component process (GPC buffer in
case reading a non word) that requires sub lexical processing to read a non word correctly.
The present study supports various other studies who discuss about development of reading
The performance of CWD was significantly poorer than normal in both word reading
and non word reading tasks. Snowling (2000) reported that CWD have problems at the
phonological level, which leads to the expectation that they should be at least slow, and at
worst fail, to develop alphabetical decoding skills .The poor performance of CWD on
Wreading can be explained through Frith’s model (1985) of reading development. According
stage it is possible to assign the appropriate phoneme to graphemes. In the present study
we can hypothesize that, this stage of development is deficient in CWD, thus this population
find difficulties reading word. Poor performance of non word reading in CWD can again be
explained based on dual route models (Newcombe & Marshall, 1984) like for normal
selective failure or delay in the acquisition of specific components of the model (Coltheart et
al., 1994). CWD even in the higher grades were found to perform poorly in the present study.
It could be that CWD have still not developed the component process (GPC buffer in case
reading a non word) that requires sub lexical processing to read a non word correctly. The
present study supports various other studies who discuss about reading in developmental
dyslexia using various models (e.g., Castles & Coltheart, 1993; Coltheart et al., 1983;
Edwards & Hogben, 1999; Hanley, Hastie, & Kay, 1992; Temple, 1984; Temple & Marshall,
1983). Thus, an inadequate development of GPC buffer might be the reason for poor
On the word repetition task, normal children in the present study, showed no
significant improvement in the lower grades (grade I and II), whereas showed significant
improvement in the higher grades (grades III, IV and V) only. Gathercole & Baddeley (1989);
Hoff, Cotre and Bridges (2008) have reported that word repetition in very young children is
related to their vocabulary size of the child. This means that children in the higher grades
have better and greater vocabulary size than the lower grades and hence the former
perform better than the latter on word repetition task indicative of an obvious
developmental trend. Findings of the present study in CWD showed that these children
performed significantly poorer than normal children on Wrep task. The difference was found
to be lesser in the lower grades and found to be more in higher grades. Studies done by
Scarborough (1990); Lyytinen, Poikkeus, Laakso, Eklund, and Lyytinen (2001), Gallagher, Frith
and Snowling (2000), Snowling, Gallagher, Frith (2003), Carroll and Snowling (2004) found
that children at risk for dyslexia show delays in lexical development, both in expressive and
receptive vocabulary. This vocabulary delays might have caused poor vocabulary repertoire
available for the CWD. According to these studies, the difference in performance between
CWD and normal children seem to increase with grade, as found in the present study, where
the CWD performed more poorly than normal children on higher grades compared to lower
grades.
Similar to word repetition task, on non word repetition task, the performance found
to have improved significantly in the higher grades (III, IV and V) only. Literature strongly
suggests that those children developing language normally who score higher on receptive
vocabulary measures repeat nonwords more accurately than do children with lower
receptive vocabulary scores (Gathercole & Baddeley 1989,Metsala 1999 ). It has been found
to be indicative of phonological working memory capacity (Coady & Evans, 2008) which
memory which in turn help them repeat or read non words appropriately compared to
younger children who are still building up their phonological repertoire and/or knowledge.
In turn, older children can figure out reading even new and unfamiliar words using their
phonological working memory capacity. Coady & Evans (2008) results on children ability to
repeat non words (which will indicate vocabulary strength) revealed that children scored
significantly better in NWrep tasks only in lower grades suggesting the strong vocabulary
construction only during early grades .The findings of the present study are in contradiction
with Coady & Evans (2008) , since the children of present study showed significant
improvement in scores of NWrep only in higher grades(grade IV and V).It can be concluded
from the present study that children continue to construct their vocabulary even in higher
grades. The CWD of the present study performed poorly compared to normal children on
NWrep task suggesting poor phonological working memory capacity, lexical (the degree of
colleagues found that children with dyslexia experienced a greater degree of difficulty with
non-words repetition, especially at longer lengths. They concluded that children with
dyslexia have difficulty with phonological analysis and articulatory assembly processes.
(Snowling 1981, Snowling et al., 1986) then used NWrep task to examine lexical and
would be used for familiar words, while such programs would be unavailable for non-words.
Successful repetition of non-words would require ‘subjects to process the auditory stimulus,
to decode the sound segments, and to recode these as instructions in the form of a speech-
motor program (Snowling 1981). This decoding and recoding deficits reported by Snowling
On rapid naming task, children in the present study showed increasing performance
performance was not significant in lower grades (grade I, II and III) and found to be
significantly increasing in higher grades (grade IV and V). This could be because the time
taken to access a lexicon from their repertoire is faster with the development of children.
Study by Wagner, Torgesen, Roshotte, Hecht, Baker and Burgess (1997) investigated the
kindergarten to grade II, from grade I to grade III and from grade II to grade IV. They
concluded that RN was a significant variation for word reading till kindergarten to grade III
only. Findings of present study contradict Wagner et al., (1997) study as in the present study
RN was found to be improving significantly in higher grades (grade IV and V) only. In other
The results of the present study also revealed that RN is a significant predictor of
reading skill with the significance value β =-.469; p<0.05. Manis, Seidenberg and Doi (1999).
