0% found this document useful (0 votes)
1K views147 pages

Dyslexia Test (DAIPC)

DDFFVNH

Uploaded by

diyajshetty2000
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
1K views147 pages

Dyslexia Test (DAIPC)

DDFFVNH

Uploaded by

diyajshetty2000
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 147

DYSLEXIA ASSESSMENT PROFILE FOR IN INDIAN

CHILDREN (DAPIC)

Kuppuraj.S

Register Number: 07SLP009

Dissertation submitted in part fulfillment

of the Master’s Degree (Speech Language Pathology),

University of Mysore, Mysore.

ALL INDIA INSTITUTE OF SPEECH AND HEARING

MANASAGANGOTHRI, MYSORE – 570 006

MAY- 2009
CERTIFICATE

This is to certify that the dissertation entitled “Dyslexia assessment profile in

Indian children (DAPIC)” is the bonafide work submitted in part fulfillment for the

degree of Master of Science (Speech Language Pathology) of the student

(Registration No.07SLP009). This has been carried out under the guidance of a

faculty of this institute and has not been submitted earlier to any other university for

the award of any other Diploma or Degree.

Dr. Vijayalakshmi Basavaraj

Director
Mysore,
All India Institute of Speech and Hearing,
May, 2009
Naimisham Campus, Manasagangothri,

Mysore - 570 006.


CERTIFICATE

This is to certify that the dissertation entitled “Dyslexia assessment profile in

Indian children (DAPIC)” has been prepared under my supervision and guidance. It

is also certified that this has not been submitted earlier in any other university for the

award of any Diploma or Degree.

Guide

Ms. Jayashree.C.Shanbal

Lecturer in Language Pathology,

(Department of Speech-Language Pathology)


Mysore,
All India Institute of Speech and Hearing,
May, 2009
Mysore - 570 006.
DECLARATION

This dissertation entitled “Dyslexia assessment profile in Indian children (DAPIC)”

is the result of my own study under the guidance of Ms. Jayashree.C.Shanbal,

Lecturer in Language Pathology, Department of Speech-Language Pathology, All

India Institute of Speech and Hearing, Mysore, and has not been submitted earlier at

any other University for the award of any Diploma or Degree.

Mysore, Register No. 07SLP009

May, 2009
Acknowledgement

I would like to thank Mrs.Jayashree Shanbal.C, Lecturer in Speech Language pathology,


for her guidance. For making me realize that dissertation is part of my course. Hats off
for your guidance mam.

I would like to thank Dr.Vijayalakshmi Basavaraj, for permitting me to carry out this
study.

My thanks also to Dr.Prema.K.S (Mr. Research), for being the impetus of selecting this
field for this study.

This dissertation would not have reached its finishing line, with out Mr.Gopi Shankar’s
timely suggestions and support through out research writing.

I would like to thank our statistician Mrs.Vasanthalakshmi, for her reckless help in
statistics calculations.

I extend my sincere thanks to Jayakumar(Jaks), Arun BT(baba) foe being with me when
needed them for dissertation.

And my heartly thanks to all those kids participated in this study who asked nothing
but, ‘anna elli bari beku’.Thanks for the school authorities for the cooperation as well.

Who else could have given me a bright future none other than our AIISH?.I thank all the
teaching staffs for giving me the bits and pieces of their knowledge which would take me
a long way.

Finally here is the opportunity to thank all the cool dudes of AIISH(viv,amit,chandan
,Akshay,Mohandas,narendra)for being with me and you guys too are the reason for all my
smiles at AIISH, and I wounder , we ever had a reason to worry, and challenge the
jinx?(and also my class pals for bearing with me in class).
TABLE OF CONTENTS

Chapters Page No.


1.Introduction 1-6
2. Review of Literature
2.1. Development of reading and writing skills.
2.2.Theories of Normal Reading 7-35
2.3Subtypes of Developmental dyslexia.
3.Method
3.1. Participants 36-43
3.2. Procedure
4. Results
4.1. Performance of normal children and CWD across various tasks
4.2. Comparison of performance of normal children
and children with dyslexia
44-65
4.3. Sub typing of dyslexia based on profiling and cluster analysis
4.3.1. Overall clusters.
4.3.2. Phonological clusters
4.3.3. Non-phonological clusters.
5.Discussion
5.1 Development of reading and writing skills in normal
children and performance of CWD
5.1.1. Alphabet task.
5.1.2. Handwriting quality (HQ) and Shape copying (SC)
5.1.3. Spelling
5.1.4. Word reading (Wreading) and Non word reading
(NWreading)
5.1.5. Word repetition (Wrep) and Non word repetition 66-87
(NWrep)
5.1.6. Rapid naming
5.1.7. Alliteration and rhyming
5.1.8. Sound discrimination (SDis)
5.2 Subtypes of dyslexia based on the profile
5.2.1. The phonological subgroup
5.2.2. The surface subgroup
5.2.3. The mixed subgroup
Summary and Conclusion
88-92
References

Appendix A
Appendix B
List of Tables

Table Title Page No.

Table 1 The task and scoring for each of the subtests 42

Table 2 Mean and Standard Deviation (SD) of scores of 46

Grades I to V.

Table 3 Mean and SD for overall performance of normal 52

children and CWD

Table 4 Sub grouping of CWD under phonological, non 64

phonological and mixed type.


List of Figures

Figure Title Page No.


Cognitive neuropsychological dual route model of reading 23
Figure 1
(adapted from Newcombe & Marshall, 1984)

Performance of normal on tasks (HQ, RN, SC, NWreading 49


Figure 2
and alliteration) across grades

Performence of normal on tasks (rhyming, Wrep, NWrep, 50


Figure 3
SDis, spelling and Wreading) across grades.
Mean scores for normal children and children with dyslexia 53
Figure 4
across tasks

Dendrogram depicting all the clusters in the CWD group 56


Figure 5

Figure 6 Clusters for phonological tasks 60

Figure 7 Clusters for non phonological tasks 62


CHAPTER 1

Introduction

Reading is a complex cognitive process that involves multiple skills (Prema, 1997).

Components of reading ability include phonological awareness, phonological decoding,

reading comprehension, spelling, orthographic knowledge and rapid automatized naming

(Gayan & Olsen, 2003). Before one becomes completely literate, he or she must develop

early reading skills which comprised of steps like literacy awareness, syntactic awareness,

word recognition, phonological awareness, orthographic awareness (Strommen & Mates,

1997).

Reading process is the conversion of print into auditory equivalents and the

subsequent interpretation of those equivalents into meanings based on previously learned

language. The reading process includes decoding of the print in to sound. Decoding begins

with the process of converting individual letters into sounds and proceeds to the decoding of

whole words and phrases. A person who cannot decode printed words cannot understand

what those words mean. Comprehension is the next step which is the ability to interpret and

understand the decoded words. Although adequate decoding is a prerequisite for reading it

is not sufficient, comprehension is also required. The ability to comprehend what one reads

is based on experience. Readers take what has been read and integrate it with previously

learned language and experience.


The next component of literacy is writing. Written language is characterized by rules

of phoneme grapheme correspondence (Nagy, Berninger, Abbott, Vaughan & Vermaulen,

2003), the rules of orthographic knowledge (processing written language letters and letter

patterns), phonological knowledge (processing or manipulating oral language sounds), and

morphological awareness in spelling performance have been well documented. As a result

spelling is related to reading and written expression. Spelling is one of the most valued yet

difficult skills in written communication. Spelling requires matching the sounds of language

with the appropriate letters in order to accurately and reliably conveys messages. A child’s

ability to spell words correctly shows a sophisticated knowledge of letters, sounds and

syllable patterns (Bear & Templeton, 1998). There are several stages through which a child

travels before master spelling skill. Gentry (1982) proposes stages like precommunicative

(where the child uses symbols from the alphabet but shows no knowledge of letter-sound

correspondences), semi phonetic(sounds are assigned to letters), phonetic (The child uses a

letter or group of letters to represent every speech sound that they hear in a word),

transitional (the speller begins to assimilate the conventional alternative for representing

sounds),and correct stage(the speller knows the English orthographic system and its basic

rules). Frith (1985) also proposes parallel but different stages through which a child goes

through before he acquires complete knowledge of spelling. These stages are logographic,

alphabetic and orthographic stage of spelling development.

Fletcher. Lyon, Fuchs, & Barnes (2006) have quoted that reading is not natural and

everyone is taught to read. Those children who lack the ability to learn literacy skills are

considered ‘dyslexic’. Despite years of research, mostly in the English language, there is still

no consensus as to the definition of dyslexia, nor its underlying cause. The current dominant

view (Masland, 1997) argues that the cause of the educational difficulties faced by dyslexics
centre around core difficulties in phonological awareness and word decoding skills.

(Stanovich, 2000) admits that it may be related to deficits in processing novel letter string.

Smythe and Everatt (2000) proposed the following definition which incorporates

many of the features identified by research as important in these diverse mono and

multilingual environments.

“Dyslexia is a difficulty in the acquisition of literacy skills that may be caused by

combination of phonological processing, visual and auditory system deficits. Lexical

confusions and speed of processing difficulties may also be present. The

manifestation of dyslexia in any individual will depend upon not only individual

cognitive differences, but also the language used.”

There are several causes proposed in the literature for dyslexia. They are heredity

(Pennington, 1989), brain differences (Galaburda, 1991), defects in rapid temporal

information processing (Tallal, Miller & Fitch 1993), selective attention and attention deficit

disorder (Zentall, 1993), middle ear problems (Roberts & Medley, 1995), Cognitive rigidity

and learned helplessness (Clay, 1984). Akin causes of dyslexia, there are several types of

learning difficulty (dyslexia) exist. Theoretically there are as many types of dyslexics as

number of cases do, making the dyslexic group a heterogeneous one. Newcombe and

Marshall (1984) proposed a reading model which explains normal visual word reading. This

model composed of two routs namely lexical (direct route) through which words and

irregular words are read. Another route of the model is the sublexical route (indirect route)

through which non words are read. Castles and Coltheart (1993) broadly classifies dyslexia in
to two main types called phonological and surface dyslexics. He opined that with difficulties

in sub lexical skills shall come under phonological dyslexics and if the children with dyslexia

have difficulties in lexical skills, he/she may fall under surface dyslexia.

Edwards and Hogben (1999) included another type of dyslexia called mixed dyslexia,

dyslexia of this subtype tend to find difficulties in both lexical and sublexical route of reading ,

along with already proposed two types. There are several other subtypes of dyslexia

proposed by other investigators. Boder (1973) classifies dyslexics into dysphonetic (having

difficulties especially with grapheme–phoneme conversion, i.e. with the indirect route for

reading), dyseidetic (having difficulties with visual recognition of whole words, i.e. with the

direct route for reading) and mixed (having both types of difficulties) subtypes. Bakker (

1979, 1990) classifies dyslexia into P-types (relying on perceptual, analytical strategies for

reading, which turns out to be slow, fragmented and hesitating), L-types (relying on linguistic,

anticipatory strategies for reading, which allow for quicker reading but produce many,

usually plausible and context-based errors) and M-types (mixed types, showing both slow,

fragmented reading and many errors). Children with dyslexia and their subtypes need to be

identified with appropriate assessment measures so that the intervention for the same can

go according to the subtype of dyslexia that has been identified.

There are several western assessment tools available to assess dyslexia using

standard scores. For e.g., Stanford diagnostic reading test-4(SDRT 4) by Karlsen and Gardner

(1996), Test of Reading comprehension-3(TORC3) by Brown, Hammil and Weiderholt (1995),

Woodcock Reading Mastery tests-Revised (WRMT-R) by Woodcock (1997), Oral and Written

language scales (OWLS) by Carrow & Woolfolk (1995) , Test of Written expression (TOWE)

by McGhee, Bryant, Larsen & Rivera (1995), Test of Written Language -3 (TOWL 3) by
Hammill and Larsen (1996),Test of Written Spelling-3 (TWS-3) by Larsen and Hammill (1994).

However, existence of successful assessment tool in Indian multilingual context has been

challenged. The purpose of an assessment should be to produce an appropriate individual

education plan (IEP) that clearly identifies the individual’s strengths and weaknesses, their

specific needs, and the timescale and resources required to implement the IEP. It should

attempt to match the teaching style to the preferred learning style of the individual in order

to maximize the amount learnt in a given time. The term dyslexia may be used to refer to

children presenting very different profiles of strengths and weaknesses, and therefore may

be of limited use in determining the IEP. What is required is greater specificity, such as a

child’s ability in phonological and non phonological tasks, which will enable us to classify

them under appropriate subtype, which will intern pave way for appropriate treatment plan.

Thus need to understand the cognitive profile and attainment measures become even more

important for planning intervention program for individual with dyslexia.

Moreover, the estimated prevalence rate of learning disability have been found to

range from 3% to 10% (Snowling, 2000) that is approximately 1 in 59 or 1.69% or 4.6 million

people in USA. Prevalence rate in India ranges from 3% to 10 %( Ramaa, 2000). The high

prevalence rate of learning disability (3% to 10%, Ramaa, 1985) indicates the need for early

identification and intervention based on individual performances. Reviewing the available

literature, it has been found that dyslexic population in vastly heterogeneous and this

heterogeneous nature in the dyslexic group poses the requirement to profile the learning

disabilities on individual based performance. To avail the existing treatment program

appropriately, it is important that the children with dyslexia are sub grouped under existing

subtypes based on their individual performances after profiling.


Objectives of the study

I. To study the development of reading and writing skills in Indian children.

II. To identify subtypes of dyslexia based on the profiles established on children with

dyslexia
CHAPTER 2

Review of literature

Literacy is defined as the minimal ability to read and write in a designated language,

as well as a mindset or way of thinking about the use of reading and writing in everyday life.

Literacy, requires an active, autonomous engagement with print and stresses the role of the

individual in generating as well as receiving and assigning independent interpretations to

messages (Harris & Hodges, 1995).The components of literacy includes reading, writing, and

listening (Cunningham, 1995). Other components of literacy include phonological

awareness, phonological decoding, reading comprehension, spelling, orthographic

knowledge and rapid automatised naming (Gayan & Olsen, 2003).

2.1. Development of reading and writing skills.

Before one becomes completely literate, he or she must develop early reading skills

which comprise of five steps (Strommen & Mates, 1997), in which each successive step is

built up on the established previous stage.

First area to develop is literacy awareness, which is concerned with the

young child’s understanding and experience of print.

The second area of reading development is syntactic awareness or knowledge of

grammatical structure.

The third area of reading development is word recognition. In this stage children are

recognizing words as visual shapes rather than as combinations of letters.


The fourth area is development in phonological awareness. Phonological skills

develop across time and that various aspects of phonological sensitivity are

differentially predictive of reading (Lonigan, Burgess, Anthony, & Barker, 1998).

The final area of early reading is orthographic awareness. It is defined as knowledge

about the writing system and how letters and letter strings are used to represent

words. Evidences from literature reveals that has highly developed perceptions from

the time of birth. With in a few days, a baby is able to distinguish the mothers voice

from others voices and from less meaningful sounds. Thus the receptive faculty, the

ability to distinguish and understand what is heard, develops from before oral

language skill. Reception and expression of oral language are the child’s first and for

several years only means of communication; they are considered most critical in the

development of reading skills.

Though there are several prerequisites for later reading development, not all the

prereading skills are proven to be significantly influencing the later reading development.

Phonological awareness has been shown to be a primary factor underlying early reading

development (Ehri, Nunes, Willows, Schuster & Shanahan, 2001). Deficits in phonological

awareness have been linked to reading disabilities (Lyon, Shaywitz & Shaywitz, 2003).

Phonological awareness, when compared to many other predictors, was the most stable and

robust indicator of later reading in the group of children who were followed from late

preschool to kindergarten and first grade (Adams, 1990; Bryant, MacLean, Bradley &

Crossland, 1990; Lundberg, Frost, Cunningham, 1991; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 2001; Lonigan,

Burgess & Anthony, 2000). Manis, Seidenberg,Doi , McBride-Chang and Petersen (1996),

Pennington, Cardoso-Martins, Green and Lefly (2001) and Torgesen, Wagner, Rashotte,

Burgess and Hecht (1997) suggested after their longitudinal studies that phonological tasks

like alliteration and rapid naming(RN) were differentially related to reading ability.
Specifically it has been suggested that although alliteration is the most important for the

development of the ability to learn to read by phonological recoding, RN skills may be

especially important for learning about the orthography of word. Naming objects rapidly has

been considered as a skill necessary for fluent, skilled reading. Indeed, RN has been

consistently found to be related to fluency of text reading even in the present study. The

individual’s writing ability can be a good predictor of reading thus spelling as a significant

predictor of reading (Read, 1971; Clarke, 1988; Dyson 2001; Richgels, 2001; Whitehurst &

Lonigan, 2001).

Once children begin reading however the best indicator of current and future

reading may simply be reading itself (Bell, McCallum & Cox, 2003). Wagner et al., (1997)

experimented the amount of information that a measure of phonological awareness could

add to the prediction of reading once a measure of current word reading and vocabulary

was considered .The results revealed that the phonological awareness is less efficient in

prediction of reading as the child gets older. It is well accepted that reading consists of

two components, decoding and comprehension (Aaron, Joshi & Williams, 1999). Decoding is

the word recognition process that transforms print to words, whereas comprehension

assigns meanings to words, sentences and texts. Catts and Kamhi (1999) reported that word

recognition relies heavily on phonological and lexical knowledge where as comprehension of

larger discourse units requires syntactic, morphologic, semantic and discourse knowledge. A

logical consequence of the language basis of reading is that children who have deficiencies in

one or more aspects of language will experience difficulty learning to read.

When the child enters school and turns to the task of reading, the visual

discrimination task becomes of key importance. This skill must already be highly developed

before letter recognition and the ability to identify the relatively small differences between

one letter and another will be possible. In addition, the social and emotional development of
the child will prove vital to all school experiences. These prerequisite skills flow in to and

influence the acquisition of the beginning reading skills. A child of average intelligence is

expected to learn to read if he has only reasonably good teaching. Learning to read, for

many children seems not to require much effort. They appear to attain this ability almost

incidentally. They quickly make associations between the printed symbol, the auditory

symbol and meaning which is referred to as grapheme to phoneme correspondence.

Meanwhile for the development of reading patterns, ordering skills or the skills of

directionality are required.

The acquisition of phonic skills or the acquisition of the relationship between specific

sounds and specific symbols must be mastered in order for the children to succeed in their

reading journey. However the relationship between the grapheme and the phoneme in all

the language are not linear .The languages where there is only one sound for each letter, the

assigning phoneme to a grapheme step is relatively easy. But in languages like English the

association of various different letters sounds with a particular letter and various different

letters with a particular sound makes mastery of the sound symbol relationship more

difficult. Further to master in reading skill, the student must also be able to blend these

sounds together into a recognizable word and to development the reading patterns,

ordering skills or the skills of directionality are required. Additional abilities of word or the

ability to divide words into syllables and use of structural word analysis to distinguish

prefixes, suffixes and root meanings are also equally vital in reading process. Simultaneous

with the acquisition of the phonic skills is the assignment of meaning to words encountered

in print much of the initial meaning of these words is drawn from the children’s background

experiences in their environment. Comprehension skills develop and mature as the child

encounters words in print that appear in a new context. The child learns that words have

multiple meanings and that some words have implied or connotative meaning according to
the culture. As sentences combine into paragraphs and paragraphs into stories, the

meanings of words and ideas become more complicated. Both literal and interpretive skills

become increasingly important.