Murphy and Pollatsek, (1994) Pennington, Cardoso-Martins, Green and Lefly (2001) and
Torgesen, Wagner, Rashotte, Burgess and Hecht (1997) suggested after their longitudinal
studies that alliteration and RN are differentially related to reading ability. Specifically it has
been suggested that although alliteration is the most Important for the development of the
ability to learn to read by phonological recoding, rapid naming skills may be especially
important for learning about the orthography of word- a skill necessary for fluent, skilled
reading. Indeed, RN has been consistently found to be related to fluency of text reading
On RN task, the performance of CWD was significantly poorer than normal children
suggesting poor phonological accessing speed in CWD. The CWD in present study performed
with same accuracy as normal children do, however they had consumed longer duration to
name all the items in the stimulus. Our findings are in consonance with study by study by
Bowers and Wolf (1993) which admitted that deficit in rapid naming are most strongly
can be hypothesized that children with slow naming speed activate the visual and
phonological codes for printed letters too slowly to allow efficient encoding of the specific
letter combinations in words. The study also supports Snyder and Downey (1995) who
compared the performance of children with dyslexia and normal reading achievement on
tasks of serial rapid naming. Their results revealed that the CWD had significantly longer
reaction times and production durations than their normal peers despite similar levels of
accuracy.
The tasks of alliteration and rhyming are discussed simultaneously since these skills
constitute evidence for children’s awareness of the sound system (phonological awareness
skills) that is manifested in their spontaneous play with language. On rhyming task, the
results revealed that the performance improved significantly from grade III till V. However,
in the lower grades, grade I performed better than grade II, which could be attributed to
existence of variability in data as suggested by large standard deviation (see Table 2). On the
alliteration task the significant improvement in performance was noticed only for higher
grades (grade III, IV and V). Van Kleeck and Bryant (1984) found that some children began
MacLean, Bryant and Bradley (1987) in an experimental task asked children to identify the
non rhyming word in a set of three words. They found that three year old subjects could
detect rhyming and alliteration at a conscious level. They also found a high correlation
between knowledge of nursery rhymes and success on phonological awareness tasks and
that both these abilities were related to early reading skills at the age of 4 years 6 months.
Wagner et al., (1997) concludes after his study that phoneme awareness tasks are significant
contributors to reading in higher grades only(grade III and IV).The present study also showed
that rhyming and alliteration tasks improved significantly only in higher grades (grade III, IV
and V), thus in consonance with Wagner et al (1997). The results of the present study also
p<0.05. Findings of the present study is in consonance with the results of investigators like
Adams (1990); Bryant, MacLean, Bradley & Crossland (1990), Lundberg, Frost , Petersen &
Yopp (1992), Cunningham (1991); Whitehurst & Lonigan (2001), Lonigan, Burgess & Anthony
(2000). Wagner et al., (1997) experimented the amount of information that a measure of
phonological awareness could add to the prediction of reading once a measure of current
word reading and vocabulary was considered. The results revealed that the phonological
Further, results of the present study showed that on rhyming and alliteration task,
CWD performed poorly than the normal children and performance are varying across
grades. This poor performance (slow development of phonemic awareness) can also be
correlated with their poor development in word reading skills. The present findings on CWD
are in consonance with study by Scarborough (1990) who concluded that at 5-year-olds who
exhibited weaknesses in object-naming, phonemic awareness, and letter-sound knowledge
turned poor readers in their early reading days suggesting poor phonemic awareness in
CWD. Studies by Moats and Foorman (1997) and Adams (1990) also suggests that children
who learn to read alphabetical language system such as English proven to be having poor
phonemic awareness tasks like rhyming and letter identification. The deficits noticed in
phonological awareness has been explained using the phonological theory (Snowling, 2000)
which is the most influential account for reading problems relates dyslexia to a deficit in
phonological awareness. Lyon, Shaywitz and Shaywitz, (2003) provided evidence for linkage
On the task of SDis the performance on normal children were improving from lower
through higher grades. It was not found to be significant in lower and higher ends whereas
only grade III found to be significantly better than grade II. In other words there was no
improvement in sound discrimination skill as the child grows older. The insignificant
Neff (2006) who concluded that both spectral and temporal cues attain adult like by the age
of six. It was evident from the present study that SDis was a significant predictor of reading
skill with the significance value of. β =-1.54; p<0.05.It is in consonance with the study done
by Talcott et al., (2000) who concluded that auditory sensitivity as a predictor of later
reading skill.
In the present study the CWD showed poor performance compared to normal
children, though it was not statistically significant. Tallal (1980) proposed that children with
specific reading difficulties are deficient in processing brief and rapidly changing acoustic
information like brief acoustic events in stop consonants. (Werker & Tees, 1987) have
contributed to the evidence that dyslexia is associated with auditory perceptual problems.
Smythe (1999) Perceptual difficulties, as described above, could adversely interfere with
consonants (as these seem particularly affected) may become blurred or distorted in
some children with dyslexia. Such noisy phonological representations may well explain
the delay that dyslexic individuals have with learning new words, as they may need more
manipulation of sounds within a syllable, which, in turn, is a very important factor when
processing is intact in dyslexics and it is only the higher (lexical) process that is affected.
Tallal, Merzenich, Miller and Jenkins (1998) reported that normal temporal sequencing is
the stream of speech and combined to form words .We can hypothesize from Tallal et al.,
(1998), the delay in temporal integration caused a cascade effects, starting with the normal
To summarize, a developmental trend has been observed in normal children on all the
tasks from grade I through grade V, except on the tasks of SDis and alphabet writing. Normal
children in the lower grades were found to perform better than the children in the higher
grades which can be been explained using the developmental stage models of reading. Children
in the lower grades perform poorly on tasks of reading as they may not have developed all the
component processes that are essential for reading familiar as well as unfamiliar words.
Further, children with dyslexia (CWD) even in the higher grades have performed significantly
poorer than younger normal children on various tasks indicating that there could be a delay or
The major aim of the present study was to subtype children with dyslexia based on
the individual profiles obtained after administering the IDT (Smythe, 2000) and cluster
analysis done on the data obtained. It was evident from cluster analysis in the present study
that dyslexia is not a homogenous group but a heterogeneous group with existence of three
major subtypes of dyslexia including phonological, surface and mixed types. Children with
dyslexia (CWD) who fell under the phonological subtype in the study included {1, 4, 14, 16,
10}.The cluster that fell under the surface subtype of dyslexia included {13}. The cluster
under the mixed subtype included CWD {2, 3, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 11, 12, 15} (See Table 5). The
suggested by various investigators earlier and recently by Zeigler et al., (2007). They opined
that there exists no homogenous group of developmental dyslexia. They also derived and
sub grouped their children with dyslexia under 3 broad classes, namely phonological, surface
and mixed types of dyslexia. Present study also supports Castles and Coltheart (1993) who
tested a sample of 53 poor readers on their ability to read aloud sets of irregular words and
whose nonword reading was poor, compared with chronological age- matched controls, but
their exception word reading was within normal range. Another 10 subjects were classified
as pure developmental surface dyslexics whose exception word reading was poor but their
nonword reading fell within normal range. Further they found that 27 subjects performed
poorly on both tasks and were therefore not classified as “pure” cases and were considered
as “mixed” cases. These children showed a significant discrepancy between their scores on
the exception word and nonword tasks. This 2 way classification has been documentad by
several other authors in their study. Manis et al., (1996) in their study reported of two
subgroups that were acknowledged to be forming their data, they were phonological and
surface dyslexia. Stanovich, Siegel & Gottardo (1997) identified 17 phonological and 15
surface dyslexics from their sample of 68 reading disabled 3rd grade children by comparing
them to chronological age controls on exception word and pseudo word reading. However,
when the dyslexic subtypes were defined by referring to Reading level controls, 17
phonological and only one surface dyslexic were identified. When the chronological age
defined subtypes were compared to reading level controls, the phonological dyslexics
displayed superior exception word reading but displayed deficits in pseudo word naming,
phonological sensitivity, working memory, and syntactic processing. The surface dyslexics in
contrast displayed a cognitive profile remarkably similar to that of the reading level controls.
The present study is in consonance with Ziegler et al., (2007) and Castles et al.,
(1993) who identified three broad subtypes of dyslexia.ie phonological, surface and mixed
varieties. The current classification has also been supported by Bakker (1979, 1990) who
classified dyslexics into P-types (relying on perceptual, analytical strategies for reading,
which turns out to be slow, fragmented and hesitating), L-types (relying on linguistic,
anticipatory strategies for reading, which allow for quicker reading but produce many,
usually plausible and context-based errors) and M-types (mixed types, showing both slow,
fragmented reading and many errors).In the present study, the CWD who had difficulties on
NWrep, NWreading, alliteration and rhyming were classified as phonological subtype and
CWD who performed well on phonological tasks but had difficulties on task of word reading
were classified as surface subtype. The CWD who had difficulty in both phonological
(NWreading, NWrep, alliteration, rhyming) and non phonological (Wreading, Wrep, RN and
tasks in comparison to the others due to deficit in the sub lexical processing of phonemes
(Castles & Coltheart, 1993). In this group it was found that CWD performed poor mostly on
nonword reading tasks. By theory, to read a non word, it is crucial to have phoneme
grapheme conversion system intact. However, children with phonological dyslexia are
unable to read non words as it is hypothesized that the sub lexical route which is composed
of grapheme to phoneme conversion system is grossly affected (see Figure 1). Elbro (1996),
Hulme and Snowling (1992) and Snowling (2000) propose a hypothesis which says that the
under-specified in phonological dyslexics. A more basic auditory processing deficit has also
been considered as a possible underlying cause in phonological dyslexics (Farmer & Klein,
1995; Tallal, 1980). Yet this theory does not seem to be able to account for most dyslexics’
Apart from poor performance on non-word reading tasks, children with phonological
dyslexia have been found to perform poorly on tasks of manipulating phonemes in order to
facilitate learning to spell and further facilitate better reading Bradley, & Bryant, 1978;
Griffiths, & Snowling, 2002; Morris , Stuebing, Fletcher, Shaywitz, Lyon, Shankweiler, et al.