Further reading process can be explained through Dual Route Cascaded (DRC) model

of reading. According to this model there are two routs in translating print in to sound. A

lexical route, which utilize word-specific knowledge, and a non-lexical Grapheme-to-

Phoneme Conversion (GPC) route, which utilize a sub-lexical spelling-sound correspondence

rule system. The lexical route translates the pronunciation of a word based on word specific

knowledge. The route consists of three components they are semantic system, the

orthographic lexicon, and the phonological lexicon. The semantic system computes the

meaning of a word, whereas the lexicons compute the words’ orthographic and phonological

form. The non-lexical route differs from the lexical route in both the knowledge base and the

type of processing it employs. The non-lexical route generates the pronunciation of Letter

string (be it a word or a non-word) via a set of sub-lexical spelling-sound Correspondence

rules. The set of rules is encapsulated in the GPC module. Based on the two routes proposed,

disruption in any of the pathway may result in reading difficulties of different varieties

namely phonological and surface dyslexia. If the affected route is lexical route the patient is

said to be having surface dyslexia. The characteristics of surface dyslexia are inability to read

irregular words, while the patient has intact ability to read regular words and regular non

words. If the non-lexical GPC route is affected, the patient may show difficulties in reading

regular words and both regular and irregular non words.

In summary reading process is the conversion of print into auditory equivalents

and the subsequent interpretation of those equivalents into meanings based on previously

learned language. The reading process includes decoding of the print in to sound. Decoding

begins with the process of converting individual letters into sounds and proceeds to the

decoding of whole words and phrases. A person who cannot decode printed words cannot
understand what those words mean. Comprehension is the next step which is the ability to

interpret and understand the decoded words. Although adequate decoding is a prerequisite

for reading it is not sufficient, comprehension is also required. The ability to comprehend

what one reads is based on experience. Readers take what has been read and integrate it

with previously learned language and experience. The next component of literacy is writing.

Written language is characterized by rules of phoneme grapheme correspondence the rules

of orthographic knowledge (processing written language letters and letter patterns),

phonological knowledge (processing or manipulating oral language sounds), and

morphological awareness in spelling performance have been well documented (Nagy,

Berninger, Abbott, Vaughan & Vermaulen, 2003). Spelling development in normal children

can be derived from connectionist model (Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989) of alphabetic

spelling development. Loosemore, Brown and Watson (1991) studied the development of

alphabetical spelling system using the artificial connectionist model and concluded that

during the early course of development, they observed regularization errors and these

errors faded as the model was exposed to more alphabetical spelling system.

According to Gentry (1982), as the grade increases several stages in spelling

development like precommunicative (where the child uses symbols from the alphabet but

shows no knowledge of letter-sound correspondences), semi phonetic(sounds are assigned

to letters), phonetic (The child uses a letter or group of letters to represent every speech

sound that they hear in a word), transitional (the speller begins to assimilate the

conventional alternative for representing sounds),and correct stage(the speller knows the

English orthographic system and its basic rules).

According to Frith (1985) spelling and reading development interact leading to

increased proficiency in each ability. He proposed of three stages through which a child goes

through while acquiring spelling and reading like logographic, alphabetic, and orthographic.
Literacy development begins with logographic reading where child acquires a small sight

vocabulary of written words and word recognition visually based but becomes increasing

less efficient with development. E.G.’ yellow' recognized by the “two sticks in the middle of

the word” 'follow' read as “yellow” due to the double ’l' shared by both words. In the

alphabetic stage of spelling development some phonological awareness is required. The

child’s wish to write brings about change from logographic stage to alphabetic stage. Here

by practicing spelling child learns that spoken words can be broken down into speech sounds

(phonemes) that map onto letters. Even though child applies letter-sound rules in spelling

but rely on visual cues for reading. In the orthographic stage through considerable practice

at reading using an alphabetic strategy child learns to recognize words as orthographic units.

Further in this stage, word recognition occurs by accessing stored internal representations of

abstract letter-by-letter strings. Orthographic representations used in this stage of reading

are precise enough to be transferred to spelling. Thus, orthographic reading drives the

development of orthographic spelling skills. To summarize Firth’s stages, it can be correlated

with Gentry’s stages which says that spelling shifts from phonetic, to transitional, to correct

spellings.

Writing is defined as a complex process linking language, thought and motor skills.

Early writing is more closely linked to early spelling than to early reading. When children

begin to spell, they use a simple decoding strategy based on sound-to-symbol

correspondence, whereas early readers rely on a visual approach to word recognition (they

tend to use contextual cues). Marsh, Friedman, Welch & Desberg (1980) investigated and

reported that at a later stage, most children seem to learn to alternate between the two

strategies. However this may not happen for children who have difficulty in learning to read.

As a result spelling is related to reading and written expression. Spelling is one of the

most valued yet difficult skills in written communication. Spelling requires matching the
sounds of language with the appropriate letters in order to accurately and reliably conveys

messages. A child’s ability to spell words correctly shows a sophisticated knowledge of

letters, sounds and syllable patterns (Bear & Templeton, 1998). In children with dyslexia

(CWD), damage to the orthographic input lexicon leading to inability to recognize words

lexically, so they would not spell lexically and so will be regularizing the irregular words (Bub,

Cancelliere & Kertesz, 1985; McCarthy & Warrington, 1986)

Literacy development is a multidimensional, complex process requiring adequate

exposure to print and word knowledge which emerges with appropriate teaching. Fletcher

et al (2003) has quoted that reading is not natural and everyone is taught to read. Those

children who lack the ability to learn literacy skills are considered ‘dyslexic’. Despite years of

research, mostly in the English language, there is still no consensus as to the definition of

dyslexia, nor its underlying cause. The current dominant view argues that the cause of the

educational difficulties faced by dyslexics centre around core difficulties in phonological

awareness and word decoding skills. Smythe and Everatt (2000) define that

“Dyslexia is a difficulty in the acquisition of literacy skills that may be caused

by combination of phonological processing, visual and auditorysystem deficits.

Lexical confusions and speed of processing difficulties may also be present. The

manifestation of dyslexia in any individual will depend upon not only individual

cognitive differences, but also the language used.”

Learning disabilities is a generic term that refers to a heterogeneous group of

disorders manifested by significant difficulties in the acquisition and use of listening,

speaking, reading and writing, reasoning a or mathematical abilities. These disorders are

intrinsic to the individual and presumed to be due to central nervous system dysfunction

even though a learning disability may occur concomitantly with other handicapping
conditions(i.e. sensory impairment and mental retardation), social and economic

disturbances or environmental influences (i.e. cultural differences , insufficient /in

appropriate instruction, psychogenic factors), it is not the direct result of those conditions or

influence (Hammil, Leigh, McNutt & Larsen ,1987).

One of the evidences is provided by Scarborough (1990), who after examining the

very early language deficits in 52 children with dyslexia, grouped 30 month old subjects into

three sets. The first group had 20 children from families with an incidence of dyslexia who

subsequently became disabled readers; the second group had 12 children from families with

incidence of dyslexia became normal readers and the third group had 20 children who and

no history of dyslexia (i.e. they were normally achieving). Scarborough (1990) found that

children who demonstrated dyslexia usually experienced difficulty with three emergent

literacy skills during the preschool period. The results showed that, at 2.5 years they

produced shorter, syntactically simpler sentences with less accurate word pronunciations

than other 2 year old while demonstrating normal lexical or speech discrimination skills. At

three years of age, they began to demonstrate deficits in receptive vocabulary and object-

naming abilities. At 5 years of age, they exhibited problems in object naming, poor rhyme

recitation abilities, poor letter sound knowledge, and phonemic awareness deficits.

Difficulties of individuals with dyslexia extend beyond the domain of written

language and can be found in performance on a range of tasks that require phonological

processing (Gallagher, Frith, Snowling, 2000; Vellutino, Fletcher, Snowling, & Scanlon, 2004).

Next to limitations of verbal short-term memory, deficient phonological awareness is the

most consistently reported difficulty in dyslexics (Snowling, 2000). It involves reflecting on

the sound structure of words separately from their meaning, grammar or spelling and is

tested in tasks in which syllables or phonemes have to be identified, manipulated, added or

deleted in spoken language Moreover, subtle impairments can be observed in speech


perception and production, including naming deficits, deficits in rapid automatized naming

and verbal repetition deficits (Snowling, 2000).

There are several types of learning disabilities described in literature.

Factors that contribute to dyslexia include:

Heredity: Converging evidence indicates phonological awareness skills and

subsequent deficient phonological coding of written language are genetically influenced.

Pennington (1989) concluded that Dyslexia is familial, substantially heritable, and

heterogeneous in its genetic mechanisms. At least some forms of familial dyslexia appear to

be autosomal dominant, with linkage studies supporting both major locus on chromosome

15 and genetic heterogeneity. Lubs et al., (1991) recognized an interaction between a

gene(s) for dyslexia, sex hormones, and possible even concomitantly caused immunologic

responses in the development of brain in dyslexia. Findings from linkage studies suggest that

there are genes that lead to dyslexia associated with chromosomes 15 and 6.

Brain differences: The overabundance of tree like connections produced during the

development of the human brain is usually pruned-weeded out- by a natural process of

selection resulting from chemical and experimental “environmental” influences. Galaburda

(1991) found that the right hemisphere in brains of dyslexics have too many brain cells,

suggesting that something has interfered with the normal pruning process”. He reported

that the left hemisphere planum temporale was larger in the brains of reading disabled

persons, suggesting a generalized problem with necessary developmental pruning of some

neuronal substrates. According to him an optimal match is needed between the number of

neurons and their connections in a neural net so that a particular behavior can be achieved.

Too many or too few neuron match ups can be deleterious for the developing skill. He also

hypothesized that the neurons in questions ate not only misplaced, but the affected cortex is
different in terms of its cellular and connectional architecture, hence its functional architecture

as well.

Defects in rapid temporal information processing in the nervous system (Tallal,

Miller & Fitch, 1993) maintained that temporal mechanisms in the nervous system play a

central role in aspects of information processing and production, and may be especially

critical foe the normal production and maintenance of sensory motor integration systems as

well as phonological systems. Findings from a series of studies beginning in the 1970s, led

Tallal and colleagues to conclude that some students with developmental language and

reading problems demonstrate a severe developmental deficit in processing brief

components of information that enter the nervous system in rapid succession, and a

concomitant motor deficit in organizing rapid sequential motor output (Tallal et al.,

1993).They described this deficit as highly specific , impinging primarily on neural

mechanisms underlying the organization of information with in the tens of millisecond

range.

Selective attention and attention deficit disorder: The primary purpose of reading is

to obtain the author’s intended meaning. To do this one, must proceed through a series of

reading skills. The most fundamental of these is the ability to attend selectively to relevant

features while ignoring irrelevant stimuli. Selective attention develops with maturation and

learning. A lag in its development can contribute to difficulties in reading, writing and

spelling. When more then one learning disability co-exists with dyslexia, the co morbid

condition is called dyslexia plus syndrome (California Department of Education,

1994).Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder has been implicated as a contributing factor to

specific reading disability and in many cases, co-occurs with it. Zentall (1993) found that
students with ADHD are more likely to receive lower grades in academic subjects and on

standardized reading and math tests that their normally achieving peers.

Middle ear problems: Roberts & Medley (1995) hypothesized that hearing loss

associated with persistent and /or recurrent OME causes the child to receive a partial or

inconsistent auditory signal. The child consequently encodes information incompletely and

inaccurately into the database from which language develops. A child may then be at a

disadvantage for learning speech and language. This, in turn may negatively influence later

academic achievement, particularly in reading and other language based subjects.

Additionally, a child who hears a decreased or inconsistent auditory signal may tune sound

out and become inattentive.

Cognitive rigidity and learned helplessness: Possibly as a result of one or more of the

above factors, many students with specific reading disabilities display cognitive rigidity that

slows information retrieval. Clay (1984) suggested that students with reading disabilities

have stopped making appropriate responses, and instead, rigidly specialize unparticular

types of responses many of which are ineffective.

The causes of dyslexia can be comprehensively explained by the model given by

Morton-Frith (1993). According to the model, four factors namely biological, cognitive,

behavioral and environmental influences are essential for normal reading development.

Genetic inheritance and bio-physico-chemico environment are assumed to be contributing

to biological factors, which is internal and exhibited through reading problems. Cognitive

processing in form of phonological processing, auditory processing, visual processing, speed

of processing and lexical access too contribute to the successful reading .Other external

environmental factors like language abilities of the child, teaching at school, socio economic

status of the child and culture in which the child is being raised are assumed to be
influencing the reading development of the child. Because any of the causal and contributing

factors previously presented may interfere with the normal, developmental progression of

reading acquisition, a child may enter school unprepared for the materials and approaches

used in the regular curriculum. Smythe (1999) reports, dyslexia may be caused by a

combination of phonological, visual and auditory processing deficits. He adds that Word

retrieval and speed of processing difficulties may also be present. A number of possible

underlying biological causes of these cognitive deficits have been identified, and it is

probable that in any one individual there may be several causes .In addition dyslexia may

also be related to a number of problems which may include some or all of the following:

Making errors with numbers (telephone numbers, reversing bus number etc.)

Difficulty with organizational skills, including time management

Misplacing personal items such as keys.

Making mistakes copying things down (instructions, number etc.)

Confusing dates, and missing appointments

Difficulty with orientation, e.g. with maps or in strange towns

Confusing left and right

Problems with explaining ideas and concepts, particularly on paper.

Word finding difficulties, and mispronunciation of long words.

While the dyslexics may experience difficulties in the acquisition of reading, writing

and spelling, they can be taught to find strategies and alternative learning methods to

overcome most of these and other difficulties. However some problems, such as poor

spelling, may persist into adulthood. He also admits that every dyslexic is different, and

should be treated as an individual. Werker & Tees (1987), revealed auditory processing

deficit in dyslexics. Study by Tallal, Miller & Fitch (1993) from their study after extensive

research about the processes those are affected in dyslexics concluded that temporal aspect
in auditory and visual processing are deficient in dyslexics. They explain that the acquisition

of higher level speech processing of basic sensory information entering the nervous system

in rapid succession, ‘within milliseconds’. Thus there is a basic temporal impairment, a tenth

of millisecond delay in the time information from peripheral sensory apparatus is relayed to

the central nervous system .The delay in temporal integration causes a cascade effects,

starting with the normal development of efficient phonological abilities, and in turn resulting

in subsequent failure to learn to speak and read normally. Normal temporal sequencing is

essential to develop neural representation of phonemes, which must be distinguished from

the stream of speech and combined to form words (Tallal, Merzenich, Miller & Jenkns,

1998).

The deficit in temporal sequencing however is not limited to the auditory area. The

magnocellular visual pathway that integrates rapid reception and rapid processing of visual

information has been described as poorly functioning in dyslexia compared with the slower

processing component of the visual system, the parvocellular system, the component

important for analyzing a scene in greater and more leisurely detail.

2.2. Theories of Normal Reading

Cognitive neuropsychological dual route models (see Figure 1) propose that a skilled

reader uses two main procedures for converting print into speech which are referred to as

the lexical and sublexical processing routes. The lexical reading route relies on visual word

recognition through access to an internal store of learned familiar words. This route allows

successful and efficient processing of familiar words (both regular and irregular), but not

novel words or pronounceable nonwords, thus the need for an alternative sublexical reading

procedure. The sublexical reading procedure involves rule-based grapheme-to phoneme

conversion and allows the skilled reader to “sound-out” unfamiliar words and nonwords.
The development of cognitive models of reading has largely been based on research

investigating normal skilled reading and acquired reading disorders of adulthood. However,

the suitability of applying such models to developmental disorders has been debated

( Bishop, 1997; Frith, 1985; Seymour, 1987; Seymour & Bunce, 1994) due to their failure to

specify explicitly the components and contents of partially formed systems and the

principles by which an “illiterate” cognitive system changes or develops into a fully skilled

adult system (Seymour, 1990). Developmental stage models have been proposed as an

alternative theoretical framework (Frith, 1985; Seymour, 1987, 1990).

Figure. 1: Cognitive neuropsychological dual route model of reading (adopted from

Newcombe & Marshall, 1984).

In brief, however, developmental models emphasize the importance of

developmental aspects of learning to read and stress that different reading and spelling

processes are favored at various stages of normal reading development. They stress the

need for models of developmental dyslexia to incorporate an explanation of the processes


by which children achieve skilled reading. The challenge facing current theorists in this area

lies in the formulation of a “static” model that can successfully represent a fluid (i.e.,

constantly changing) developing process. In addition, such a model would need to

incorporate individual differences in innate preferences (or strengths and weaknesses) in

early reading, as well as the possibility that the manner in which children acquire reading

skills depends on teaching strategies employed in the classroom (Mann, 1986; Seymour,

1987; Seymour & Elder, 1986; Seymour & Evans, 1992; Stuart, 1995; Stuart, Masterson, &

Dixon, 2000).

There is now growing evidence that current dual route models of adult skilled

reading can be applied to developmental reading disorders (e.g., Castles & Coltheart, 1993;

Coltheart, Bates & Castles, 1994). More specifically, one can propose that developmental

dyslexia reflects a selective failure or delay in the acquisition of specific components of the

model (Coltheart et al., 1994). A growing number of case studies of developmental dyslexia

as well as studies of normal reading development are supporting this proposal (e.g., Castles

& Coltheart, 1993; Coltheart et al., 1983; Edwards & Hogben, 1999; Hanley, Hastie & Kay,

1992; Temple & Marshall, 1983). It has been hypothesized that the representations of

speech sounds (phonological representations) are coarsely coded and under-specified in

phonological dyslexics (Elbro, 1996; Hulme & Snowling, 1992 and Snowling, 2000). In surface

dyslexics the disconnection is in between the semantics and phonological functions leaving

only the sub lexical route in operation (see Figure.1). So, the reading of non words is

relatively intact in this population. Most studies in literature have found that surface

dyslexics had weaker phonological deficits than phonological dyslexics (Manis, McBride-

Chang, Seidenberg, Keating, Doi & Munson, 1997; Stanovich, Siegel & Gottardo, 1997).

Castles and Coltheart (1993) propose that children with developmental dyslexia can

be broadly classified into two main subtypes, those with a relative delay in the development
of lexical or sublexical reading skills, commonly referred to as surface and phonological

dyslexia, respectively. Current classification systems such as this readily highlight children

with specific reading disorders (e.g., surface or phonological dyslexia). However, there is a

significant proportion of children who present with a “mixed” picture of reading difficulties

incorporating characteristics of both phonological and surface dyslexia subtypes (Edwards &

Hogben, 1999), the mixed dyslexia subtype. Cognitive neuropsychologist s have paid little

attention to this subgroup and it has been suggested that the cognitive neuropsychological

approach “may not be very useful” for them in clinical settings (Coltheart et al., 1994).

Although a child with mixed dyslexia has deficits in both the lexical and sublexical

processing routes, both have numerous sub-components, any one (or more) of which could

be impaired. Without a theoretically driven framework and methodical assessment of both

processing routes the nature of impairment and general treatment focus is difficult to

determine.

2.3. Subtypes of Developmental dyslexia.

There are several subtypes of dyslexia reported in literature, in some cases, Dyslexia

is characterized by distinct patterns of impairment on auditory, visual or cross-modal tasks.