(1998)). The main features of phonological deficit can be highlighted by three main types of
tasks (Snowling, 2000). Firstly, dyslexics perform poorly on tasks which require phonological
awareness, for instance paying attention to and manipulating individual speech sounds.
CWD with phonological deficit in the present study also showed poor performances on
rhyming and alliteration tasks which are considered tasks of phonological awareness
(Smythe, 2000). Secondly, they have difficulty when required to name series of objects
(rapid automatized naming) rapidly as seen in the present study. Thirdly, their verbal short-
lower memory span and poor nonword repetition, and impacts negatively on list learning,
story recall, paired-associate learning, and the more complex phonological awareness tasks
such as spoonerisms (Blomert & Mitterer, 2004; Tijms, 2004; Vellutino, Harding, Phillips, &
Steger, 1975).
The reason for the performance of the surface dyslexic profile could be due to a
nonphonological deficit in the lexical route as suggested also by Castles and Coltheart,
(1993). In surface dyslexics the disconnection is in between the semantics and phonological
functions leaving only the sub lexical route in operation (see fig.1). So, the reading of non
words is relatively intact in this population. Most studies in literature have found that
surface dyslexics had weaker phonological deficits than phonological dyslexics (Manis,
McBride-Chang, Seidenberg, Keating, Doi & Munson, 1997); Stanovich, Siegel, & Gottardo,
1997).The present study demonstrates similar results, whereas Zeigler et al., (2007)
dyslexics. In the present study, apart from good performance of surface dyslexics on
phonological related tasks, it was noteworthy that these children also performed better on
rapid naming tasks. A contradicting study to our findings was that of study by Zeigler et al
(2007) who indicated clearly a different picture of surface dyslexia than the one commonly
suggested in the literature. Although their surface dyslexics showed small impairments in
orthographic access (letter search deficit in words, which resulted in reduced word
superiority effects compared to phonological dyslexics), the main deficits were phonological
The performance of mixed group in the present study could be due to the deficits in
both the routes of reading. i.e., sublexical and lexical route as suggested by Edwards &
Hogben, 1999) It can be hypothesized that the combined deficits in orthographic lexicon and
in Grapheme Phoneme (GPC) rules could have resulted in mixed dyslexia (see Figure.1). In
other words they have difficulty in reading stimuli that requires both lexical and sub lexical
processing (Edwards & Hogben, 1999). Various hypothetical reasons have been purported
for the occurrence of mixed dyslexia in CWD. Castles and Coltheart (1993) reported of three
reasons for occurrence of mixed dyslexia in CWD population. The first reason relates to the
structure of the model itself- the computational Dual Route Cascaded (DRC) model of
Coltheart and colleagues (Coltheart, Curtis, Atkins, & Haller, 1993; Coltheart, Rastle, Perry,
Langdon, & Ziegler, 2001). The lexical and nonlexical routes of the DRC are not completely
independent, but have three components in common: visual feature detectors, abstract
letter units, and the phoneme output system (see Figure-1) Therefore, a deficit in any one of
these components can lead to impairments in both lexical (surface) and sub lexical
(phonological) pathways. The second reason why deficits in lexical and sub lexical system
might co-occur relates not to the structure of the dual-route model itself but to the other
distal factors that may have influenced the development of the components of the model in
the first place. Even though the processes themselves may be distinct, there are numerous
speed and efficiency of children’s acquisition of both the lexical and the nonlexical routes,
and to an approximately equal degree. The third reason why deficits in lexical and sub lexical
system might co-occur is that, although the orthographic lexicon and the GPC procedure in
the DRC model operate independently, this does not mean that they are learned
independently.
study. Three major subtypes were identified based on cluster analysis and profiling and this
could be explained using dual route model. The phonological subtype has been hypothesized
to be occurring due to poor sublexical processing; the surface subtype has been
hypothesized to be occurring due to poor lexical processing. The most prevalent mixed type
has been hypothesized to be occurring due to deficit in both lexical and sublexical route of
Literacy is defined as the minimal ability to read and write in a designated language.
automatized naming. Phonological awareness among others has been shown to be a primary
factor underlying early reading development. Learning to read, for many children seems not
to require much effort. They appear to attain this ability almost incidentally. However, it
seems to be correct in the languages where there is only one sound for each letter, where
the assigning phoneme to a grapheme step is relatively easy. But in languages like English
the association of various different letters sounds with a particular letter and various
different letters with a particular sound makes mastery of the sound symbol relationship
more difficult. This irregularity in the language itself makes it even more difficult in those
Dyslexia can be described as the person who has difficulty in reading even though
sufficient teaching has been offered. As there are number of causes of dyslexia, there are
number different types of dyslexia making dyslexia a heterogeneous group .Though there
are several different types of dyslexia practically can exist for the convenience of
classification different subtypes have been proposed in the literature. One of the major
classifications is proposed by Castles and Colthart (1993) based on dual route model. The
difficulty in phonological tasks), surface (CWD finding difficulty in word reading) and mixed
(CWD finding difficulties both in phonological and non phonological tasks) type. Sixty normal
children, 12 in each grade from grade I to V was taken to form a normal group. The children
with dyslexia (CWD) consisted of 16 children, with 3 children from each grade except grade
normal children for all the grades across different grades. The mean and standard deviation
table showed that tasks like SC, HQ, Spelling and Wreading showed clear developmental
trend from lower to higher grade. On tasks like NWreading, Rhyming, Wrep, NWrep ,
alliteration, RN and SDis lower grades didn’t reveal any increasing trend where as higher
grades revealed developmental trend. Normal children in the lower grades were found to
perform better than the children in the higher grades which can be been explained using the
developmental stage models of reading. Children in the lower grades perform poorly on tasks
of reading as they may not have developed all the component processes that are essential for
reading familiar as well as unfamiliar words. Further, children with dyslexia (CWD) even in the
higher grades have performed significantly poorer than younger normal children on various
tasks indicating that there could be a delay or deficit in the acquisition of component processes
Results are also discussed for the comparison of normal group against CWD group
which showed that CWD performed poorer than normal children on all the tasks. A
regression analysis was carried out to obtain the predictors for reading. Results indicated
that that tasks like rapid naming (RN), alliteration, sound discrimination (SDis) and spelling
were found to be the predictors for reading. Further the data was analyzed qualitatively, and
the major findings were explained. This included mirror images in alphabet task, difficulty in
alliteration and rhyming, GPC usage in reading words and non words, inappropriate working
memory leading to poor performance in repetition (word and non word) task and the
tendency to skip a whole series while naming given series of items on RN task. The results of
the sub grouping revealed that 7 were phonological dyslexics, 1 was surface dyslexics and 9
were mixed group. Thus, heterogeneity in developmental dyslexia was confirmed in the
present study. The phonological subtype has been hypothesized to be occurring due to poor
sublexical processing; the surface subtype has been hypothesized to be occurring due to
poor lexical processing. The most prevalent mixed type has been hypothesized to be
occurring due to deficit in both lexical and sublexical route of the dual route processing for
The result of the present study reveal that DAPIC can be used as a tool to profile
those children who show difficulties in phonological and non phonological tasks
generalize the findings and use regularly for the assessment of children with
reading problems.
The results of the present study reveal that differentiating among dyslexia
disorder than simply comparing dyslexics with normal. The profiling and sub
typing of the present study also lead us to understand and plan for
Various treatment programs has been suggested in the literature namely relay
sited , showed that programs must focus on phonics , early diagnosis with
The present study also highlights on predictors of reading ability, which will
foster the versatility of the present profiling tool to be used as a screening tool.
Which supposedly include that are significant predictors of the reading skill like
This study included a small sample of children for each grade. Administering it on
larger sample would help in standardization of the tool for assessing children with
dyslexia.
An additional, large scale study might also look at the prevalence of domain general
concerned with overall group differences, but given a larger sample, it would be
Other domain specific tasks of phoneme awareness skills like phoneme stripping,
word attack tasks could have been included in the test to give a complete profile of
Aaron, P.G., Joshi, M., & Williams, K.A. (1999). Not all reading disabilities are
alike. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 2, 120–137.
Bell, S. M., McCallum, R. S., & Cox, E .A. (2003). Toward a research based
assessment of Dyslexia: Using cognitive, measures to identify reading
disabilities .Journal of reading disabilities, 36, 505-516
Blomert, L., & Mitterer, H. (2004). The fragile nature of the speech-perception deficit in
dyslexia: Natural Vs synthetic speech. Brain and Language, 89, 1, 21–26.
Boder, E. (1973). Developmental dyslexia: A diagnostic approach based on three atypical
reading patterns. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 15, 663–687.
Bowers, P.G., & Wolf, M. (1993).Theoretical links among naming speed , precise timing
mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 5, 69-85.
Bryant, P.E., MacLean, L, M., Bradley, & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration,
phoneme detection, and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429-
38.
Bub, D., Cancelliere, A., & Kertesz, A. (1985), Whole-word and analytic translation of
spelling to sound in a non-semantic reader, in Surface dyslexia:
neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (eds. K.E.
Patterson, J.C. Marshall and M. Coltheart), Erlbaum, London.
Carroll, J. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Language and phonological skills in children at
high-risk of reading difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45,
631–640.
Carrow-Woolfolk,E.(1995). Oral and Written language scales (OWLS). Circle Pines, MN:
American Guidance service.
Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47,
149–180.
Catts, H. W., & Kamhi, A. G. (1999). Language and reading disabilities. Needham Heights,
MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Clarke, L.K. (1988). Invented versus traditional spelling in first graders’ writings: Effects
on learning to spell and read. Research in the Teaching of English 22: 281-309.
Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks.
Dyslexia, 7, 197–216.
Coltheart, M., Bates, A., & Castles, A. (1994). Cognitive neuropsychology and
rehabilitation. In G.W. Humphreys & M.J. Riddoch (Eds.), Cognitive
neuropsychology and cognitive rehabilitation. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates Ltd.
Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual
route and parallel distributed processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100,
589-608.
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. C. (2001). DRC: A Dual Route
Cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological
Review, 108, 204–256.
Dyson, A.H. (2001). Writing and children’s symbolic repertoires: Development unhinged.