Among the well-known classification systems for subtypes of dyslexia is Boder’s classification

(Boder, 1973). It classifies dyslexics into dysphonetic (having difficulties especially with

grapheme–phoneme conversion, i.e. with the indirect route for reading), dyseidetic (having

difficulties with visual recognition of whole words, i.e. with the direct route for reading) and

mixed (having both types of difficulties) subtypes. Another sub typing system is Bakker’s

classification (Bakker, 1979, 1990) into P-types (relying on perceptual, analytical strategies

for reading, which turns out to be slow, fragmented and hesitating), L-types (relying on

linguistic, anticipatory strategies for reading, which allow for quicker reading but produce
many, usually plausible and context-based errors) and M-types (mixed types, showing both

slow, fragmented reading and many errors). In Bakker’s model, the two subtypes are

characterized by a different degree of involvement of the two cerebral hemispheres in the

reading process, the right hemisphere being more activated in reading for P-types, the left

one for L-types. Both classification systems, therefore, suggest that visual functions can be

impaired or under activated in different dyslexia subtypes.

Ziegler, Castel, Pech-Georgel, George, Alario and Perry (2007) investigated

developmental dyslexia within a well-understood and fully specified computational model of

reading aloud: the dual route cascaded model (DRC) (Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, &

Ziegler, 2001). The authors designed four tasks to assess each representational level of the

DRC.i.e, letter level, orthographic lexicon, phonological lexicon, and phoneme system. The

data showed no single cause of dyslexia, but rather a complex pattern of phonological,

phonemic, and letter processing deficits. Importantly, most dyslexics had deficits in more

than one domain. Sub typing analyses also suggested that both the phonological and surface

dyslexics almost always had more than a single underlying deficit. Manis, Seidenberg, Doi,

McBride-Chang and Petersen (1996) in their study reported of two subgroups that formed

their data were phonological and surface dyslexia.

Stanovich, Siegel and Gottardo (1997) identified 17 phonological and 15 surface

dyslexics from their sample of 68 reading disabled 3rd grade children by comparing them to

chronological age controls on exception word and pseudo word reading. However, when the

dyslexic subtypes were defined by referring to Reading level controls, 17 phonological and

only one surface dyslexic were identified. When the chronological age defined subtypes

were compared to reading level controls, the phonological dyslexics displayed superior

exception word reading but displayed deficits in psuedoword naming, phonological


sensitivity, working memory, and syntactic processing. The surface dyslexics in contrast

displayed a cognitive profile remarkably similar to that of the reading level controls.

The cognitive mechanism underlying subtypes Dyslexia are still a matter of debate.

Numerous theoretical approaches have identified different potential causes of dyslexia. The

phonological theory (Snowling, 2000) which is the most influential account for reading

problems relates dyslexia to a deficit in phonological awareness. Study by Moats and

Foorman (1997) and Adams (1990) also suggests that children who learn to read

alphabetical language system such as English proven to be having poor phonemic awareness

tasks like rhyming and letter identification. In contrast, the auditory processing deficit theory

(Tallal, 1980) assumes that dyslexics have deficit in (rapid) auditory processing .According to

this theory; phonological problems are only secondary to the auditory deficits. Yet other

researchers conceptualize dyslexia as a visual processing deficit arising from the impairment

of the visual magnocellular system in the brain (Stein & Walsh, 1997).The role of attentional

deficits for the development of dyslexia is also discussed (Hari & Renvall 2001).Attentional

deficits are thought to interfere with the encoding of a sequence of letters, resulting in the

confusion of letters and visual word forms. Interestingly, attentional deficits can be

dissociated from phonological deficits, and both types of deficits are valid predictors for

reading ability (Valdois, Bosse, & Tainturier, 2004).

Finally, the cerebellar theory (Nicolson, Fawcett, Berry, Jenkins, Deen & Brooks,

1999; Nicolson, Fawcett & Dean, 2001) argues that reading disabilities are a consequence of

the impaired ability to automatize processes. It is assumed that the cerebellum supports the

automatisation of basic articulatory and auditory abilities which are relevant for the

grapheme-phoneme correspondence. All of these theories have seen supporting empirical

evidence. Interestingly , however, not all dyslexics suffer from deficits in all cognitive

domains or profit equally from all remediation techniques (Ramus,2 003).Thus, it is possible
that distinguishable phenotypes of dyslexia exist on the cognitive level (Ho, Chan, Lee, Tsang

& Luan, 2000;& Ramus, 2004) for which universal or distinct genetic (Olson, 2002) and

neurobiological (Ramus, 2004) causes are controversially discussed.Unrevelling different

subtypes of dyslexia would be an essential prerequisite for developing or applying

specifically targeted and thus more efficient remediation strategies .Likewise there can be a

subtype of dyslexia named based on the homogeneity of performances of dyslexics among

group. So the present study aims at sub grouping based on profile that will be obtained.

Several assessment tools are available for the assessment of dyslexia. Those can be

broadly divided in to tests for reading and tests for writing. The assessment tools those are

available for assessing dyslexics in English are as follows,

Cloze procedure (reading assessment):

In the cloze procedure, the student must read the passage and provide the missing

words by analyzing the content and its structure. This procedure measures the reader’s

ability to interpret written passages, and it requires the student to use both comprehension

skills and knowledge of linguistic structure. The percentage of correct responses can be

determined and can serve as a rough indication of the reading level of the student. Usually

every fifth or sixth word is deleted called fixed ratio approach, however Farhady and

Keramati (1996) found that using this method versus other close methods resulted in

different interpretations of student’s reading abilities.

Maze procedure (reading assessment):

Another technique used to measure a student’s reading comprehension and

knowledge of linguistic structure in the maze procedure .This procedure is similar to the
cloze method, except that vertically presented choices are given instead of blanks. Parker,

Asbrouck, and Tindal (1992) reviewed twenty years of research on the maze procedure and

reported overall support of the technique of the maze procedure. Both the cloze and the

maze procedures are examples of informal adaptations of curriculum materials in to

assessment devices.

Diagnostic reading tests:

Diagnostic assessment tools helps screening the patient for the problem, identifying

the problem and helps in informal determination of objectives and teaching strategies, and

also in documentation of educational needs, and establishment of IEP goals. Gray oral

reading test-Diagnostic (GORT-D) by Bryant and Wiederholt, (1991)is an individually

administered set of reading tasks designed for children ages 51/2 through 12. It assesses the

paragraph reading, decoding, word attack, word identification, morphemic analysis,

contextual analysis, word ordering abilities. This test covers the main areas of oral reading

and comprehension, although it contains a large number of structural analysis tasks as well.

Stanford diagnostic reading test-4(SDRT 4) by Karlsen and Gardner, (1996) is a group

administered and individually administered instrument designed to measure a student’s

strengths and weaknesses in reading. It is used with students from the middle first grade

through grade 12.The authors suggest that the results from the SDRT4 can be used to help in

grouping students and in developing appropriate instructional strategies. The SDRT4 places

special emphasis on the low achievement student, and it contains more easy items than

many reading achievement test. This measures four major components of reading:

Vocabulary, phonetic analysis, comprehension and scanning. It has got six levels each

designed for different age ranges:

Red level-Used with students at the middle of grade 1 to middle of grade 2


Orange level-Used with students at the middle of grade 2 to middle of grade 3

Green level-Used with students at the middle of grade 3 to the middle of grade 4

and low achieving students in grade 5

Purple level-Used with students from grades 4.5 to 6.5

Brown level-Used with students from 6.5 to 8.9 and with low achieving high school

students.

Blue level-Designed for use with students from grades 9 through 12.

For each level, several tests are given that measure performance in such areas as

auditory vocabulary, auditory discrimination, comprehension, reading rate, and phonetic

and structural analysis. There is one form for the first three levels and two forms for the last

three. The SDRT4 is a well constructed diagnostic reading test. Its standardization sample

was large and representative and the reliability and the validity good. It can therefore be

used as a reasonable measure vocabulary, phonetic analysis, and comprehension and

scanning, particularly for screening purposes. It gives more normative information but less

informal information like error analysis than other diagnostic reading tests.

Test of Reading comprehension-3 (TORC3) by Brown, Hammil and Weiderholt (1995)

is an individually administered instrument designed for students between the ages of 7 and

18.It can also be used with small groups of students. The test measures the comprehension

of silent reading; therefore, individuals who have articulation or other oral reading problems

are not penalized.8 subtests are included in the TORC-3.Subtests 1 to 4 are called the

general reading comprehension core. Subtests 4 to 8 are called Diagnostic supplements

which include General vocabulary, Syntactic similarities, Syntactic similarities, paragraph

reading. The TORC3 is theoretically a measure of silent reading comprehension .Only a small

part of the test actually focuses on traditional comprehension measures (that is, on reading

a passage and answering questions about the content).


Woodcock Reading Mastery tests-Revised (WRMT-R) by Woodcock (1997a). This test

includes six individually administered tests and a two-part supplementary checklist designed

for individuals ages 5 to 75 and older. This test can be used for a variety of reasons including

diagnosis of reading problems, program planning, and program evaluation. The subtests

include, Visual –Auditory learning, letter identification, Word identification, Word attack,

Word comprehension, Passage comprehension Supplementary letter checklist. The WRMT-R

is probably the most popular norm- referenced diagnostic reading test used in special

education. The tests are easy to administer and cover a number of abilities. It is however

very tedious to score, so it is recommended the scores uses computer version of scoring. It

appears that WRMT-R has improved many of the shortcomings of the original test. Although

the norms are relatively new, the test itself, including the format and items are over 10 years

old. Thus, there may be some questions about the items being consistent with today’s

curriculum.

Oral and Written language scales (OWLS) Carrow and Woolfolk (1995). It is a Written

expression scale designed for individuals ages 5 through 21(the oral scales are for ages 3

through 21).It measures the individual’s ability to communicate using written linguistic

forms. The test measures written expression in three areas using two different methods. The

three areas are conventions(e.g. spelling, punctuation, capitalization),Linguistics(e.g. using

complex sentences, different verb forms)and content(e.g. appropriate word choice ,subject

matter).These areas are measured through indirect writing tasks such as writing dictated

sentences or combining sentences, and direct writing tasks. This test provides a potential

valuable measure of both the mechanics and fluency of writing. A wide variety of derived

scores are available for comparison purposes .One disadvantage is that the Written

Expression Scale is packed and sold separately from the Oral Scales of the OWLS. It would
seem that the advantage of the OWLS is in giving an overall picture of language using the

same comparison group. Having to use two separate tests tends to discourage that purpose.

Test of Written expression (TOWE) by McGhee, Bryant, Larsen and Rivera, (1995). It

is an individually administered instrument that measures the six areas of ideation,

semantics, syntax, spelling, capitalization, and punctuation. The TOWE is designed for use

with students from ages 6 1/5 years through 14 years, 11 months. According to the TOWE

authors, the test can be used to identify students who have writing problems and to

discover the writing strength and weaknesses. It can also be used to help evaluate a

student’s progress and can be used by researchers who are interested in studying the area

of written expression. The major sections of this test are items section and essay section.

Item section consists of subsections like ideation, semantics, Syntax, Capitalizations,

Punctuation, and Spelling. The essay component of TOWE requires the student to write an

essay based on a story in which the beginning is provided.

Test of Written Language -3(TOWL 3) by Hammill and Larsen, (1996). It was

developed to identify students who have problems with written expression, to indicate

strengths and weaknesses in writing language skills, to document progress, and to aid in

research studies related to the writing process. OWL 3 can be either individually or group

administered and is designed to be used with students ages 7 through 17.The authors divide

the area of written language into three components :conventional, linguistic and cognitive.

They also refer to two formants or methods of eliciting writing samples: contrived and

spontaneous. There are eight subtests on the TOWL-3.The first five are considered

contrived, and the second three are considered spontaneous. The capacity to use written

language is unfortunately; often overlooked in educational assessment .The TOWL-3 was

developed to offer the special educator a measure in that area. The TOWL 3 measures
several components of written language and appears to be a considerable improvement

over its processors.

Test of written Spelling-3(TWS-3) by Larsen and Hammill, (1994).Is an individually

administered or group administered test designed to pinpoint a child’s written spelling level

and to specify the types of words with which a child is having problem. Its use as a group

administered instrument is somewhat limited, however it is designed for use with children

ages 6 to 18.the TWS 3 comprises one hundred spelling words divided in to two categories:

Predictable and Unpredictable. The predictable subtest includes fifty words; these conform

to usual spelling rules and generalizations .the unpredictable subtest also includes fifty

words; these words do not follow the usual rules and therefore, primarily have to be

memorized. Although the TWS-3 yields normative data, its real value lies in the informal use

of the word lists. The idea of having predictable and unpredictable word lists is a good one.

The reliability of the newest version has improved over the earlier version, however which

makes the normative use more appropriate.

However well standardized the assessment tools are the heterogeneity in the

dyslexic group poses the requirement to profile the learning disabilities on individual based

performance. The purpose of an assessment should be to produce an appropriate individual

education plan (IEP) that clearly identifies the individual’s strengths and weaknesses, their

specific needs, and the timescale and resources required to implement the IEP. It should

attempt to match the teaching style to the preferred learning style of the individual in order

to maximize the amount learnt in a given time. The term dyslexia may be used to refer to

children presenting very different profiles of strengths and weaknesses, and therefore may

be of limited use in determining the IEP. What is required is greater specificity, such as a

child’s ability in phonological discrimination and rhyming tasks, which may inform teaching
practice. This need to understand the cognitive profile and attainment measures becomes

even more important with the assessment of the multilingual individual, since behavioral

outcomes, such as literacy difficulties, can manifest in very different ways across different

language contexts. Profiling means a lot especially in the Indian multilingual contexts as it is

difficult to generalize about the assessment and remediation of the multilingual dyslexic

individual since the context of language, culture and learning environment, can be very

diverse.

Unraveling different subtypes of dyslexia would be an essential pre-requisite for

developing or applying specifically targeted and thus more efficient remediation strategies .

Hence, the skills based on which they can be profiled should be assessed and a standard

scores should be developed based on which the dyslexic group can be compared.

Meanwhile, the high prevalence rate of learning disability (3% to 10%, Ramaa, 2000)

indicates the need for early identification and intervention based on individual

performances. The lack of Indian tools for assessment of dyslexia and the heterogeneity of

dyslexic group urges to develop a tool to profile dyslexics based on individual characteristics.
CHAPTER 3

Method

3.1. Participants

The participants of the present study included two groups. One group consisted of

60 school going normal children from grades I, II, III, IV and V. Each grade consisted of twelve

children. The other group consisted of 16 children with dyslexia (CWD) who were identified

as dyslexics at the institute using Test for Early Reading Skills (ERS) (Loomba, 1995) in the

clinical set up. All the participants were native Kannada speakers with English as the medium

of instruction.

A WHO Ten –Question Disability Screening Checklist (cited in Singhi, Kumar,

Prabhjot and Kumar, 2007 ) and Developmental Screening Test (Bharathraj, 1972) was used

to screen for normal children in terms of hearing, intelligence, motor and other factors like

school performance, emotional or behavioral factors. The participants either had normal or

corrected-to-normal vision. Written consent was obtained from all the participants prior to

testing. This study was conducted with the understanding and consent of the participants

and their parents. They were provided information in their language he/she was capable of

understanding and were explained about the aims, method of the research and approximate

duration of testing.
3.2. Procedure

The study was conducted in the following phases,

Phase I: Test Development

The test was adopted from International Dyslexia Test (IDT) developed by Ian

Smythe (2000). IDT was earlier called as International cognitive profile test (ICPT), which

included vast domains of cognitive skills for assessment of literacy skills. The International

Dyslexia Test was born out of a research project to study the role of Orthography and

phonology in dyslexia, and was first publicized at the International Dyslexia Conference in

York in April 1997. To date (November 1999) the test has been “translated” into a number of

languages including Russian, Hungarian and Chinese, with a number of others currently in

progress. The authors of the test do not claim to answer the entire question, but the test can

lead to a greater understanding of dyslexia, and lead towards improved diagnosis and

remediation of the individual with dyslexia with Tasks that have been found to be crucial for

reading and writing skill in children have been adapted for the present study and the

rationale behind assessing them has been justified in the following sections. The final test
that was derived out of the IDT after revisions was called the Dyslexia assessment profile in

Indian children (DAPIC).

The subtests adopted from the IDT (Smythe, 2000) included,

 Alphabet

 Shape copying

 Spelling

 Reading words and non words

 Phonological awareness skills

 Word non word repetition

 Rapid naming

 Sound discrimination

The alphabet subtest

The first stage of decoding stage is to learn that reading involves use of codes.

Words are not written in arbitrary ways but according to an alphabetic principle by which

letters have a regular and predictable relationship with sounds. Children come to

understand that the alphabetic principle simplifies the reading process and that it is crucial

that they attend to all of the letters to read accurately .The reader must also know the

specific correspondence between letters and letter patterns and sounds. Acquiring this

knowledge is primary task of decoding stage (Loomba, 1995). The alphabet subtest will help

us know whether confirms that children have acquired this knowledge.


Shape copying

It is a test of visual perception and visual motor integration. This involves demonstrating

children’s competence by executing complicated motor operations such as drawing

geometric forms from memory, tracing and copying (Crandall , Hammill, Witkowski &

Barkovich (1968).Shape copying will help us further to know whether the child’s fine motor

skills are ready and are prepared to be used for writing skills.

Spelling

Normal spelling needs auditory and visual reception, auditory and visual memory,

auditory and visual discrimination, association of auditory and visual stimuli, motor

expression and vocal expression. Ability to spell enables the writer to concentrate on the

ideas he wishes to convey rather than on the mechanics of writing. (Frostig and Maslow,

1967).

Reversals, Omissions and poor spacing are a characteristic of a young child’s writing.

It is the persistence of such errors over a long period of time that is indicative of writing

difficulty. Johnson and Myklebust (1967), suggest that such difficulties are associated with
deficiencies in visual motor integration (dysgraphia), revisualization (memory), and

formulation (syntax).

Reading word and Non word

The rationale for assessing person’s reading rate lies in the centrality of automaticity.

Extremely slow reading stimulates the decoding of a student with Learning disability.

Decoding problems consume his/her cognitive resources, leaving little of them for

comprehension, eventually reading comprehension becomes more difficult. An inability to

decode non-words highlights the grapheme phoneme translation difficulties may lie at the

heart of an individual’s specific learning difficulties. It also establishes the subject’s degree of

familiarity with the possibilities of written English orthography (Ehri, 1992).

Phonological awareness skills

Children start to recognize similarities in sounds between words which have

different meanings. It has been demonstrated that this prepares them well for learning

about orthographic links between words once they begin the formal construction of linking

phonemes to graphemes, (Maclean, Bryant & Bradley, 1987). The phonological awareness

like rhyming and alliteration skills have been found to become predictors of reading skill in

children learning to read English..


Word, Non word repetition

Word repetition in very young children have been found to be related to the vocabulary

size of the child ,(Gathercole & Baddeley ,1989 ; Hoff, Cotre & Bridges 2008) .The evidence

for deficits in a core phonological domain predicts that children with below average reading

ability will experience particular problems in print related decoding tasks such as pseudo

word reading .

Word and non word repetition may become measure of phonological working

memory capacity in spite of the fact that researchers have consistently acknowledged that

the task taps many language processes, including speech perception, phonological encoding,

phonological memory, phonological assembly, and articulation. Poor performance on this

task can be an indicator of phonological dyslexia. Because repetition accuracy depends on

lexical and sub lexical properties. The Non word repetition can be used to examine the

structural properties of the lexicon in both children with normal language and with specific

language problems (Anderson, 1953).