In Handbook of Early Literacy Research, Eds. S.B. Neuman & D.K. Dickinson. New
York: The Guilford Press.
Edwards, V., & Hogben, J. (1999). New norms for comparing children’s lexical and
nonlexical reading: A further look at subtyping dyslexia. Australian Journal of
Psychology, 5, 37–49.
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S.R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B.V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z. & Shanahan, T.
(2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence
from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly,
36, 250-287.
Elbro, C. (1996). Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a
hypothesis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 453–485.
Farmer, M.E, Klein RM (1995) .The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to
dyslexia: a review. Psychonom Bull Rev 2: 460-493.
Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Barnes, M. A. (2006). Learning disabilities. New
York, NY: Guilford. A.91, 8010–8013.
Gallagher, A., Frith U., Snowling M.(2000). Precursors of literacy delay among children at
genetic risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 202–213.
Hanley, J.R., Hastie, K., & Kay, J. (1992). Developmental surface dyslexia and dysgraphia:
An orthographic processing impairment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 44a, 2, 285-319.
Hari, R., & Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in
dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 525–532.
Harris, T. L., & Hodges. R. E. (1995).The literacy dictionary: The vocabulary of reading
and writing. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. 318 pages.
0872071383. Interest level: general.
Hatfield, F.M. (1983). Aspects of acquired dysgraphi a and implications for re-education . In
C. Code & D. Muller (Eds.), Aphasia therapy. London: Edward Arnold.
Ho, C.S.H., Chan, D.W.O., Tsang ,S.M., Lee ,S.H. (2000). The Hong Kong Test of Specific
Learning Difficulties in Reading and Writing. Hong Kong: Hong Kong Specific
Learning Difficulties Research Team.
Karlsen, B. and Gardner, E.(1996). Stanford Diagnostic Reading Test-4. San Antonio,
TX;harcoutrt-brace Educational Measurement.
Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S.R., & Anthony, J.L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy
and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent variable
longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596-613.
Loosemore, R.P.W., Brown, G.D.A. & Watson, F.L. (1991). A neural network model of normal
and dyslexic spelling. Proceedings of the International Joint Conference on Neural
Networks, 231-236. Seattle: IEEE Press.
Lundberg, I., Frost ,J.,& Petersen ,O.P.(1988). Effects of an extensive program for
stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research
Quarterly 23: 263-284.
Maclean, M., Bryant, P.E. & Bradley. (1987) .Rhymes, Nursery rhymes and reading in
early childhood. Merrill-Palmer quarterly, 3, 3, 255-81.
Manis, F. R., McBride-Chang, C., Seidenberg, M. S., Keating, P., Doi, L. M.,
Munson, B., et al. (1997).Are speech perception deficits associated with
developmental dyslexia? .Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66,
211–235.
Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M., McBride-Chang, C., & Petersen, A.
(1996). On the bases of two subtypes of development dyslexia. Cognition,
58, 157–195.
Mann, V. (1986). Phonological awareness: The role of reading experience .Cognition, 24, 65–
92.
Morris, R. D., Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Lyon, G. R., Shankweiler, D. P., et
al. (1998). Subtypes of reading disability: Variability around a phonological core.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 347–373.
Morton,J. & Frith.U.(1993).What lesson for dyslexia from Down’s syndrome? Comments on
Cossu, Rossini, and Marshall. Cognition, 48, 2889-296.
Nagy, W., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of
morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second grade
readers and at-risk fourth grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology,
95(4), 730-742.
Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: The cerebellar
deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neuroscience, 24, 508–511.
Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., Berry, E. L., Jenkins, I. H., Dean, P., & Brooks, D. J. (1999).
Association of abnormal cerebellar activation with motor learning difficulties in
dyslexic adults. Lancet, 353, 1662–1667.
Pennington, B.F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P.A. & Lefly, D.L. (2001).Comparing the
phonological and double deficit hypotheses for developmental dyslexia. Reading and
Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 705-755.
Prema, K.S. (1997). Reading acquisition profile in Kannada. Ph. D. dissertation, University
of Mysore, Mysore.
Roberts, J. E & Medley, L. P. (1995). Ottitis media and speech language sequale in young
children: Current issue ion measurement .Americal Journal of Speech Language
Pathology, 4, 15-24.
Seymour, P., & Elder, L. (1986). Beginning reading without phonology. Cognitive
Neuropsychology, 3, 1–37.
Seymour, P., & Evans, H. (1992). Beginning reading without semantics: A cognitive study
of hyperlexia. Cognitive Neuropsycholog y, 9, 89–122.
Singhi, P., Kumar, M., Malhi, P., & Kumar, R. (2007). Utility of the WHO Ten Questions Screen
for Disability Detection in a Rural Community—the North Indian Experience. Journal
of Tropical Pediatrics. 53, 6, 383-387.
Smythe, I., (2000). International dyslexia test. Retrieved January 21, 2008 from E-mail
communication.
Smythe, I., & Everatt J (2000). Dyslexia diagnosis in different languages. In Peer L and
Reid G, Multilingualism, Literacy and Dyslexia. David Fulton Publishers. London.
Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous:
Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74,
358–373.
Snowling, M. J., Goulandris, N., Bowlby, M., & Howell, P. (1986). Segmentation and
speech perception in relation to reading skill: A developmental analysis. Journal
of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 489–507.
Snyder, L. S., & Downey, D. M. (1995). Serial rapid naming skills in children with reading
disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia. 45, 31– 49.
Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and new
frontiers. New York: Guilford Press.
Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S., & Gottardo, A. (1997). Converging evidence for
phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 89, 114–127.
Stein, J., & Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of
dyslexia. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 147–152.
Stuart, M. (1995). Prediction and qualitative assessment of five and six year old
children’s reading: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 65, 287–296.
Stuart, M., Masterson, J., & Dixon, M. (2000). Sponge like acquisition of sight vocabulary
in beginning readers? Journal of Research in Reading, 23, 12–27.
Talcott, J. B., Witton, C., McClean, M., Hansen, P. C., Rees, A., Green, G. G. R., & Stein, J.
F. (1999). Can sensitivity to auditory frequency modulation predict children's
phonological and reading skills? Neuroreport, 10(10), 2045-2050.
Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics and reading disabilities in Children.
Brain and Language. 9,182–98.
Tallal, P., Merzenich, M., Miller, S., & Jenkins, W. (1998) Language Learning Impairments:
Integrating Basic Science, Technology and Remediation, Experimental Brain
Research, 123, 210-219.
Tallal. P., Miller, S., & Fitch, H.(1993).Neurobiological basis of speech : A case for the
preeminence of temporal processing. In Tallal, A.M. Gulbarda, R.R.Llinas, &
C.Von Euler.(Eds.), Annals of the New York academy of Sciences,682,27-47.
Temple, C. (1984). Surface dyslexia in a child with epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 22, 569–576.
Terepocki, M., Kruk, R. S., & Willows, D. M. (2002). The incidence and nature of letter
orientation errors in reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 214-
233.
Tijms, J. (2004). Verbal memory and phonological deficit in dyslexia. Journal of Research
in Reading, 27(3), 300–310.
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashtte, C. A., Burgess,S., & Hecht, S.(1997).Contributions
of phonologicalawareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of
word-reading skills in second- fifth grade children.Scientific Studies of
Reading,1(2),161-195.Mahwah,NJ:Erlbaum.
Valdois, S., Bosse, M. L., & Tainturier, M. J. (2004). The cognitive deficits responsible for
developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a selective visual attentional
disorder. Dyslexia, 10, 339–363
Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading
disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1, 2–40.
Vellutino, F. R., Harding, C. J., Phillips, F., & Steger, J. A. (1975). Differential transfer in
poor and normal readers. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 126 (1st Half), 3–18.
Viholainen, H., Ahonen, T., Cantell, M., Lyytinen, P., & Lyytinen, H. (2002). Development
of early motor skills and language in children at risk for familial dyslexia.
Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 44, 761-769.
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A.,
Burgess, S. R., Donahue, J., & Garon, T. (1997). Changing relations
between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as
children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal
study. Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 468-479.
Werker, J.F., & Tees, R.C. (1984) .Phonemic and phonetic factors in adult cross –
language speech perception .Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
75, 1866-1878.
Whitehurst, G.J., & Lonigan, C.J. (2001). Emergent literacy: Development from prereaders
to readers. In Handbook of Early Literacy Research, eds. S.B. Neuman & D.K.
Dickinson. New York: The Guilford Press.
Ziegler ,J.C., Castel. C., Pech-Georgel .C., George,F., Alario, X., & Perry, C. (2007).
Developmental dyslexia and the dual route model of reading: Simulating
individual differences and subtypes.Cognition, 1-28. Article in press.
Appendix A
Test Material
1. Alphabet test
2. Shape copying
3. Writing
4. Reading test
1. tree
2. little
3. milk
4. egg
5. book.
6. school
7. sit
8. frog
9. playing
10. bun
11. flower
12. road
13. clock
14. train
15. light
16. picture
17. think
18. summer
19. people
20. something
21. dream
22. downstairs
23. biscuit
24. shepherd
25. thirsty
26. crowd
27. sandwich
28. beginning
29. postage
30. island
31. Saucer
32. angel
33. ceiling
34. appeared
35. knife
36. canary
37. attractive
38. imagine
39. nephew
40. gradually
41. smoulder
42. applaud
43. disposal
44. nourished
45. diseased
46. university
47. orchestra
48. knowledge
49. audience
50. situated
51. physics
52. campaign
53. choir
54. intercede
55. fascinate
56. Forfeit
57. siege
58. recent
59. plausible
60. prophecy
61. colonel
62. soloist
63. systematic
64. slovenly
65. classification
66. genuine
67. institution
68. pivot
69. conscience
70. heroic
Practice items:
Test items:
1. Gat 6.higure
2. Rop 7.kibnick
3. Shug 8.Pachine
4. Hild 9.clabnag
5.Narge 10.tringdom
6. Alliteration test
Practice item 1. Practice item 2.