Rapid naming

Naming speed task assess the rate at which a verbal label for high frequency visual

stimuli is produced. If an individual takes much longer than average to name all the stimuli,

that individual is said have a naming speed deficit. Bowers and Wolf (1993) stated that slow

naming speed is implicated in failure to learn to recognize words quickly. Wolf, Bowers and
Biddle (2000) stated that naming speed (particularly serial naming speed) provides an early,

simpler approximation of the reading process. They believed that naming speed is

conceptualized as a complex ensemble of attentional, perceptual, conceptual, memory,

phonological, semantic and motoric sub processes that place heavy emphasis on precise

time requirements within each component and across all components.

Sound discrimination

Phoneme discrimination is the auditory processing skill, where a few Children with

dyslexia have been reported to have difficulties in discriminating between two similar

sounds auditory. ( Morais ,Content , Cary, Mehler , & Segui 1989).

The tasks of the material have been divided under phonological and non phonological

depending on the rationale those they assess phonological skills specifically and non-

phonological skills in general. The tasks those are considered as phonological are Alphabet,

SDis, NWreading, NWrep, alliteration and rhyming. And the tasks those are considered as

non phonological are Wrep, Wreading, HQ, SC, RN and spelling. The following Table shows

illustrations of tasks and scoring.

Phase II: Test administration

Testing environment
The test was administered in a quiet, noise free set up using a paper and pencil only.

Table 1 below shows the subtests for the present study and the instructions that were given

o the children prior to testing each of the tasks. The scoring was done as given in the Table 1.

Table 1. The task and scoring for each of the subtests

S.No. Subtest Task Scoring

1. Alphabet To write the alphabet that is named. 1- Correct


Alphabets those are visually similar are
considered. Which includes 0- Incorrect
b,d,n,u,m,w,p,9,q Total score: 9

2. Shape copying(SC) Copy the shapes that are given. 7- Most


approximating
There are totally four shapes
shape

1- Least clear
(The shape that is displayed supposedly shape.
the most complicated) Score:7

1 for correct and 0


for incorrect
shapes for another
three simple
shapes.

Score:3

Maximum score:10

3. Written Language Has to write down the spelling for 1- Correct


words and non words that are dictated.
a)Spelling There are 30 words, 10 non words. 0- Incorrect
However, the non words are also Total score:40
following the phonotactics of English

b)Handwriting Scored from Spelling performances 5- Very good


quality(HQ) handwriting

1- Bad handwriting.

Total score: 5
4. Reading Has to read the words that are 1- Correct
word(Wreading) given(list consists of few irregular
words) 0- Incorrect

Total score: 70

5. Reading non Has to read the non words that are 1- Correct
word(NWreading) given
0- Incorrect

Total score: 10

6. Repetition of Has to repeat the word after examiner. 1- Correct


word(Wrep)
0- Incorrect

Total score: 7

7. Repetition of non Has to repeat the nonword after 1- Correct


word(NWrep) examiner
0- Incorrect

Total score: 8

8. Rhyming and Has to find out the word which are in 1- Correct
Alliteration rhyme
0- Incorrect
E.g. Bat, Mat, Wall- Here “Bat” and
“Mat” are in rhyme with each other Total score: 30
where as the ‘Wall” is not Rhyming:20
He has to find out the words which are Alliteration:10
in alliteration with other words.

E.g. Shine, Shoe, Shop, Monsoon. Here


all the words but Monsoon begin with
different letter, or not in alliteration
with other words.

9. Rapid naming(RN) Has to name the pictures those are Time taken to
given. completely name
all the pictures.

10. Sound Has to say whether presented two 1- Correct


discrimination words are same or different.
0- Incorrect
E.g.
Total score: 20
Cat, Rat- Different

Pin, Pin- Same


The scoring was done as given in the Table 1.Among all the subtests RN was the only

timed test. On the other tasks the performance was scored for correct and incorrect

responses. The responses were coded .The data was subjected to quantitative (details of

statistical procedure are mentioned in the results section) analysis and further a qualitative

analysis was also done. The results of this study have been discussed in the following

sections.
CHAPTER 4

Results

The aims of the study were

I. To study the development of reading and writing skills in Indian children.

II. To identify subtypes of dyslexia based on the profiles established on children with

dyslexia

The following statistical techniques were used to analyze the data obtained from the

study:

a) Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) was used to compare the means

across grades.

b) A post-hoc Duncan analysis was done to explore which grades are significantly

different from each other.

c) Kruskal-Wallis, test was used to check for performance of children with Dyslexia

on various tasks

d) Bonferroni test was carried out to explore the performance of children across tasks

and if they were different across grades in Children with dyslexia (CWD) group.

d)e)A multiple hierarchical regression analysis was carried out on normal group to find

out the best predictors of reading skill among all the tasks
e)f) Cluster analysis was done to derive the different clusters /subtypes among children

with dyslexia (CWD).

The results of the study are described under the following sections,

4.1 .Performance of normal children and CWD across various tasks.

4.2 .Comparison of performance of normal children and children with dyslexia (CWD).

4.3. Sub typing of dyslexia based on profiling and cluster analysis.

4.1. Performance of normal children and CWD across various tasks

Participants for the normal group consisted of 60 children from grades I to V. Each

grade consisted of 12 children. All the 12 tasks were administered on all the subjects and

results are discussed in the following sections.

Multivariate Analysis Of Variance (MANOVA) was done with grades as independent

variable and task as dependent variable. Table 2 shows mean and standard deviation (SD)

for normal children of grades I to V on all the tasks.

The Table 2 shows that the performance of normal children improved from lower

grades to higher grades on tasks like HQ, SC, spelling and Wreading. Thus, showing a

developmental trend on these tasks. It is evident from the Table, on the alphabet subset, all

children in the grade I itself were able to score the maximum .This indicates that the

development of alphabet writing have already taken place by grade I itself.


Table 2: Mean and Standard Deviation (SD) of scores of Grades I to V.

Grades

Tasks I II III IV V

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

Alphabet 9 0 9 0 9 0 9 0 9 0

HQ 3.83 0.94 4.00 0.74 4.17 0.72 4.67 0.89 4.83 0.39

RN 37.67 4.12 40.92 7.66 39.50 5.27 31.50 4.96 25.75 4.00

SC 8.08 2.07 8.75 1.22 8.92 1.24 9.42 .90 9.92 0.29

NW reading 5.17 1.75 5.08 2.78 7.00 2.26 7.33 1.83 7.58 1.97

Alliteration 2.92 2.27 3.50 2.71 6.42 2.15 6.17 2.40 9.08 1.24

Rhyming 3.92 3.39 0.42 0.99 7.33 2.35 10.33 4.70 14.50 3.00

Wrep 4.17 1.11 3.75 1.06 4.92 0.90 5.58 0.99 5.25 0.96

NWrep 4.33 0.89 4.17 1.53 5.17 1.11 6.17 0.72 6.50 0.67

SDis 17.42 1.88 17.08 1.56 18.67 1.15 19.58 1.44 20.00 0

Spelling 8.67 3.85 8.75 2.14 16.00 3.36 18.42 7.09 29.25 3.16

Wreading 15.58 7.23 20.08 4.17 26.67 4.03 31.25 10.97 54.67 6.36
[Handwriting Quality(HQ), Rapid Naming(RN), Shape Copying(SC), Non Word Reading (NWreading),

Word Repetition(Wrep), Non Word repetition (NWrep), Sound Discrimination(SDis),Word Reading(W

reading)]

On the handwriting quality (HQ) task, results showed that children in the higher

grades showed better performance than the lower grades (see Table 2) and significant

difference was noticed at F (4, 55) =3.88, p< 0.01. Post –Hoc Duncan results showed no

significant difference among grades I through III, however children in grade V performed

significantly better than Grade IV. On the shape copying (SC) task, results revealed that

children in the higher grades showed better performance than the lower grades (see Table

2) and significant difference was noticed at F (4, 55) =3.53, p<0.05 . Post –Hoc Duncan

results showed no significant difference among grades I through III, however grade V

participants performed significantly better than grade IV. On both spelling and reading tasks,

results revealed that children in the higher grades showed better performance than the

lower grades (see table 2). For the spelling task, significant difference was found at F (4, 55)

=47.44, p<0.001. For the reading task, significant difference was found at F (4, 55) =56.35,

p<0.001. The Post-hoc Duncan analysis revealed that grades I and II were not significantly

different from each other, while grade III performed significantly better than grade II. The

results also showed that grade III and IV were not significantly different from each other;

however children in grade V performed significantly better than grade IV participants.

Overall, there was a developmental pattern observed on tasks like handwriting quality (HQ),

shape copying (SC), spelling and reading words (Wreading).

Results in Table 2 further revealed that performance of children on tasks like

NWreading, rhyming, Wrep, NWrep and SDis, showed that children in grade I performed
better than grade II, this was not found to be statistically significant. However, the

performances of children improved from grade III to grade V on these tasks significantly. On

word repetition (Wrep) task, results showed that the performance of children improved from

grade III to grade V and this was found to be significant at F (4, 55) =6.81, p<0.001. On the

non word repetition (NWrep) task , results showed that the performance of children

improved from grade III to grade V and this was found to be significant at F (4, 55) =12.44,

p<0.001 level of significance. On the sound discrimination (SDis) task results revealed that

the performance of children improved from grade III to grade V and this was found to be

significant at F (4, 55) =10.56, p<0.001. For all of these three tasks, Post-hoc Duncan analysis

revealed no significant difference between grade I and II, while it showed that grade III was

significantly different than grade I and II . There was no significant difference between grade

IV and V on this task. Performance of children in grade IV and V was found to be better than

grade III.

On the non word reading (NWreading) task results, showed that grade I performed

better than grade II, and the performance of children improved from grade III to grade

V(see Table 2) and this was found to be significant at F (4, 55) =3.82, p <0.01 level of

significance. Post-hoc Duncan analysis revealed that there was no significant difference

across grade I and grade II. There was no significant difference across grades III, IV and V.

On rhyming task, significant at F (4, 55) =36.62, p<0.001 level of significance. Surprisingly,

grade II children performed significantly poorer than grade I children; however from grade III

through V the performance improved. Post-hoc Duncan analysis revealed that all the grades

performed significantly different from each other on this task and an increasing trend was

observed for d V were in increasing order (see Figure 2). Also to note, a high standard

deviation score revealed a high variability of performance amongst children on rhyming task.
On the alliteration task the performance of normal improved as the grade increased,

the pattern was observed only till grade III, while the children in grade IV students

performed better than grade V (see Table 2). For the alliteration task significant difference

was noticed at F (4, 55) =15.15, p< 0.001 level of significance. Post-hoc Duncan analysis did

not show any significant difference between grades I and II, and between grade IV and grade

III, grade V performed significantly better than grade III and IV. On the rapid naming (RN)

task, grade I took more time compared to grade III. Grade V took lesser time compared to

grade IV .Grade II took more time than grade I (see Table 2). For the RN task significant

difference was noticed at F (4, 55) =16.69, p< 0.01 level of significance. Post –Hoc Duncan

results showed no significant difference among grades I through III, however grade V

participants performed significantly better than grade IV.


Figure: 2. Performance of normal on tasks (HQ, RN, SC, NWreading and alliteration) across

grades.

As shown in Figure 2 and 3 describes, the performances on tasks has been found to

be increasing grades, on rhyming task where the grade II have performed worse than grade

I. The trend is not exactly in increasing pattern on tasks of Wrep and SDis as well.

To summarize, performance of normal children across all the tasks

HQ,SC,alphabet,spelling,Wreading,NWreading,alliteration,rhyming,Wrep,NWrep,

RN,SDis.The Figures shows that there is a developmental trend across all the tasks however,

only a few tasks have been found to be significant across grades

Figure: 3.Performence of normal on tasks (rhyming, Wrep, NWrep, SDis, spelling and

Wreading) across grades.


A multiple regression analysis for the entire sample was done to arrive at the

regression equation which was significant at F (9, 50) =44.73, p< 0.01.Combined reading task

which included word reading (Wreading) and nonword reading (NWreading) was considered

to arrive at the regression equation. The multiple regression for the entire sample revealed

that rapid naming, alliteration, sound discrimination and spelling were the four significant

predictors for the reading score (r2=0.89; Rapid Naming β =-.469; p<0.05; Alliteration β

=1.016; p<0.05; Sound Discrimination β =-1.54; p<0.05; Spelling: β=1.3 03; p<0.001.

4.2. Comparison of performance of normal children and children with dyslexia

The CWD group consisted of sixteen children with dyslexia (CWD) who participated

in the study. There were three CWD in each group of grade I, grade II, grade III and grade V.

Only grade IV consisted of four CWD. An individual profile of each child with dyslexia was

derived after a detailed qualitative analysis of all the samples.

The data was further analyzed using Kruskal-Wallis non parametric test to check for

performance of children with dyslexia on various tasks. Results showed that the
performance of children with dyslexia on tasks of alphabet, SC, NWreading, and Wrep were

not significantly different.

Independent sample–t test was administered to arrive at mean and standard

deviation (SD) to compare normal children and CWD, since they are two different groups,

following Table 3 displays the mean and SD for overall performance of normal children and

CWD.

Table 3 displays the mean and standard deviation of two groups of the study.In

order to explore the tasks those are significantly different among normal and CWD groups,

Mann-Whitney U test was administered, and results revealed that they are significant at p<

0.05. Results showed that normal group were significantly better than CWD group on most

of the tasks but CWD performed as good as normal children on phonological awareness

tasks like alliteration, rhyming and on SDis. Figure 4 shows compare the performances of

normal and CWD groups across all the tasks across grades I to V.

Table 3: Mean and SD for overall performance of normal

children and CWD

Groups
Subtests Normal CWD
N Mean SD N Mean SD
HQ 60 4.30 .83 16 2.44 1.26
RN 60 35.07 7.71 16 50.63 20.30
Alphabet 60 9.00 .00 16 7.94 1.91
SC 60 9.017 1.38 16 6.56 2.37
NWreading 60 6.43 2.35 16 2.75 1.39
Alliteration 60 5.62 3.10 16 3.94 3.13
Rhyme 60 7.30 5.79 16 4.12 4.21
Wrep 60 4.73 1.19 16 4.00 1.09
NWrep 60 5.267 1.38 16 4.12 1.26
SD 60 18.55 1.76 16 16.94 5.94

Spelling 60 16.22 8.68 16 6.50 5.39

WReading 60 29.65 15.31 16 12.38 8.71

[Handwriting Quality(HQ), Rapid Naming(RN), Shape Copying(SC), Non Word Reading (NWreading),

Word Repetition(Wrep), Non Word repetition (NWrep), Sound Discrimination(SDis),Word Reading(W

reading)]

As it can be evidently observed from the Figure, CWD were found to perform poorly

than the normal children on all tasks. Mann-Whitney U test was used to compare between

grades of normal and CWD group as well. The results are described below,
[HQ-Handwriting Quality, RN-Rapid Naming, SC-Shape Copying, NW reading-Non Word reading,

Wrep-Word repetition, NWrep-Non Word repetition, SDis -Sound Discrimination, WReading-Word

reading]

Figure: 4. Mean scores for normal children and children with dyslexia across tasks.

On the alphabet task CWD in lower grades performed poorer than normal, but it

was not statistically significant. In the grade I the tasks on those where CWD performed

similar to normal are alliteration, rhyming and Wrep. In the grade II, like grade I CWD

performed similar to normal, in tasks like alliteration, rhyming and Wrep. But unlike grade I

participants, grade II participants of CWD performed similar to normal on NWrep as well.


In the grade III, normal children performed better than CWD on tasks like HQ,

alphabet, SC, NWreading, alliteration, rhyming, spelling and on Wreading. Where as CWD

could match the performance of normal children on tasks of RN, Wrep, NWrep, and SDis.

Grade IV CWD performed significantly poorer only on tasks of HQ, RN, NWreading,

Wrep, and NWrep. On all the other tasks they were able to match the normal children

performance in this grade. Normal children in grade V and CWD was compared, the results

show that normal children of grade V performed better than CWD on most of the tasks, but

performed similar to CWDs on tasks of RN, alliteration, Wrep, NWrep and SDis.

The performance of CWD was compared with normal children qualitatively,

On qualitative analysis it was found that, on spelling and handwriting quality tasks

regularization error, inappropriate spacing between letters, unfinished or omitted

words were seen in CWD compared to normal children. When the CWD were asked

to write the alphabets, they seemed to produce the mirror images of the alphabet

they were attempting to write (e.g. writing 9 for p), though these errors were

seldom seen in young normal children.

The scores of Shape Copying task was reflected in Handwriting Quality in both

Normal and CWD group. In other words children who performed better in Shape

Copying were found to have better handwriting quality and vice versa.

On the task of alliteration and rhyming, the CWD were not able to keep the

presented three words of one stimulus in their working memory in order to identify
the words those have the same first letter or words those are in rhyme with each

other. They also showed difficulty in understanding the instruction for these tasks,

and most of the times tried to identify the first two of three presented stimuli as the

appropriate response, which might have been due to their working memory deficit

rather than a deficit in alliteration and rhyming task itself. Though these similar

errors were seen in normal children as well, however, the errors found were lesser

and more errors were found in the earlier grades in normal children.

On the Wreading task, it was evident that CWD performed much poorer than the

normal children in reading words as well as non-words. A few CWD were able to

read non words with difficulty, however most of them (CWD) performed poorly on

this task. CWD failed to use the strategy of reading the word as whole there by

implementing lesser lexical cue compared to normal. When it came to reading non

words the CWD group exhibited difficulty in implementing grapheme phoneme

rules, there by exhibiting slower decoding skills.

On the repetition (words and nonwords), compared to normal children, the CWD

showed difficulty in repetition when the length of the stimuli increased from

monosyllabic to disyllabic words .Showing that the errors made are predominantly

due to deficit in inappropriate phonological working memory.

On rapid naming task, one salient feature observed among the subjects was that

they tend to skip a whole series while naming given series of items. Otherwise there

were no qualitatively different responses observed in CWD compared to normal

children. CWD children took relatively more time in comparison to normal children.
On sound discrimination task, the quality of error observed in CWD was similar to

normal.

4.3. Sub typing of dyslexia based on profiling and cluster analysis

Cluster analysis was carried out as part of quantitative analysis for the CWD group in

order to classify them in to different clusters based on homogeneity among subjects on

various tasks. The tasks those were considered to arrive at overall Dendrogram were SDis,

Wrep, NWrep, Wreading, NWreading, alliteration, rhyming, RN and spelling. The results are

described in terms of overall clusters, phonological and non phonological clusters.

4.3.1. Overall clusters.

Following Figure 5 shows the Dendrogram obtained using phonological and non

phonological tasks.
Figure: 5 Dendrogram depicting all the clusters in the CWD group

Analysis revealed that Subjects 3, 2, 5, 6 performed similarly thereby forming a

cluster (Cluster I) themselves(see Appendix B). The typical characteristics of these clusters

were,

 Errors exhibited on alphabet task itself

 Very poor performance in spelling task

 Poor performance in alliteration and rhyming tasks.

Subject 1 (Ia) also performed similar to this cluster but it fell slightly apart, since this subject

couldn’t manage any score on Spelling task (see Appendix B).

Subject 4(Ib) Having taken maximum time for performing the Rapid Naming task

falls slightly apart from Cluster the cluster I and Ia (see Appendix B).

Subjects 7 and 9 formed the cluster II. The typical features of this cluster were,
 In the Wrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had four

words in it

 In the NWrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had three

non words in it

 Subjects scored just over ten in Wreading task, and most of the stimulus words read

correctly was with in first ten.

 Both the subjects performed poorly on rhyming task

 In the NWreading task only single syllable words were read correctly by subjects in

this cluster(see Appendix B)

Cluster III was formed by the subjects 8, 12 &13.The common features of this cluster were,

 Subjects performed similarly on Wreading task (i.e. number of correct responses

were 10and 13 respectively)

 The time taken for RN was almost same duration (38 and 37 seconds respectively)

 Performed poorly on spelling task. (see Appendix B)

Subject 11 (IIIa) was slightly different from the cluster III, since its performance on spelling

was better compared to subjects in cluster III.

Subjects 14, 15 and 16 fall in a same region of the Dendrogram forming its own cluster

(Cluster IV), the characteristic features they shared are,

 Fair performance on spelling

 On the Wrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had four

words in it

 On the NWrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had three

non words in it
 In the NWreading task only single syllable words were read correctly by subjects in

this cluster

Ultimately, lonely subject that didn’t seem to fall near any of the cluster was the 10 th

(X) subject (see Appendix B). The reason for being solace in the Dendrogram was that this

was the only subject who read even one of the bisyllabic words in NWreading

This was the only subject who read half of the words correctly in reading task (i.e.35 words),

and scored maximum in this task compared to all the other tasks.

Overall Clusters classification:

Cluster I-3, 2, 5, and 6

Ia-1

Ib-4

Cluster II-7&9

Cluster III-8, 12 &13

IIIa-11

Cluster IV-14, 15, 16


X-10

In order to classify the CWD into phonological and non phonological group the data

was further analyzed considering phonological and non phonological tasks separately. The

phonological tasks considered were SDis, NWrep, NWreading, alliteration and rhyming (see

Figure 6). The nonphonological tasks considered were Wreading, NWreading, RN and

spelling (see Figure.7).

4.3.2. Phonological clusters

The following Figure 6 shows the Dendrogram acquired for clusters among phonological

tasks.

Figure: 6 .Clusters for phonological tasks


Subjects 2, 3, 5 and 6 formed cluster a, the common phonological features they shared

in this cluster were,

 Performed good on alliteration task

 Performed good on rhyming task

 In the NWrep task the performance was poor ,managed to repeat stimulus series

which had two nonwords in it (see Appendix B).

Subjects 1 and 4 formed the cluster b, the common phonological features they

shared in this cluster were,

 In the NWrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had one

and two non words in it respectively(poor performance)

 Poor performance in NWreading, alliteration and rhyming tasks. (see Appendix B)

Cluster c was formed by subjects 7 and 9. The common phonological features of this

group were,

 In the NWrep task subjects could repeat only up to stimulus series which had three

non words in it (poor to fair performance)

 Both the subjects performed poorly on rhyming task

 In the NWreading task only single syllable words were read correctly by subjects in

this cluster (see Appendix B).

Cluster d was formed by the subjects 8, 11 &13. The common phonological features of

this cluster were,

 Read only the monosyllable words in NWreading


 Good performance in NWrep

 Performed relatively better than other clusters on alliteration task.

 Poor performance in rhyming task(see Appendix B)

Subject 12 formed the Cluster d1, since this subject performed fair in rhyming task.

Cluster e was formed by the subject 14 since this subject had salient feature of very

poor performance on rhyming and good performance on alliteration task.

Cluster f was formed by the subject 15, the salient phonological feature it had from

other clusters was that it had very good performance on alliteration and fair performance

on rhyming task.

Cluster g is formed by subjects 10 and 16 and the common features these subjects

shared were that all the phonological tasks were performed fair to good. Even very good

performance noticed in alliteration tasks of subject 10(see Appendix B).

Cluster classification for phonological tasks.

Cluster a – 2, 3, 5 and 6

Cluster b- 1 and 4

Cluster c- 7 and 9
Cluster d- 8, 11 and 13

Cluster d1- 12

Cluster e- 14

Cluster f-15

Cluster g-10 and 16

4.3.3. Non-phonological clusters.

The following Figure 7 shows the Dendrogram acquired for clusters among non phonological

tasks.

Figure: 7. Clusters for non phonological tasks


Subjects 8, 12 and 13 forms the cluster (a), the common non phonological features

of this group were, that all the subjects performed poorly on Spelling, WReading and Wrep

(see Appendix B).

Subject 7 and 9 forms the cluster (b), the common non phonological features they

shared were that they had good performance on Wrep and the correct responses in

Wreading task was over ten in both the subjects (poor).

Subjects 11 formed cluster (b1) performed fairly on spelling, fairly on Wreading ad

very poor on Wrep thus forming a different cluster (see Appendix B).

Cluster (c) formed by 15 and 16, the features of this cluster were their fair

performance on Wrep and Spelling tasks and Very poor to poor performance in Wreading

task (see appendix II).

Subject 10 formed the cluster (d) performed good on Wreading and spelling and

poor on Wrep.

Subject 14 formed the cluster (d1), since they had very poor performance in

Wreading and spelling. These clusters also had fair performance in RN and Wrep.

Subjects 2, 3, 5 and 6 forms the Cluster (e) the common non phonological features

were, They performed poorly on spelling and Wreading but fair performance on Wrep task.
Subject 1(e1) Spelled none of the words and read none of the words. The subject

repeated repeat only the first series of stimulus which had only two words in it.

Subject 4 formed the cluster (f) since they showed Very poor performance on

spelling and Wreading and fair performance on Wrep task (see Appendix B).

Cluster Classification for Non phonological tasks.

Cluster a- 8, 12 and 13

Cluster b-7 and 9

Cluster c- 15 and 16

Cluster d-10

Cluster d1-14

Cluster e- 2, 3, 5, and 6

Cluster e1-1

Cluster f-4

Following Table 4 displays the classification of subgroups of dyslexics based on phonological

and non phonological tasks.

Table 4. Sub grouping of CWD under phonological, non phonological and mixed type.
Phonological Surface Mixed

Subjects 1,4,16,14 & 10 Subject 15 Subjects 2,3,5,6,7,8,9,11, 12 and 13

Subjects were classified as phonological dyslexics {1, 4, 16, 14, 10} if they showed

difficulties on tasks of NWreading, NWrep, alliteration and rhyming. Subjects were classified

as surface dyslexics {15} if they find difficulties in tasks of Wreading, Wrep, RN and Spelling.

Subjects were classified as Mixed {2, 3, 5, 6, 8, 9, 11, 12, 13} if they show difficulty in both

phonological and non phonological tasks.

To summarize, the quantitative clusters and the qualitative analysis reveal that the

group of CWD children which seem to have been a part of the study are of mostly the

phonological(7)and the mixed dyslexia subtypes(9), however one of the surface dyslexics(1)

also identified. More number of mixed groups was found in the study compared to the

phonological types. The data again shows the existence heterogeneity in children with

dyslexia.
CHAPTER 5

Discussion

The results of the study will be discussed in terms of,

Development of reading and writing skills based on the adopted IDT (Smythe, 2000)

in normal children from grade I through grade V. This section will also include

discussion over the performance of children with dyslexia in comparison to the

normal grade level children.

Subtypes of dyslexia based on the profile

5.3 Development of reading and writing skills in normal children and performance of

CWD

Discussion for normal development and comparison of CWD with normal will be

discussed for tasks of alphabet, HQ, SC, spelling, Wreading and NWreading, Wrep and

NWrep, RN, alliteration and rhyming, SDis.

5.1.1. Alphabet task.


On the alphabet task all the normal children performed excellently. A few children

with dyslexia in lower grades (I and II) showed errors in alphabet writing which resembled

mirror images of target letter (9 for p). The mirror writing feature observed in the lower

grades (I, II and III) of dyslexics could be due to directional confusion nature for forms of

CWD Directional confusion is the reason for reversing of letters, Directional confusion also

tend to occur in handedness (right or left) Hornsby (1999). However this mirror writing

features in this population disappeared as grades improved it was appeared in lower grades.

Visual errors have also been reported by Terepocki, Kruk, and Willows (2002) who compared

10-year-old average readers and children with reading disability. The children with reading

disability made more orientation errors than average readers on computer-based reversal

detection tasks (numbers, letters, letter strings, words), and more reversal errors on

controlled writing tasks. The authors suggest that the difficulties of reading disabled group in

discriminating similar looking items could be due to poorly specified representations of

letters. They concluded that although reversal errors are likely to disappear in children with

reading disability as their reading and writing skills improve.

5.1.2. Handwriting quality (HQ) and Shape copying (SC)

It is evident from the results of the present study performance of normal children

improved from grade I to grade V on both the visuo-motor tasks such as handwriting quality

and shape copying. This may be attributed to better fine motor control over this skill with

development. It was found that performance on both the motor tasks (handwriting quality

and shape copying) have increased parallelly from lower grades to higher grades suggesting

the interdependence of these two skills for writing skill. Qualitative analysis of their

handwriting revealed that children in the lower grade wrote large letters, poorly spaced and
offline writing. Whereas, those children in the higher grades like grade IV and grade V had

improved writing in terms of better spacing, smaller and more uniformity compared to the

lower grades. Gessel and Amatruda (1947) reported that around 5-6 years the child

improves in his/her grasping and co ordination and at about 6 years copies capital letters.

The authors also reported that they can write, but the writing is large, awkward, uneven,

and irregular in size and position at around 7 years. They added that penmanship becomes

smaller and more uniform at around 9 years. These findings of the present study evidenced

that these skills are acquired much earlier that they were ought to be as mentioned in the

Gessel and Amatruda (1947) study.

With respect to children with dyslexia, the results of the present study showed that

CWD children were found to have poorer hand writing quality and shape copying in

comparison to normal children. Literature suggests that children with developmental

dyslexia have been found to have poor fine motor abilities and often poor writing skills

(Denckla, 1985). Motor problems are frequently observed in dyslexic children (Snowling,

2000, 2005). It is estimated that about 60% of children with reading disability have a

developmental coordination disorder or some other problems with motor skills (Viholainen

et al., 2002). A developmental coordination disorder, also referred to as dyspraxia, is

characterized by difficulties with gross and fine motor movements (Snowling, 2000) in

children with dyslexia, thus reflecting on their poor writing skills.

5.1.3. Spelling

Results of the present study also showed that the performance on spelling task

increased from lower to higher grades. It was found that children in the earlier grades used
symbols from the alphabet but showed no knowledge of letter-sound correspondences

(indicating the precommunicative stage of spelling development according to Gentry (1982).

In the later grades children were found to make better letter-sound correspondences and

further higher grades (grade IV and grade V), children were able to understand the basic

spelling rules of English and use them appropriately for reading purpose. The results also

support Frith’s developmental model for reading (Frith, 1985) in which she proposed three

stages which a child goes through while acquiring spelling and reading like logographic,

alphabetic and orthographic stages. Logographic stage is a more primitive stage of learning

to read whereas orthographic stage is a much developed stage of reading and the alphabetic

stage is a transition from logographic to orthographic stage. From the present study, it is

evident that the errors made by normal children (e.g., writing ‘bot’ for ‘boat’) in the lower

grades (grade I and II) could be due to the transition period from alphabetic to orthographic

stage. Empirical support for the present findings has been drawn from other studies in

literature which uses connectionist model (Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989) of alphabetic

spelling development. Loosemore, Brown and Watson (1991), studied the development of

alphabetical spelling system using the artificial connectionist model and concluded that

during the early course of development, they observed regularization errors and this errors

faded as the model was exposed to more alphabetical spelling system. The findings of

present study are tallying with the findings of Richard et al., (1991), since normal children of

lower grades performed poorly on irregular words from the stimuli. They used regular

spellings for irregular words. (e.g. ‘hart’ for ‘heart’). As the grades increased, the normal

children of the present study performed better on irregular words suggesting the

construction of orthographic lexicon in them. This was evident when the data was

qualitatively analyzed. Thus, the present study supports various other studies suggesting

different stages of spelling development in children.


Further, a regression analysis in the present study revealed that spelling is a

significant predictor of reading skill with the significant value of β=1.3 03; p<0.001.The

findings of the present study is in consonance with Read (1971), Clarke (1988), Dyson (2001),

Richgels (2001), Whitehurst and Lonigan, (2001) who also found spelling as a significant

predictor of reading in their study. This was evident when the data was qualitatively

analyzed. Thus, the present study supports various other studies suggesting different stages

of spelling development in children.

On spelling tasks, CWD were found to perform poorer than the normal children in

the present study. Qualitative analysis revealed that CWD even in the higher grades showed

more errors in comparison to young normal children. The errors revealed that CWD in higher

grades who were supposed to have reached the orthographic stage of spelling (Frith, 1985)

are still in logographic or alphabetic or in a transition from logographic stage to alphabetic

stage. For e.g., some CWD wrote ‘butter’ as ‘better’, which means that these children are still

using the logographic strategy as the two words are visually similar. Other errors like

regularization errors were again found to be more in CWD compared to normal children.

Regularization errors in CWD may be due to their poor representation of words in

orthographic lexicon. A study by Bub, Cancelliere and Kertesz, 1985, McCarthy and

Warrington, 1986 who discussed this as damage to the orthographic input lexicon because

of which CWD would not recognize words lexically, so they would not spell lexically and so

will be regularizing the irregular words. We can also hypothesize from Bub et al., 1985, and

McCarthy and Warrington, 1986) that CWD of higher grades have not completely acquired

the orthographic lexicon thus committing regularization errors (e.g. ‘cattel’ for ‘cattle’).
5.1.4. Word reading (Wreading) and Non word reading (NWreading)

The present study showed improved performance on word reading and non word

reading tasks from lower to higher grades in normal children. However, the performance of

word reading was comparatively better than the non word reading task in all the grades. The

beginners (grade I & II) seemed to read both the word and non word not analytically, but as

a whole (e.g. reading ‘yoll’ as ‘doll’). These findings are in consonance with Frith (1985)

whose model can be used to explain word reading. This could be attributed to lack of

decoding or absence of alphabetical knowledge in their repertoire in younger children. In

other words, children in their lower grades treat a visual word as a visual object. It can be

hypothesize that these lower grades normal children are still in logographic to alphabetic

stage of Frith’s model. Typically, according to Frith this alphabetical stage is followed by

orthographic stage. This refers to the mastery of the alphabetic principle of phonography

according to which written words may be segregated into left to right series of letters, each

of which can be decoded as standing for as segment of speech. These segments correspond

to the linguistic abstractions, the phonemes, by which the set of vowels and consonants

composing the syllables of the spoken language are identified.

The features observed in the present study can be contradicted with Beers (2007)

where he suggests that the first two stages of reading development (discusses only word

reading where as this stages does not explain non word reading). Stage I in her study
included decoding Stage (grades 1-2; Ages 6-7) where the student's central task is learning

arbitrary letters and associating them with corresponding parts of spoken words. The

children in grades I and II of the present study didn’t seem to use decoding strategy (letter

by letter reading) to read the given word (where they seemed to use logographic skills). The

next stage according to her is confirmation, fluency, ungluing from print, and automaticity

stage (grades 2-3; Ages 7-8) where readers do experiments with their phonic skill. This stage

is also a consolidation of what was learned in Stage 1. The children in present study of these

grades (grade III) tried experimenting with their phonic skills; this was evident from their

inquisitiveness to read the new words with their phonic skills, which enabled them to

improve in their reading scores (as you can see the increase in reading scores gradually). It

was also evident that children in these stage of the present study read words correctly even

tough they had not encountered those words in past (lack of prior word knowledge). She

calls the third stage as Reading for Learning the New Stage (Grades 4-8; ages 9-13) where

the readers need to bring prior knowledge to their readings which enable them to acquire

facts. Grade IV and V of our present study performed better compared to lower grades since

they might have used their previous knowledge of word meaning.

Poor performance of normal children lower grades on non word reading task can be

explained using the developmental stage models of reading like the dual route models

suggested in literature (Newcombe & Marshall, 1984; Castles & Coltheart, 1993). The

performance of normal children in present study improved from lower grade through higher

grades. To read a non word, it is the sub lexical processing which is important. It involves

rule-based grapheme-to phoneme conversion and allows the skilled reader to “sound-out”

unfamiliar words and nonwords. Results of the present study are indicative that probably in

the lower grades, children have still not developed the component process (GPC buffer in

case reading a non word) that requires sub lexical processing to read a non word correctly.
The present study supports various other studies who discuss about development of reading

using various models (Castles & Coltheart, 1993).

The performance of CWD was significantly poorer than normal in both word reading

and non word reading tasks. Snowling (2000) reported that CWD have problems at the

phonological level, which leads to the expectation that they should be at least slow, and at

worst fail, to develop alphabetical decoding skills .The poor performance of CWD on

Wreading can be explained through Frith’s model (1985) of reading development. According

to this model, in order to read a word, it is significant to have an orthographic stage of

reading development. Having acquired this transition from alphabetical to orthographic

stage it is possible to assign the appropriate phoneme to graphemes. In the present study

we can hypothesize that, this stage of development is deficient in CWD, thus this population

find difficulties reading word. Poor performance of non word reading in CWD can again be

explained based on dual route models (Newcombe & Marshall, 1984) like for normal

children. In literature, developmental dyslexia is considered to be a disorder which reflects a

selective failure or delay in the acquisition of specific components of the model (Coltheart et

al., 1994). CWD even in the higher grades were found to perform poorly in the present study.

It could be that CWD have still not developed the component process (GPC buffer in case

reading a non word) that requires sub lexical processing to read a non word correctly. The

present study supports various other studies who discuss about reading in developmental

dyslexia using various models (e.g., Castles & Coltheart, 1993; Coltheart et al., 1983;

Edwards & Hogben, 1999; Hanley, Hastie, & Kay, 1992; Temple, 1984; Temple & Marshall,

1983). Thus, an inadequate development of GPC buffer might be the reason for poor

performance in CWD on non word reading task.


5.1.5. Word repetition (Wrep) and Non word repetition (NWrep)

On the word repetition task, normal children in the present study, showed no

significant improvement in the lower grades (grade I and II), whereas showed significant

improvement in the higher grades (grades III, IV and V) only. Gathercole & Baddeley (1989);

Hoff, Cotre and Bridges (2008) have reported that word repetition in very young children is

related to their vocabulary size of the child. This means that children in the higher grades

have better and greater vocabulary size than the lower grades and hence the former

perform better than the latter on word repetition task indicative of an obvious

developmental trend. Findings of the present study in CWD showed that these children

performed significantly poorer than normal children on Wrep task. The difference was found

to be lesser in the lower grades and found to be more in higher grades. Studies done by

Scarborough (1990); Lyytinen, Poikkeus, Laakso, Eklund, and Lyytinen (2001), Gallagher, Frith

and Snowling (2000), Snowling, Gallagher, Frith (2003), Carroll and Snowling (2004) found

that children at risk for dyslexia show delays in lexical development, both in expressive and

receptive vocabulary. This vocabulary delays might have caused poor vocabulary repertoire

available for the CWD. According to these studies, the difference in performance between

CWD and normal children seem to increase with grade, as found in the present study, where

the CWD performed more poorly than normal children on higher grades compared to lower

grades.

Similar to word repetition task, on non word repetition task, the performance found

to have improved significantly in the higher grades (III, IV and V) only. Literature strongly

suggests that those children developing language normally who score higher on receptive

vocabulary measures repeat nonwords more accurately than do children with lower
receptive vocabulary scores (Gathercole & Baddeley 1989,Metsala 1999 ). It has been found

to be indicative of phonological working memory capacity (Coady & Evans, 2008) which

means developmentally older children have a better capacity of phonological working

memory which in turn help them repeat or read non words appropriately compared to

younger children who are still building up their phonological repertoire and/or knowledge.