7. Rhyming test
Practice items:
8. Word repetition
1. Pin cat
2. Cap sky tin
3. Mat crow pick
4. Rain pen chair men
5. Pit day log shirt
6. Cow wool snake hut grape
7. Plank lion heel plot den
1. Ket
2. Lum
3. Mup hin
4. Ret spige
5. Trum frut nabe
6. Ronch tarp keld
7. Horp brid nate proog
8. Fode wike drup cren
Prctice item:
Pig Dig-different
Bat Bat-Same
Lake Date-different
Test items:
1. Rip tip
2. Sick sack
3. Side side
4. Pet bet
5. Big bog
6. Sit sit
7. Bed bad
8. Dam mad
9. Slow snow
10. End and
11. Fish fish
12. Shelled shield
13. Halt hall
14. Try tie
15. Tilt tilt
16. Ship sheep
17. Raw war
18. Throw throw
19. Rip reap
20. Nib nip
Score Sheet
Name :
School:
Grade:
Tested by:
Scores for screening tests
DST
Individual’s score:
Test domain Maximum score Individual’s score
1.Alphabet 9
2.Shape copying 10
3.writing
a) spelling 40
b)handwriting quality 5
4.Reading test 70
6.Alliteration 10
7.Rhyme 20
8.Word repetition 7
11.Sound discrimination 20
1.
2.
3.
4.
3.Writing
2) a. b. c. d. e.
3) a. b. c. d. e.
4) a. b. c. d. e.
5)a. b. c. d. e.
6)a. b. c. d. e.
7)a. b. c. d. e.
8)a. b. c. d. e.
9)a. b. c. d. e.
10)a. b. c. d. e.
11)a. b. c. d. e.
12)a. b. c. d. e.
13)a. b. c. d. e.
14)a. b. c. d. e.
5.Non word reading test: ( score one for each correctly read non word)
1. 6.
2. 7.
3. 8.
4. 9.
5. 10.
6.Alliteration: (one score for each correct responses)
1. 6.
2. 7.
3. 8.
4. 9.
5. 10.
1. 11.
2. 12.
3. 13.
4. 14.
5. 15.
6. 16.
7. 17.
8. 18.
9. 19.
10. 20.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
1. 5.
2. 6.
3. 7.
4. 8.
10.Rapid naming: (note the number of items named at the end of one minute)
11.Sound discrimination:
1. 11.
2. 12.
3. 13.
4. 14.
5. 15.
6. 16.
7. 17.
8. 18.
9. 19.
10. 20.
Appendix B
Subject 1. (Grade I)
Wrep: Managed to repeat only the first series of stimulus which had only two words (e.g.
pin, cat)
NWrep: Managed to repeat the first two non word series only, they were made of one
nonword consisted of single syllable. The subject couldn’t repeat the third nonword since it
had two words.
Subject 2 (Grade 1)
SC: Copies the first three simple shapes exactly, but only fairly well the complex one.
Spelling: lot, be, fish and tent were spelled correctly out of all forty words.
Wrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to four words in it.
NWrep: Could manage to repeat stimulus series which had up to two non words only.
SDis: side, reap, nip were the ones which were incorrect.
Subject 3(Grade 1)
SC: Completed the simple ones with ease, but managed to copy the final shape only lesser
than fair.
Spelling: lot, be and much were written correctly
Reading: First three items (tree, little and milk) then 6th (school) and eleven (flower) were
correctly read.
Wrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to four words. (Fair performance)
NWrep: Managed only the first two series, which had only one word each (poor
performance)
SC: simple shapes are drawn correctly, poor copying of complex shape
Wrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to four words in it.
NWrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to two non words in it
RN: Took 92 seconds to name all the items
SC: Correctly written simple shapes, complex shape was better approximating towards
stimulus shape
NWrep. Could repeat stimulus series which had up to two non words in it
SC: Correctly written simple shapes, complex shape was fairly drawn.
Reading: Six of the words were read correctly, still a poor performance
NWreading: Managed to read only three of the monosyllabic words
NWrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to two non words in it
SC: All the simple shapes were copied correctly, good performance in copying complex
shape
Reading: Number of correct responses is over ten, and most of the correct responses were
correct for the first ten stimulus.
NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it
SC: All the simple shapes were copied correctly, fair performance in copying complex shape
Reading: Number of correct responses was ten, and most of the correct responses were
correct for the first ten stimulus
NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it
SC: All the simple shapes were copied correctly, fair performance in copying complex shape
Reading: Number of correct responses was over ten, and most of the correct responses were
correct for the first ten stimulus.
NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it
Reading: Half of the words were read correctly (i.e. 35, however 90 percent of the correctly
read words were in first half of the stimulus)
Wrep: Could repeat only up to three word stimulus series (very poor performance)
NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it
Wrep: Could repeat only up to three word stimulus series (very poor performance)
NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had two nonwords in it
SDis: All are correct except 19th item, i.e rip and reap (Very good performance)
Wrep: Could repeat only up to three word stimulus series (very poor performance
NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had two nonwords in it
NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had two nonwords in it
Subject 14 (Grade V)
SC: Could not copy two of the simple shapes and poor copying of complex shape
NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it
Subject 15(Grade V)
NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it
NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it
*Performance rating-very bad <bad < very poor <poor < fair <good < very good <excellent