In turn, older children can figure out reading even new and unfamiliar words using their

phonological working memory capacity. Coady & Evans (2008) results on children ability to

repeat non words (which will indicate vocabulary strength) revealed that children scored

significantly better in NWrep tasks only in lower grades suggesting the strong vocabulary

construction only during early grades .The findings of the present study are in contradiction

with Coady & Evans (2008) , since the children of present study showed significant

improvement in scores of NWrep only in higher grades(grade IV and V).It can be concluded

from the present study that children continue to construct their vocabulary even in higher

grades. The CWD of the present study performed poorly compared to normal children on

NWrep task suggesting poor phonological working memory capacity, lexical (the degree of

accuracy correspondence to lexical intactness)and phonological knowledge. Snowling and

colleagues found that children with dyslexia experienced a greater degree of difficulty with

non-words repetition, especially at longer lengths. They concluded that children with

dyslexia have difficulty with phonological analysis and articulatory assembly processes.

(Snowling 1981, Snowling et al., 1986) then used NWrep task to examine lexical and

phonological processing in a group of CWD. They hypothesized that speech-motor programs

would be used for familiar words, while such programs would be unavailable for non-words.

Successful repetition of non-words would require ‘subjects to process the auditory stimulus,

to decode the sound segments, and to recode these as instructions in the form of a speech-

motor program (Snowling 1981). This decoding and recoding deficits reported by Snowling

could be given as a reason for poor performance of CWD on NWrep task.


5.1.6. Rapid naming

On rapid naming task, children in the present study showed increasing performance

from lower to higher grades suggesting a developmental pattern. The increasing

performance was not significant in lower grades (grade I, II and III) and found to be

significantly increasing in higher grades (grade IV and V). This could be because the time

taken to access a lexicon from their repertoire is faster with the development of children.

Study by Wagner, Torgesen, Roshotte, Hecht, Baker and Burgess (1997) investigated the

relative contribution of RN to later reading ability in three developmental periods from

kindergarten to grade II, from grade I to grade III and from grade II to grade IV. They

concluded that RN was a significant variation for word reading till kindergarten to grade III

only. Findings of present study contradict Wagner et al., (1997) study as in the present study

RN was found to be improving significantly in higher grades (grade IV and V) only. In other

words RN was also found to be influencing reading in children of higher grades.

The results of the present study also revealed that RN is a significant predictor of

reading skill with the significance value β =-.469; p<0.05. Manis, Seidenberg and Doi (1999).

Murphy and Pollatsek, (1994) Pennington, Cardoso-Martins, Green and Lefly (2001) and

Torgesen, Wagner, Rashotte, Burgess and Hecht (1997) suggested after their longitudinal

studies that alliteration and RN are differentially related to reading ability. Specifically it has

been suggested that although alliteration is the most Important for the development of the

ability to learn to read by phonological recoding, rapid naming skills may be especially

important for learning about the orthography of word- a skill necessary for fluent, skilled
reading. Indeed, RN has been consistently found to be related to fluency of text reading

even in the present study.

On RN task, the performance of CWD was significantly poorer than normal children

suggesting poor phonological accessing speed in CWD. The CWD in present study performed

with same accuracy as normal children do, however they had consumed longer duration to

name all the items in the stimulus. Our findings are in consonance with study by study by

Bowers and Wolf (1993) which admitted that deficit in rapid naming are most strongly

associated with deficits in the development of orthographic representations for words. It

can be hypothesized that children with slow naming speed activate the visual and

phonological codes for printed letters too slowly to allow efficient encoding of the specific

letter combinations in words. The study also supports Snyder and Downey (1995) who

compared the performance of children with dyslexia and normal reading achievement on

tasks of serial rapid naming. Their results revealed that the CWD had significantly longer

reaction times and production durations than their normal peers despite similar levels of

accuracy.

5.1.7. Alliteration and rhyming

The tasks of alliteration and rhyming are discussed simultaneously since these skills

constitute evidence for children’s awareness of the sound system (phonological awareness

skills) that is manifested in their spontaneous play with language. On rhyming task, the

results revealed that the performance improved significantly from grade III till V. However,

in the lower grades, grade I performed better than grade II, which could be attributed to

existence of variability in data as suggested by large standard deviation (see Table 2). On the
alliteration task the significant improvement in performance was noticed only for higher

grades (grade III, IV and V). Van Kleeck and Bryant (1984) found that some children began

indicating conscious awareness of the rhyming process as young as 2 yrs 8 months.

MacLean, Bryant and Bradley (1987) in an experimental task asked children to identify the

non rhyming word in a set of three words. They found that three year old subjects could

detect rhyming and alliteration at a conscious level. They also found a high correlation

between knowledge of nursery rhymes and success on phonological awareness tasks and

that both these abilities were related to early reading skills at the age of 4 years 6 months.

Wagner et al., (1997) concludes after his study that phoneme awareness tasks are significant

contributors to reading in higher grades only(grade III and IV).The present study also showed

that rhyming and alliteration tasks improved significantly only in higher grades (grade III, IV

and V), thus in consonance with Wagner et al (1997). The results of the present study also

found alliteration as a significant predictor of reading with significance value of β =1.016;

p<0.05. Findings of the present study is in consonance with the results of investigators like

Adams (1990); Bryant, MacLean, Bradley & Crossland (1990), Lundberg, Frost , Petersen &

Yopp (1992), Cunningham (1991); Whitehurst & Lonigan (2001), Lonigan, Burgess & Anthony

(2000). Wagner et al., (1997) experimented the amount of information that a measure of

phonological awareness could add to the prediction of reading once a measure of current

word reading and vocabulary was considered. The results revealed that the phonological

awareness is less efficient in prediction of reading as the child gets older.

Further, results of the present study showed that on rhyming and alliteration task,

CWD performed poorly than the normal children and performance are varying across

grades. This poor performance (slow development of phonemic awareness) can also be

correlated with their poor development in word reading skills. The present findings on CWD

are in consonance with study by Scarborough (1990) who concluded that at 5-year-olds who
exhibited weaknesses in object-naming, phonemic awareness, and letter-sound knowledge

turned poor readers in their early reading days suggesting poor phonemic awareness in

CWD. Studies by Moats and Foorman (1997) and Adams (1990) also suggests that children

who learn to read alphabetical language system such as English proven to be having poor

phonemic awareness tasks like rhyming and letter identification. The deficits noticed in

phonological awareness has been explained using the phonological theory (Snowling, 2000)

which is the most influential account for reading problems relates dyslexia to a deficit in

phonological awareness. Lyon, Shaywitz and Shaywitz, (2003) provided evidence for linkage

between lack of phonological awareness and reading disabilities.

5.1.8. Sound discrimination (SDis)

On the task of SDis the performance on normal children were improving from lower

through higher grades. It was not found to be significant in lower and higher ends whereas

only grade III found to be significantly better than grade II. In other words there was no

improvement in sound discrimination skill as the child grows older. The insignificant

improvement in scores of auditory discrimination findings are in consonance with study by

Neff (2006) who concluded that both spectral and temporal cues attain adult like by the age

of six. It was evident from the present study that SDis was a significant predictor of reading

skill with the significance value of. β =-1.54; p<0.05.It is in consonance with the study done

by Talcott et al., (2000) who concluded that auditory sensitivity as a predictor of later

reading skill.

In the present study the CWD showed poor performance compared to normal

children, though it was not statistically significant. Tallal (1980) proposed that children with

specific reading difficulties are deficient in processing brief and rapidly changing acoustic

information like brief acoustic events in stop consonants. (Werker & Tees, 1987) have
contributed to the evidence that dyslexia is associated with auditory perceptual problems.

Smythe (1999) Perceptual difficulties, as described above, could adversely interfere with

building up and stabilizing phonological representations as the boundaries of two

consonants (as these seem particularly affected) may become blurred or distorted in

some children with dyslexia. Such noisy phonological representations may well explain

the delay that dyslexic individuals have with learning new words, as they may need more

tokens of the same word to establish a phonological representation of a word. Furthermore,

‘fuzzy’ phonological representations may also lead to in accurate segmentation and

manipulation of sounds within a syllable, which, in turn, is a very important factor when

learning to relate graphemes to phonemes. The insignificant poor performance of CWD

compared to normal is supported by Ramus (2001) who concluded that subconscious

processing is intact in dyslexics and it is only the higher (lexical) process that is affected.

Tallal, Merzenich, Miller and Jenkins (1998) reported that normal temporal sequencing is

essential to develop neural representation of phonemes, which must be distinguished from

the stream of speech and combined to form words .We can hypothesize from Tallal et al.,

(1998), the delay in temporal integration caused a cascade effects, starting with the normal

development of efficient phonological abilities, and in turn resulting in subsequent failure to

learn to speak and read normally in CWD.

To summarize, a developmental trend has been observed in normal children on all the

tasks from grade I through grade V, except on the tasks of SDis and alphabet writing. Normal

children in the lower grades were found to perform better than the children in the higher

grades which can be been explained using the developmental stage models of reading. Children

in the lower grades perform poorly on tasks of reading as they may not have developed all the

component processes that are essential for reading familiar as well as unfamiliar words.

Further, children with dyslexia (CWD) even in the higher grades have performed significantly
poorer than younger normal children on various tasks indicating that there could be a delay or

deficit in the acquisition of component processes for reading in these children.

5.4 Subtypes of dyslexia based on the profile

The major aim of the present study was to subtype children with dyslexia based on

the individual profiles obtained after administering the IDT (Smythe, 2000) and cluster

analysis done on the data obtained. It was evident from cluster analysis in the present study

that dyslexia is not a homogenous group but a heterogeneous group with existence of three

major subtypes of dyslexia including phonological, surface and mixed types. Children with

dyslexia (CWD) who fell under the phonological subtype in the study included {1, 4, 14, 16,

10}.The cluster that fell under the surface subtype of dyslexia included {13}. The cluster

under the mixed subtype included CWD {2, 3, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 11, 12, 15} (See Table 5). The

present thus, supports the existence of heterogeneity in developmental dyslexia as

suggested by various investigators earlier and recently by Zeigler et al., (2007). They opined

that there exists no homogenous group of developmental dyslexia. They also derived and

sub grouped their children with dyslexia under 3 broad classes, namely phonological, surface

and mixed types of dyslexia. Present study also supports Castles and Coltheart (1993) who

tested a sample of 53 poor readers on their ability to read aloud sets of irregular words and

nonwords. Eight subjects were identified as pure developmental phonological dyslexics

whose nonword reading was poor, compared with chronological age- matched controls, but

their exception word reading was within normal range. Another 10 subjects were classified

as pure developmental surface dyslexics whose exception word reading was poor but their

nonword reading fell within normal range. Further they found that 27 subjects performed

poorly on both tasks and were therefore not classified as “pure” cases and were considered
as “mixed” cases. These children showed a significant discrepancy between their scores on

the exception word and nonword tasks. This 2 way classification has been documentad by

several other authors in their study. Manis et al., (1996) in their study reported of two

subgroups that were acknowledged to be forming their data, they were phonological and

surface dyslexia. Stanovich, Siegel & Gottardo (1997) identified 17 phonological and 15

surface dyslexics from their sample of 68 reading disabled 3rd grade children by comparing

them to chronological age controls on exception word and pseudo word reading. However,

when the dyslexic subtypes were defined by referring to Reading level controls, 17

phonological and only one surface dyslexic were identified. When the chronological age

defined subtypes were compared to reading level controls, the phonological dyslexics

displayed superior exception word reading but displayed deficits in pseudo word naming,

phonological sensitivity, working memory, and syntactic processing. The surface dyslexics in

contrast displayed a cognitive profile remarkably similar to that of the reading level controls.

The present study is in consonance with Ziegler et al., (2007) and Castles et al.,

(1993) who identified three broad subtypes of dyslexia.ie phonological, surface and mixed

varieties. The current classification has also been supported by Bakker (1979, 1990) who

classified dyslexics into P-types (relying on perceptual, analytical strategies for reading,

which turns out to be slow, fragmented and hesitating), L-types (relying on linguistic,

anticipatory strategies for reading, which allow for quicker reading but produce many,

usually plausible and context-based errors) and M-types (mixed types, showing both slow,

fragmented reading and many errors).In the present study, the CWD who had difficulties on

NWrep, NWreading, alliteration and rhyming were classified as phonological subtype and

CWD who performed well on phonological tasks but had difficulties on task of word reading

were classified as surface subtype. The CWD who had difficulty in both phonological
(NWreading, NWrep, alliteration, rhyming) and non phonological (Wreading, Wrep, RN and

spelling) tasks were classified under mixed dyslexics.

5.2.1. The phonological subgroup

CWD of phonological subtype could have performed poorly on phonological related

tasks in comparison to the others due to deficit in the sub lexical processing of phonemes

(Castles & Coltheart, 1993). In this group it was found that CWD performed poor mostly on

nonword reading tasks. By theory, to read a non word, it is crucial to have phoneme

grapheme conversion system intact. However, children with phonological dyslexia are

unable to read non words as it is hypothesized that the sub lexical route which is composed

of grapheme to phoneme conversion system is grossly affected (see Figure 1). Elbro (1996),

Hulme and Snowling (1992) and Snowling (2000) propose a hypothesis which says that the

representations of speech sounds (phonological representations) are coarsely coded and

under-specified in phonological dyslexics. A more basic auditory processing deficit has also

been considered as a possible underlying cause in phonological dyslexics (Farmer & Klein,

1995; Tallal, 1980). Yet this theory does not seem to be able to account for most dyslexics’

phonological deficit (Ramus, 2003).

Apart from poor performance on non-word reading tasks, children with phonological

dyslexia have been found to perform poorly on tasks of manipulating phonemes in order to

facilitate learning to spell and further facilitate better reading Bradley, & Bryant, 1978;

Griffiths, & Snowling, 2002; Morris , Stuebing, Fletcher, Shaywitz, Lyon, Shankweiler, et al.

(1998)). The main features of phonological deficit can be highlighted by three main types of

tasks (Snowling, 2000). Firstly, dyslexics perform poorly on tasks which require phonological
awareness, for instance paying attention to and manipulating individual speech sounds.

CWD with phonological deficit in the present study also showed poor performances on

rhyming and alliteration tasks which are considered tasks of phonological awareness

(Smythe, 2000). Secondly, they have difficulty when required to name series of objects

(rapid automatized naming) rapidly as seen in the present study. Thirdly, their verbal short-

term memory is reported to be deficient compared to controls which is manifested by a

lower memory span and poor nonword repetition, and impacts negatively on list learning,

story recall, paired-associate learning, and the more complex phonological awareness tasks

such as spoonerisms (Blomert & Mitterer, 2004; Tijms, 2004; Vellutino, Harding, Phillips, &

Steger, 1975).

5.2.2. The surface subgroup

The reason for the performance of the surface dyslexic profile could be due to a

nonphonological deficit in the lexical route as suggested also by Castles and Coltheart,

(1993). In surface dyslexics the disconnection is in between the semantics and phonological

functions leaving only the sub lexical route in operation (see fig.1). So, the reading of non

words is relatively intact in this population. Most studies in literature have found that

surface dyslexics had weaker phonological deficits than phonological dyslexics (Manis,

McBride-Chang, Seidenberg, Keating, Doi & Munson, 1997); Stanovich, Siegel, & Gottardo,

1997).The present study demonstrates similar results, whereas Zeigler et al., (2007)

suggested that phonological deficits were as strong in surface dyslexics as in phonological

dyslexics. In the present study, apart from good performance of surface dyslexics on

phonological related tasks, it was noteworthy that these children also performed better on

rapid naming tasks. A contradicting study to our findings was that of study by Zeigler et al

(2007) who indicated clearly a different picture of surface dyslexia than the one commonly
suggested in the literature. Although their surface dyslexics showed small impairments in

orthographic access (letter search deficit in words, which resulted in reduced word

superiority effects compared to phonological dyslexics), the main deficits were phonological

in nature (picture naming and phoneme matching).

5.2.3. The mixed subgroup

The performance of mixed group in the present study could be due to the deficits in

both the routes of reading. i.e., sublexical and lexical route as suggested by Edwards &

Hogben, 1999) It can be hypothesized that the combined deficits in orthographic lexicon and

in Grapheme Phoneme (GPC) rules could have resulted in mixed dyslexia (see Figure.1). In

other words they have difficulty in reading stimuli that requires both lexical and sub lexical

processing (Edwards & Hogben, 1999). Various hypothetical reasons have been purported

for the occurrence of mixed dyslexia in CWD. Castles and Coltheart (1993) reported of three

reasons for occurrence of mixed dyslexia in CWD population. The first reason relates to the

structure of the model itself- the computational Dual Route Cascaded (DRC) model of

Coltheart and colleagues (Coltheart, Curtis, Atkins, & Haller, 1993; Coltheart, Rastle, Perry,
Langdon, & Ziegler, 2001). The lexical and nonlexical routes of the DRC are not completely

independent, but have three components in common: visual feature detectors, abstract

letter units, and the phoneme output system (see Figure-1) Therefore, a deficit in any one of

these components can lead to impairments in both lexical (surface) and sub lexical

(phonological) pathways. The second reason why deficits in lexical and sub lexical system

might co-occur relates not to the structure of the dual-route model itself but to the other

distal factors that may have influenced the development of the components of the model in

the first place. Even though the processes themselves may be distinct, there are numerous

factors—personality, cognitive, social, and educational—that are likely to influence the

speed and efficiency of children’s acquisition of both the lexical and the nonlexical routes,

and to an approximately equal degree. The third reason why deficits in lexical and sub lexical

system might co-occur is that, although the orthographic lexicon and the GPC procedure in

the DRC model operate independently, this does not mean that they are learned

independently.

To summarize, heterogeneity in the dyslexia data was confirmed in the present

study. Three major subtypes were identified based on cluster analysis and profiling and this

could be explained using dual route model. The phonological subtype has been hypothesized

to be occurring due to poor sublexical processing; the surface subtype has been

hypothesized to be occurring due to poor lexical processing. The most prevalent mixed type

has been hypothesized to be occurring due to deficit in both lexical and sublexical route of

the dual route processing for reading.


Summary and Conclusion

Literacy is defined as the minimal ability to read and write in a designated language.

The components of literacy include reading, writing, listening, phonological awareness,

phonological decoding, reading comprehension, spelling, orthographic knowledge and rapid

automatized naming. Phonological awareness among others has been shown to be a primary

factor underlying early reading development. Learning to read, for many children seems not

to require much effort. They appear to attain this ability almost incidentally. However, it

seems to be correct in the languages where there is only one sound for each letter, where

the assigning phoneme to a grapheme step is relatively easy. But in languages like English

the association of various different letters sounds with a particular letter and various

different letters with a particular sound makes mastery of the sound symbol relationship

more difficult. This irregularity in the language itself makes it even more difficult in those

children who are poor in reading and writing skills.

Dyslexia can be described as the person who has difficulty in reading even though

sufficient teaching has been offered. As there are number of causes of dyslexia, there are

number different types of dyslexia making dyslexia a heterogeneous group .Though there

are several different types of dyslexia practically can exist for the convenience of

classification different subtypes have been proposed in the literature. One of the major

classifications is proposed by Castles and Colthart (1993) based on dual route model. The

subtype classification proposed by these authors includes phonological (CWD finding

difficulty in phonological tasks), surface (CWD finding difficulty in word reading) and mixed

(CWD finding difficulties both in phonological and non phonological tasks) type. Sixty normal

children, 12 in each grade from grade I to V was taken to form a normal group. The children
with dyslexia (CWD) consisted of 16 children, with 3 children from each grade except grade

IV which had 4 children in it.

Descriptive statistical procedures were used in order to compare the performance of

normal children for all the grades across different grades. The mean and standard deviation

table showed that tasks like SC, HQ, Spelling and Wreading showed clear developmental

trend from lower to higher grade. On tasks like NWreading, Rhyming, Wrep, NWrep ,

alliteration, RN and SDis lower grades didn’t reveal any increasing trend where as higher

grades revealed developmental trend. Normal children in the lower grades were found to

perform better than the children in the higher grades which can be been explained using the

developmental stage models of reading. Children in the lower grades perform poorly on tasks

of reading as they may not have developed all the component processes that are essential for

reading familiar as well as unfamiliar words. Further, children with dyslexia (CWD) even in the

higher grades have performed significantly poorer than younger normal children on various

tasks indicating that there could be a delay or deficit in the acquisition of component processes

for reading in these children.

Results are also discussed for the comparison of normal group against CWD group

which showed that CWD performed poorer than normal children on all the tasks. A

regression analysis was carried out to obtain the predictors for reading. Results indicated

that that tasks like rapid naming (RN), alliteration, sound discrimination (SDis) and spelling

were found to be the predictors for reading. Further the data was analyzed qualitatively, and

the major findings were explained. This included mirror images in alphabet task, difficulty in

understanding the instruction on tasks of alliteration and rhyming, poor performance on

alliteration and rhyming, GPC usage in reading words and non words, inappropriate working
memory leading to poor performance in repetition (word and non word) task and the

tendency to skip a whole series while naming given series of items on RN task. The results of

the sub grouping revealed that 7 were phonological dyslexics, 1 was surface dyslexics and 9

were mixed group. Thus, heterogeneity in developmental dyslexia was confirmed in the

present study. The phonological subtype has been hypothesized to be occurring due to poor

sublexical processing; the surface subtype has been hypothesized to be occurring due to

poor lexical processing. The most prevalent mixed type has been hypothesized to be

occurring due to deficit in both lexical and sublexical route of the dual route processing for

reading (Newcombe & Marshall, 1984; Castles & Coltheart, (1993).

Implications of the study

 The result of the present study reveal that DAPIC can be used as a tool to profile

those children who show difficulties in phonological and non phonological tasks

of literacy. The tool needs to be further administered on a larger sample to

generalize the findings and use regularly for the assessment of children with

reading problems.

 The results of the present study reveal that differentiating among dyslexia

subtypes with specific impairments allows a more fine grained understanding of

disorder than simply comparing dyslexics with normal. The profiling and sub

typing of the present study also lead us to understand and plan for

individualized education program (IEP) for children with developmental dyslexia.

Various treatment programs has been suggested in the literature namely relay

strategy by Hatfield (1983) and decompositional (sublexical) strategy by


Tuchman (2000) for treating phonological dyslexia. To be effective, the review

sited , showed that programs must focus on phonics , early diagnosis with

remedial intervention and followed by teaching programs tailored for individual

needs(MOE literature review,2007).Such treatment program for other type of

dyslexia can be developed and they are identified.

 The present study also highlights on predictors of reading ability, which will

foster the versatility of the present profiling tool to be used as a screening tool.

Which supposedly include that are significant predictors of the reading skill like

RN, SDis, alliteration and spelling

Limitations of the study

 This study included a small sample of children for each grade. Administering it on

larger sample would help in standardization of the tool for assessing children with

dyslexia.

 An additional, large scale study might also look at the prevalence of domain general

and domain-specific impairments in dyslexia. In the current study, we were

concerned with overall group differences, but given a larger sample, it would be

interesting to classify the dyslexic group further.

 Other domain specific tasks of phoneme awareness skills like phoneme stripping,

word attack tasks could have been included in the test to give a complete profile of

individual’s phonemic awareness skills.


 Other factors like socio- economic status, educational background of parents were

not explored in the study.


References

Aaron, P.G., Joshi, M., & Williams, K.A. (1999). Not all reading disabilities are
alike. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 2, 120–137.

Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print.


Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Bakker, D. J. (1979). Hemispheric differences and reading strategies: Two


dyslexias? Bulletin of the Orton Society, 29, 84–100.

Bakker, D. J. (1990). Neuropsychological treatment of dyslexia. New York:


Oxford University Press.

Bear, D.R., & Templeton, S. (1998).Explorations in developmental spelling:


Foundations for learning and teaching phonics, spelling, & vocabulary.
The reading teacher, 52, 222-242.

Beers, P. (2007). Stages of reading development. Retrieved January 25 2009 from


http://EzineArticles.com/?expert=Pamela_Beers.

Bell, S. M., McCallum, R. S., & Cox, E .A. (2003). Toward a research based
assessment of Dyslexia: Using cognitive, measures to identify reading
disabilities .Journal of reading disabilities, 36, 505-516

Bishop, D. (1997). Cognitive neuropsychology and developmental disorders:


Uncomfortable bedfellows. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50,
899–923.

Blomert, L., & Mitterer, H. (2004). The fragile nature of the speech-perception deficit in
dyslexia: Natural Vs synthetic speech. Brain and Language, 89, 1, 21–26.
Boder, E. (1973). Developmental dyslexia: A diagnostic approach based on three atypical
reading patterns. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 15, 663–687.

Bowers, P.G., & Wolf, M. (1993).Theoretical links among naming speed , precise timing
mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 5, 69-85.

Brown,V., Hammil, D., & Weiderholt, J.L.(1995).Test of Reading Comprehension-3 .Austin


,TX:Pro-Ed.

Bryant, P.E., MacLean, L, M., Bradley, & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration,
phoneme detection, and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429-
38.

Bub, D., Cancelliere, A., & Kertesz, A. (1985), Whole-word and analytic translation of
spelling to sound in a non-semantic reader, in Surface dyslexia:
neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (eds. K.E.
Patterson, J.C. Marshall and M. Coltheart), Erlbaum, London.

Carroll, J. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Language and phonological skills in children at
high-risk of reading difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45,
631–640.

Carrow-Woolfolk,E.(1995). Oral and Written language scales (OWLS). Circle Pines, MN:
American Guidance service.

Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47,
149–180.
Catts, H. W., & Kamhi, A. G. (1999). Language and reading disabilities. Needham Heights,
MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Clarke, L.K. (1988). Invented versus traditional spelling in first graders’ writings: Effects
on learning to spell and read. Research in the Teaching of English 22: 281-309.

Clay, M. M. (1984) (Ed). Reading: The patterning of complex behavior (2nd


Edn.).Auckland, New Zealand: Heinemann Educational books.

Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks.
Dyslexia, 7, 197–216.

Coltheart, M., Bates, A., & Castles, A. (1994). Cognitive neuropsychology and
rehabilitation. In G.W. Humphreys & M.J. Riddoch (Eds.), Cognitive
neuropsychology and cognitive rehabilitation. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates Ltd.

Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual
route and parallel distributed processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100,
589-608.

Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. C. (2001). DRC: A Dual Route
Cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological
Review, 108, 204–256.

Cunningham, A. (1991). Explicit verses implicit instruction in phonemic awareness.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 429-44.
Cunningham, P. (1995). The Four-Blocks Literacy Model: How and why it really works
[Videotape]. (Available from Carson-Dellosa Publishing Co., PO Box 35665, 4321
Piedmont Parkway, Greensboro, NC 27425-5665).

Denkla, M. B. (1985). Revised neurological examination for subtle signs.


Psychoparmacology Bulletin, 21, 4, 773-800.

Dyson, A.H. (2001). Writing and children’s symbolic repertoires: Development unhinged.
In Handbook of Early Literacy Research, Eds. S.B. Neuman & D.K. Dickinson. New
York: The Guilford Press.

Edwards, V., & Hogben, J. (1999). New norms for comparing children’s lexical and
nonlexical reading: A further look at subtyping dyslexia. Australian Journal of
Psychology, 5, 37–49.

Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S.R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B.V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z. & Shanahan, T.
(2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence
from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly,
36, 250-287.

Elbro, C. (1996). Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a
hypothesis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 453–485.

Farhady,H., & Keramati,M.(1996).A text-driven method for the deletion procedure in


cloze passages. Language testing,13,190-207

Farmer, M.E, Klein RM (1995) .The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to
dyslexia: a review. Psychonom Bull Rev 2: 460-493.
Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Barnes, M. A. (2006). Learning disabilities. New
York, NY: Guilford. A.91, 8010–8013.

Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. E. Patterson, J. C.


Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and
cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301-330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Galaburda, A. M. (1991).Anatomy of dyslexia:Argument against phrenology .In D.D


Duane&D.B. Gray ,(Eds.), The reading brain: The biological basis of
dyslexia(pp.119-131).Parkton,MD:York press.

Gallagher, A., Frith U., Snowling M.(2000). Precursors of literacy delay among children at
genetic risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 202–213.

Gayán J. and Olson R.K. (2003). Genetic and Environmental Influences on


Individual Differences in Printed Word Recognition. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 2, 97-123.

Gentry,R.(1982). An analysis of developmental spelling in GNYS AT WRK.The reading


Teacher, 36, 192-200.

Gessel,A., & Amatruda, C.S(1947).Developmental diagnosis, 2nd Ed. New York;Paul


B.Hoeber

Griffiths, Y. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2002). Predictors of exception word and


nonword reading in dyslexic children: The severity hypothesis. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 94, 34–43.
Hammil,D.D. Leigh,J.E ., McNutt,G., & Larsen,S.C (1987).A new definition of
learning disabilities. Journal of learning disabilities, 20, 109-112

Hammill,D, & Larsen,S. (1996).Test of Written Language -3,Austin,Tx:Pro-Ed.

Hanley, J.R., Hastie, K., & Kay, J. (1992). Developmental surface dyslexia and dysgraphia:
An orthographic processing impairment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 44a, 2, 285-319.

Hari, R., & Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in
dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 525–532.

Harris, T. L., & Hodges. R. E. (1995).The literacy dictionary: The vocabulary of reading
and writing. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. 318 pages.
0872071383. Interest level: general.

Hatfield, F.M. (1983). Aspects of acquired dysgraphi a and implications for re-education . In
C. Code & D. Muller (Eds.), Aphasia therapy. London: Edward Arnold.

Ho, C.S.H., Chan, D.W.O., Tsang ,S.M., Lee ,S.H. (2000). The Hong Kong Test of Specific
Learning Difficulties in Reading and Writing. Hong Kong: Hong Kong Specific
Learning Difficulties Research Team.

Hoff.E.,Core.C.,Briges.K. (2008). Non-Word Repetition Assesses Phonological Memory


and Is Related to Vocabulary Development in 20- to 24-Month-Olds. Journal of
Child Language, 35, 4, 903-916.

Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (1992). Deficits in output phonology: An explanation of


reading failure? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 47–72.
in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI).

Karlsen, B. and Gardner, E.(1996). Stanford Diagnostic Reading Test-4. San Antonio,
TX;harcoutrt-brace Educational Measurement.

Larsen,S. & Hammill, D. (1994). Test of written Spelling-3 ,Austin,Tx:Pro-Ed.

Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S.R., & Anthony, J.L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy
and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent variable
longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596-613.

Loosemore, R.P.W., Brown, G.D.A. & Watson, F.L. (1991). A neural network model of normal
and dyslexic spelling. Proceedings of the International Joint Conference on Neural
Networks, 231-236. Seattle: IEEE Press.

Lubs,H., Duara,R., Levin,B., Jallad,B., Lubs,M.L., Rabin,M., Kushch,A., &Gross –Glenn, K.


(1991)Dyslexia types:Genetics,Behavior and brain imaging .In D.D.Duane &
D.b.Gray(Eds.),The reading brain: The biological basis of dyslexia(pp.89-
118).Parkton,MD:York press.

Lundberg, I., Frost ,J.,& Petersen ,O.P.(1988). Effects of an extensive program for
stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research
Quarterly 23: 263-284.

Lyon, G. R, Shaywitz, S. E, & Shaywitz, B.A(2003).Definition of dyslexia. Annals of


Dyslexia, 53, 1-14.
Lyytinen, H., Aro, M., & Holopainen, L. (2004). Dyslexia in highly orthographically regular
Finnish. In I. Smythe, J. Everatt & R. Salter (Eds). The International Handbook of
Dyslexia. (pp.81-91).West Sessex, UK: Wiley.

Maclean, M., Bryant, P.E. & Bradley. (1987) .Rhymes, Nursery rhymes and reading in
early childhood. Merrill-Palmer quarterly, 3, 3, 255-81.

Manis, F. R., McBride-Chang, C., Seidenberg, M. S., Keating, P., Doi, L. M.,
Munson, B., et al. (1997).Are speech perception deficits associated with
developmental dyslexia? .Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66,
211–235.

Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M., McBride-Chang, C., & Petersen, A.
(1996). On the bases of two subtypes of development dyslexia. Cognition,
58, 157–195.

Mann, V. (1986). Phonological awareness: The role of reading experience .Cognition, 24, 65–
92.

Marsh, G., Friedman, M., Welch, V. & Desberg, P. (1981). A cognitive-developmental


theory of reading acquisition. In G.E. MacKinnon & T.G. Waller (Eds.), Reading,
research: Advances in theory and practice. 3, 199–221. New York: Academic
Press.

McCarthy, R., & Warrington, K. K. (1986).Visual associative agnosia: A clinic-anatomical


study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 49, 1123-1240.

McGhee,R., Bryant,B., Larsen,S. & Rivera,D. (1995).Test of Written Expression


.Austin,TX:Pro-Ed.
Moats, L. C. & Foorman, B. R. (1997). “Introduction to the special issue of SSR:
Components of effective reading instruction.” Scientific Studies of Reading 1 (3),
p. 187-189.

Morris, R. D., Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Lyon, G. R., Shankweiler, D. P., et
al. (1998). Subtypes of reading disability: Variability around a phonological core.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 347–373.

Morton,J. & Frith.U.(1993).What lesson for dyslexia from Down’s syndrome? Comments on
Cossu, Rossini, and Marshall. Cognition, 48, 2889-296.

Nagy, W., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of
morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second grade
readers and at-risk fourth grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology,
95(4), 730-742.

Newcombe, F & Marshall,J.C. (1984).Varieties of acquired dyslexia:A linguistic approach.


Seminars in Neurology 4:2:181-95.

Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: The cerebellar
deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neuroscience, 24, 508–511.

Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., Berry, E. L., Jenkins, I. H., Dean, P., & Brooks, D. J. (1999).
Association of abnormal cerebellar activation with motor learning difficulties in
dyslexic adults. Lancet, 353, 1662–1667.

Olson, R. K. (2002). Dyslexia: Nature and nurture. Dyslexia, 8, 143-159.


Parker,R., Hasbrouck,J.,& Tindal,G. (1992).The maze as a class room –based reading
measure:Construction method, reliability and validity. Journal of special
education, 26,195-218.

Pennington, B.F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P.A. & Lefly, D.L. (2001).Comparing the
phonological and double deficit hypotheses for developmental dyslexia. Reading and
Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 705-755.

Pennington,B.F.(1989).Using genetics to understand dyslexia. Annals of dyslexia, 39, 81-


93

Prema, K.S. (1997). Reading acquisition profile in Kannada. Ph. D. dissertation, University
of Mysore, Mysore.

Ramaa,S.(1985). Diagnosis and remediation of dyslexia-An attempt. Ph.D


dissertation, University of Mysore,Mysore.

Ramus, F. (2004). Neurobiology of dyslexia: A reinterpretation of the data. Trends in


Neuroscience, 27, 720–726.

Ramus, F. (2001). Outstanding questions about phonological processing in dyslexia.


International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 1-40.

Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general


sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(2), 212–218.

Read, C. (1971).Pre-school children’s knowledge of English phonology. Harvard


Educational Review, 41, 1-33
Richgels, D. J. 2001. Invented spelling, phonemic awareness, and reading and writing
instruction. In Handbook of Early Literacy Research, eds. S.B. Neuman & D.K.
Dickinson. New York: The Guilford Press.

Roberts, J. E & Medley, L. P. (1995). Ottitis media and speech language sequale in young
children: Current issue ion measurement .Americal Journal of Speech Language
Pathology, 4, 15-24.

Sarborough, H. S. (1990).Very early language deficits in dyslexic children .Child


development, 61, 1728-1743.

Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word


recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.

Seymour, P. (1987). Developmental dyslexia: A cognitive experimental analysis. In M.


Coltheart, G. Sartori, & R. Job (Eds.), The cognitive neuropsychology of language.
Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.

Seymour, P. (1990). Developmental dyslexia. In M.W. Eysenck (Ed.), Cognitive


psychology: An international review. Chichester, UK: John Wiley.

Seymour, P., & Bunce, F. (1994). Application of cognitive models to remediation in


developmental dyslexia. In G.W. Humphreys&M.J. Riddoch (Eds.), Cognitive
neuropsycholog y and cognitive rehabilitation. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates Ltd.

Seymour, P., & Elder, L. (1986). Beginning reading without phonology. Cognitive
Neuropsychology, 3, 1–37.
Seymour, P., & Evans, H. (1992). Beginning reading without semantics: A cognitive study
of hyperlexia. Cognitive Neuropsycholog y, 9, 89–122.

Singhi, P., Kumar, M., Malhi, P., & Kumar, R. (2007). Utility of the WHO Ten Questions Screen
for Disability Detection in a Rural Community—the North Indian Experience. Journal
of Tropical Pediatrics. 53, 6, 383-387.

Smythe, I., (2000). International dyslexia test. Retrieved January 21, 2008 from E-mail
communication.

Smythe, I., & Everatt J (2000). Dyslexia diagnosis in different languages. In Peer L and
Reid G, Multilingualism, Literacy and Dyslexia. David Fulton Publishers. London.

Snowling, M. J. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychological


Research, 43, 219–234.

Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd Ed.). Oxford, England: Blackwell.

Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous:
Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74,
358–373.

Snowling, M. J., Goulandris, N., Bowlby, M., & Howell, P. (1986). Segmentation and
speech perception in relation to reading skill: A developmental analysis. Journal
of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 489–507.

Snyder, L. S., & Downey, D. M. (1995). Serial rapid naming skills in children with reading
disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia. 45, 31– 49.
Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and new
frontiers. New York: Guilford Press.

Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S., & Gottardo, A. (1997). Converging evidence for
phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 89, 114–127.

Stein, J., & Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of
dyslexia. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 147–152.

Strommen,L.T., Mates .B.F.,(2004).Learning to love reading. Interviews with


older children and teens. Journal of Adolescent and adult literacy,48:3,1
18-200

Stuart, M. (1995). Prediction and qualitative assessment of five and six year old
children’s reading: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 65, 287–296.

Stuart, M., Masterson, J., & Dixon, M. (2000). Sponge like acquisition of sight vocabulary
in beginning readers? Journal of Research in Reading, 23, 12–27.

Talcott, J. B., Witton, C., McClean, M., Hansen, P. C., Rees, A., Green, G. G. R., & Stein, J.
F. (1999). Can sensitivity to auditory frequency modulation predict children's
phonological and reading skills? Neuroreport, 10(10), 2045-2050.

Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics and reading disabilities in Children.
Brain and Language. 9,182–98.
Tallal, P., Merzenich, M., Miller, S., & Jenkins, W. (1998) Language Learning Impairments:
Integrating Basic Science, Technology and Remediation, Experimental Brain
Research, 123, 210-219.

Tallal. P., Miller, S., & Fitch, H.(1993).Neurobiological basis of speech : A case for the
preeminence of temporal processing. In Tallal, A.M. Gulbarda, R.R.Llinas, &
C.Von Euler.(Eds.), Annals of the New York academy of Sciences,682,27-47.

Temple, C. (1984). Surface dyslexia in a child with epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 22, 569–576.

Temple, C. M., & Marshall, J. C. (1983). A case study of developmental phonological


dyslexia. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 517-533.

Terepocki, M., Kruk, R. S., & Willows, D. M. (2002). The incidence and nature of letter
orientation errors in reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 214-
233.

Tijms, J. (2004). Verbal memory and phonological deficit in dyslexia. Journal of Research
in Reading, 27(3), 300–310.

Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashtte, C. A., Burgess,S., & Hecht, S.(1997).Contributions
of phonologicalawareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of
word-reading skills in second- fifth grade children.Scientific Studies of
Reading,1(2),161-195.Mahwah,NJ:Erlbaum.
Valdois, S., Bosse, M. L., & Tainturier, M. J. (2004). The cognitive deficits responsible for
developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a selective visual attentional
disorder. Dyslexia, 10, 339–363

Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading
disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1, 2–40.

Vellutino, F. R., Harding, C. J., Phillips, F., & Steger, J. A. (1975). Differential transfer in
poor and normal readers. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 126 (1st Half), 3–18.

Viholainen, H., Ahonen, T., Cantell, M., Lyytinen, P., & Lyytinen, H. (2002). Development
of early motor skills and language in children at risk for familial dyslexia.
Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 44, 761-769.

Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A.,
Burgess, S. R., Donahue, J., & Garon, T. (1997). Changing relations
between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as
children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal
study. Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 468-479.

Werker, J.F., & Tees, R.C. (1984) .Phonemic and phonetic factors in adult cross –
language speech perception .Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
75, 1866-1878.

Whitehurst, G.J., & Lonigan, C.J. (2001). Emergent literacy: Development from prereaders
to readers. In Handbook of Early Literacy Research, eds. S.B. Neuman & D.K.
Dickinson. New York: The Guilford Press.

Woodcock, R. (1997a). Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised. Circle


Pines,MN:American Guidance service.
Zentall, S. S. (1993). Research on the educational implications of attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Exceptional Children, 60, 143-153.

Ziegler ,J.C., Castel. C., Pech-Georgel .C., George,F., Alario, X., & Perry, C. (2007).
Developmental dyslexia and the dual route model of reading: Simulating
individual differences and subtypes.Cognition, 1-28. Article in press.
Appendix A

Test Material

1. Alphabet test

1)b, 2)d, 3)p, 4)9, 5)q, 6)m, 7)w, 8)u, 9)n

2. Shape copying

3. Writing

A) Spelling (Linguistic component)

1) Lot A lot of people watch television. Write lot


2) Pig The farmer had a pig to sell. Write pig
3) Cug They couldn’t fine the cug anywhere. Write cug(bug)
4) This This cake is better than that cake. Write this
5) Be Be careful crossing the road. Write be
6) Fish Haddock is one kind of fish. Write fish
7) Tent Campers use a tent. Write tent
8) Yoll He did not want to yoll the boat. Write yoll.(doll)
9) Step In the dark, you must mind your step. Write step
10) Much She did not like chocolate very much. Write much
11) Boat The man wanted the boat to go fishing. Write boat
12) Crisp Biscuits should be chrisp. Write crisp
13) Brin There was a fine brin that morning. Write brin(pin)
14) Garden She grows flowers in her garden. Write garden
15) Butter Butter is made from milk. Write butter
16) Nothing A man who has lost everything has notyhing.Write nothing
17) Wooden A cook sometimes uses a wooden spoon. write wooden
18) Moid She put the paper on the moid.Write moid(void)
19) Swan A swan has a long neck. Write swan
20) Another After trying one, he asked another. Write another
21) Angry Angry people sometimes shout .Write angry
22) Cattle Cattle graze in the field .Write cattle
23) Rilt He could see the rilt clearly. Write rilt(hilt)
24) Promise A promise should be kept. Write promise
25) Country France is the country across the channel. Write country
26) Hospital A nurse works in a hospital. Write hospital
27) Driggle The draggle was covered in the green paint. Write draggle(wriggle)
28) Trousers In hot climates, men wear short trousers. Write trousers
29) Thread To make a necklace , you thread beads on a string. Write thread
30) Sword He soldier carried a sword. Write sword
31) Passage He walked down a long passage. Write passage
32) Plicant The boy was very plicant. Write plicant(ant)
33) Stronk He could not stronk easily. Write stronk(honk)
34) Quarter A fourth is quarter. Write quarter
35) Heart The heart pumps blood through our body. Write heart
36) Cladition There was not much cladition that day.Write cladition(tradition)
37) Biscuit She had a biscuit with her tea.Write biscuit
38) Parcel Parcel for the post should be wel wrapped.Write parcel.
39) Trabnag The trabnag did not move. Write trabnag
40) Adventure Most boys and girls like adventure stories. Write adventure.

B) Handwriting quality(Non linguistic component)

4. Reading test

1. tree
2. little
3. milk
4. egg
5. book.
6. school
7. sit
8. frog
9. playing
10. bun
11. flower
12. road
13. clock
14. train
15. light
16. picture
17. think
18. summer
19. people
20. something
21. dream
22. downstairs
23. biscuit
24. shepherd
25. thirsty
26. crowd
27. sandwich
28. beginning
29. postage
30. island
31. Saucer
32. angel
33. ceiling
34. appeared
35. knife
36. canary
37. attractive
38. imagine
39. nephew
40. gradually
41. smoulder
42. applaud
43. disposal
44. nourished
45. diseased
46. university
47. orchestra
48. knowledge
49. audience
50. situated
51. physics
52. campaign
53. choir
54. intercede
55. fascinate
56. Forfeit
57. siege
58. recent
59. plausible
60. prophecy
61. colonel
62. soloist
63. systematic
64. slovenly
65. classification
66. genuine
67. institution
68. pivot
69. conscience
70. heroic

5. Non word reading test

Practice items:

hab: lib: feg:

Test items:

One syllable Two syllable

1. Gat 6.higure

2. Rop 7.kibnick

3. Shug 8.Pachine

4. Hild 9.clabnag

5.Narge 10.tringdom

6. Alliteration test
Practice item 1. Practice item 2.

Sell big bang block bright plate

Fat hot felt stair trick swop

Test items 1 . Test items 2.

1.Gap got nod 6. train plane prone

2.Net dig nip 7. spade start break

3.Rope take time 8. crumb twist climb

4.Big pit ball 9 . pram trap plan

5.Form fat van 10. great glue crane

7. Rhyming test

Practice items:

Tack need hack

Rap lap nag

Fit till bill

Test items1. Test items 2 .

1. Main line pain 11. buckle puddle


muddle
2. Fog log bag 12. tight light ride
3. Fuss tilt wilt 13. niece cheese please
4. Neck peck beg 14. nip fib tip
5. Nap hip sap 15. tone home phone
6. Pen hen pet 16. cattle battle handle
7. Red big dig 17. should wood food
8. Pip top hop 18. neat weed seed
9. Hid did dub 19. ship rip stop
10. Pack buck rack 20 .tree need free

8. Word repetition

1. Pin cat
2. Cap sky tin
3. Mat crow pick
4. Rain pen chair men
5. Pit day log shirt
6. Cow wool snake hut grape
7. Plank lion heel plot den

9. Non word repetition

1. Ket
2. Lum
3. Mup hin
4. Ret spige
5. Trum frut nabe
6. Ronch tarp keld
7. Horp brid nate proog
8. Fode wike drup cren

10. Rapid naming


11. Sound discrimination

Prctice item:

Pig Dig-different
Bat Bat-Same

Lake Date-different

Test items:

1. Rip tip
2. Sick sack
3. Side side
4. Pet bet
5. Big bog
6. Sit sit
7. Bed bad
8. Dam mad
9. Slow snow
10. End and
11. Fish fish
12. Shelled shield
13. Halt hall
14. Try tie
15. Tilt tilt
16. Ship sheep
17. Raw war
18. Throw throw
19. Rip reap
20. Nib nip
Score Sheet

Name :

School:

Grade:

Age on test time and Gender:

Tested by:
Scores for screening tests

Screening Test Maximum score Individual’s score

A Ten-question disability screening 10


test

DST

Total screening score:

Individual’s score:
Test domain Maximum score Individual’s score

1.Alphabet 9

2.Shape copying 10

3.writing

a) spelling 40

b)handwriting quality 5

4.Reading test 70

5.Non word reading test 10

6.Alliteration 10

7.Rhyme 20
8.Word repetition 7

9.Non word repetition 8

10.Rapid naming Time taken

11.Sound discrimination 20

Total score:209 (excluding the


duration taken in rapid naming section)

Individual’s total score:

1.Alphabets: ( mention the letters that are written incorrectly)


2. Shape copying: (0= not attempting,7= most appropriate shape)

1.

2.

3.

4.

3.Writing

a)spelling: (1 for each correctly spelled word)

1. 11. 21. 31.

2. 12. 22. 32.

3. 13. 23. 33.

4. 14. 24. 34.


5. 15. 25. 35.

6. 16. 26. 36.

7. 17. 27. 37.

8. 18. 28. 38.

9. 19. 29. 39.

10. 20. 30. 40.

b)Handwriting quality:( 1=bad,5=very good)

4.Reading test: ( one score for correctly read word)


1) a. b. c. d. e.

2) a. b. c. d. e.

3) a. b. c. d. e.

4) a. b. c. d. e.

5)a. b. c. d. e.

6)a. b. c. d. e.

7)a. b. c. d. e.

8)a. b. c. d. e.

9)a. b. c. d. e.
10)a. b. c. d. e.

11)a. b. c. d. e.

12)a. b. c. d. e.

13)a. b. c. d. e.

14)a. b. c. d. e.

5.Non word reading test: ( score one for each correctly read non word)

1. 6.

2. 7.

3. 8.

4. 9.

5. 10.
6.Alliteration: (one score for each correct responses)

1. 6.

2. 7.

3. 8.

4. 9.

5. 10.

7.Rhyming test: (one score for each correct responses)

1. 11.

2. 12.

3. 13.

4. 14.

5. 15.

6. 16.

7. 17.
8. 18.

9. 19.

10. 20.

8.Word repetition: (one score for each correct responses)

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

6.

7.

9.Non word repetition:(score one for each correct responses)

1. 5.
2. 6.

3. 7.

4. 8.

10.Rapid naming: (note the number of items named at the end of one minute)

11.Sound discrimination:

1. 11.

2. 12.

3. 13.

4. 14.

5. 15.

6. 16.
7. 17.

8. 18.

9. 19.

10. 20.

Appendix B

Profile on Qualitative analysis of individual data

Subject 1. (Grade I)

Alphabet: Correctly wrote the alphabets b and m only

SC: Performed very poorly

Spelling: Unable to spell any of the words

HQ: Very poor, managed no score

Reading: Unable to read any of the word

NWreading: unable to read any of the non word

Alliteration: Managed no score

Rhyming: Managed no score

Wrep: Managed to repeat only the first series of stimulus which had only two words (e.g.
pin, cat)
NWrep: Managed to repeat the first two non word series only, they were made of one
nonword consisted of single syllable. The subject couldn’t repeat the third nonword since it
had two words.

RN: Took 78 seconds to name all the 35 items in correct order

SDis: Correctly discriminated only three stimuli.

Subject 2 (Grade 1)

Alphabet: Wrote all of the alphabets without error

SC: Copies the first three simple shapes exactly, but only fairly well the complex one.

Spelling: lot, be, fish and tent were spelled correctly out of all forty words.

HQ: Quality was bad.

Reading: Managed to correctly read only the first eight items

NWreading: Could read only two of the single syllable nonword.

Alliteration: Correctly identified only the alliterated words in second stimuli

Rhyming: Didn’t manage any correct responses

Wrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to four words in it.

NWrep: Could manage to repeat stimulus series which had up to two non words only.

RN: Took 62 seconds to correctly name all of the items.

SDis: side, reap, nip were the ones which were incorrect.

Subject 3(Grade 1)

Alphabet: unable to write d, p, and m

SC: Completed the simple ones with ease, but managed to copy the final shape only lesser
than fair.
Spelling: lot, be and much were written correctly

HQ: Performed fairly

Reading: First three items (tree, little and milk) then 6th (school) and eleven (flower) were
correctly read.

NWreading: Managed to read three of the one syllable nonword only.

Alliteration: Poor performance (only 1 and 8 were correct)

Rhyming: Very poor performance

Wrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to four words. (Fair performance)

NWrep: Managed only the first two series, which had only one word each (poor
performance)

RN: took 71 seconds to correctly name all the items.

SDis: performed relatively well.

Subject 4 (Grade II)

Alphabet: Excellent performance

SC: simple shapes are drawn correctly, poor copying of complex shape

Spelling: Very poor performance

HQ: Bad performance

Reading: Managed only 3(very poor performances)

NWreading: Managed to read only three of the monosyllabic words

Alliteration: Poor performance

Rhyming: Poor performance

Wrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to four words in it.

NWrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to two non words in it
RN: Took 92 seconds to name all the items

SDis: Performed poorly

Subject 5 (Grade I1)

Alphabet: m was written incorrectly

SC: Correctly written simple shapes, complex shape was better approximating towards
stimulus shape

Spelling: Very poor performance

HQ: Performed fairly

Reading: Seven of the words were read correctly (poor performance)

NWreading: No correct responses

Alliteration: Poor performance

Rhyming: Poor performance

Wrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to four words.

NWrep. Could repeat stimulus series which had up to two non words in it

RN: Took 73 seconds to name all the items

SDis: Performed fairly well

Subject 6 (Grade 1I)

Alphabet: Had confusion between 9, q and couldn’t write u.

SC: Correctly written simple shapes, complex shape was fairly drawn.

Spelling: Very poor performance

HQ: Performed fairly

Reading: Six of the words were read correctly, still a poor performance
NWreading: Managed to read only three of the monosyllabic words

Alliteration: Poor performance

Rhyming: Poor performance

Wrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to four words.

NWrep: Could repeat stimulus series which had up to two non words in it

RN: Took 66 seconds to repeat all the items.

SDis: Performed fairly well

Subject 7 (Grade II1)

Alphabet: Performed excellently

SC: All the simple shapes were copied correctly, good performance in copying complex
shape

Spelling: Performed poorly

HQ: Bad performance

Reading: Number of correct responses is over ten, and most of the correct responses were
correct for the first ten stimulus.

NWreading: Managed only the single syllable words.

Alliteration: Fair performance

Rhyming: Poor performance

Wrep: Good, managed up to stimulus series which had four words in it

NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it

RN: Took 48 seconds to name all the items

SDis: Excellent performance


Subject 8 (Grade II1)

Alphabet: Performed excellently

SC: All the simple shapes were copied correctly, fair performance in copying complex shape

Spelling: Performed poorly

HQ: Good performance

Reading: Number of correct responses was ten, and most of the correct responses were
correct for the first ten stimulus

NWreading: Managed to read only the single syllable words.

Alliteration: Fair performance

Rhyming: Poor performance

Wrep: Good, managed up to stimulus series which had four words in it

NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it

RN: Took 38 seconds to name all the items

SDis: Good performance

Subject 9 (Grade III )

Alphabet: Confusion exhibited between 9 and q

SC: All the simple shapes were copied correctly, fair performance in copying complex shape

Spelling: Performed poorly

HQ: Good performance

Reading: Number of correct responses was over ten, and most of the correct responses were
correct for the first ten stimulus.

NWreading: Managed only the single syllable words.


Alliteration: Poor performance

Rhyming: Poor performance

Wrep: Fair, managed up to stimulus series this had four words in it

NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it

RN: Took 44 seconds to name all the given items

SDis: Good performance

Subject 10 (Grade IV)

Alphabet: Excellent performance

SC: Excellent performance

Spelling: Good performance

HQ: Very good, closely approximating excellence

Reading: Half of the words were read correctly (i.e. 35, however 90 percent of the correctly
read words were in first half of the stimulus)

NWreading: Could even read one bisyllabic nonword

Alliteration: Very good performance

Rhyming: Good performence

Wrep: Could repeat only up to three word stimulus series (very poor performance)

NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it

RN: Took 32 seconds to name all the items

SDis: Excellent performence

Subject 11(Grade 1V)


Alphabet: Excellent performance

SC: Performed very good in complex shape

Spelling: Fair performance

HQ: Good performance

Reading: Fair performance

NWreading: Read only monosyllabic words

Alliteration: Poor performance

Rhyming: Poor performance

Wrep: Could repeat only up to three word stimulus series (very poor performance)

NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had two nonwords in it

RN: Took 45 seconds to name all the items

SDis: All are correct except 19th item, i.e rip and reap (Very good performance)

Subject 12(Grade 1V)

Alphabet: Excellent performance

SC: Good Performance in complex shape

Spelling: Poor performance

HQ: Very good performance

Reading: Poor performance

NWreading: Read only the monosyllabic words

Alliteration: Poor performance

Rhyming: Fair performance

Wrep: Could repeat only up to three word stimulus series (very poor performance
NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had two nonwords in it

RN: Took 37 seconds to name all the items

SDis: Excellent performance

Subject 13(Grade 1V)

Alphabet: Excellent performance

SC: Good Performance in complex shape

Spelling: Poor performance

HQ: Very good performance

Reading: Poor performance

NWreading: Read only the monosyllabic words

Alliteration: Good performance

Rhyming: Poor performance

Wrep: Fair, managed up to stimulus series this had four words in it

NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had two nonwords in it

RN: Took 40 seconds to name all the items

SDis: Excellent performance

Subject 14 (Grade V)

Alphabet: u was written incorrectly

SC: Could not copy two of the simple shapes and poor copying of complex shape

Spelling: Very poor performance


HQ: Poor performance

Reading: Very poor performance

NWreading: Only monosyllable words

Alliteration: Good performance

Rhyming: Very poor performance

Wrep: Fair, managed up to stimulus series this had four words in it

NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it

RN: Took 27 seconds to name all the items

SDis: Excellent performence

Subject 15(Grade V)

Alphabet: Excellent performance

SC: Good copying of complex shape

Spelling: Fair performance

HQ: Good performance

Reading: Less than half is read correctly (poor performance)

NWreading: Only monosyllable words were read correctly

Alliteration: Very good performance

Rhyming: Fair performance

Wrep: Fair, managed up to stimulus series this had four words in it

NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it

RN: Took 26 seconds to name all the items

SDis: Excellent performance


Subject 16(Grade V)

Alphabet: Excellent performance

SC: Good copying of complex shape

Spelling: Fair performance

HQ: Very good performance

Reading: Very poor performance

NWreading: Only the monosyllable words were read correctly

Alliteration: Good performance

Rhyming: Good performance

Wrep: Fair, managed up to stimulus series this had four words in it

NWrep: Good, managed to repeat stimulus series which had three nonwords in it

RN: Took 31seconds to name all the items

SDis: Excellent performance

*Performance rating-very bad <bad < very poor <poor < fair <good < very good <excellent

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy