Evaluation Revision Questions
Evaluation Revision Questions
Retrieval failure theory suggests that forgetting occurs when information stored in
long-term memory cannot be accessed due to a lack of appropriate retrieval cues. The
theory emphasizes the importance of context and cues in aiding recall. If the
necessary cues are not present, the information may temporarily seem forgotten, even
it is still stored in memory.
1. Empirical Support: Studies such as Tulving and Pearlstone (1966) show that providing
retrieval cues significantly improves recall, demonstrating the role of cue-dependent
forgetting.
2. Practical Applications: The theory explains everyday phenomena like "tip-of-the-tongue"
states and suggests practical strategies, such as context reinstatement, to improve memory.
3. Broad Applicability: It accounts for forgetting across different settings, including educational
and clinical contexts, making it a versatile explanation.
1. Limited Scope: It primarily addresses accessibility issues rather than permanent memory
loss, overlooking other explanations such as decay or interference.
2. Difficult to Distinguish: It can be challenging to determine whether information is truly
inaccessible due to retrieval failure or has decayed over time.
3. Overemphasis on Cues: While cues are essential, the theory may oversimplify the complex
nature of memory, ignoring other factors like emotional states or neurological changes.
Conclusion
While retrieval failure theory provides a robust framework for understanding
forgetting, its reliance on cues and limited scope suggest that it works best in
conjunction with other theories to fully explain the complexities of forgetting.
1. Sensory Memory: Receives sensory input from the environment and retains it briefly
(milliseconds to a few seconds). If attention is paid, the information moves to STM.
2. Short-Term Memory: A temporary store with a limited capacity of 7±2 items (Miller, 1956)
and a duration of around 18–30 seconds (Peterson & Peterson, 1959). Maintenance
rehearsal is needed to keep information active or transfer it to LTM.
3. Long-Term Memory: A potentially unlimited store with a duration ranging from minutes to a
lifetime. Information here is encoded semantically and retrieved when needed.
Strengths of the MSM
1. Evidence for Separate Stores: Case studies like that of HM demonstrate a distinction
between STM and LTM. HM’s inability to form new LTMs while retaining STM supports the
idea of separate memory systems.
2. Experimental Support: Research on the serial position effect (primacy and recency effects)
by Glanzer and Cunitz shows how information is managed differently by STM and LTM.
3. Foundational Model: The MSM was pioneering and laid the groundwork for later, more
detailed memory models.
1. Oversimplification: The model assumes STM and LTM are unitary stores, but research (e.g.,
Baddeley and Hitch’s working memory model) suggests STM has multiple components, and
LTM consists of different types (e.g., episodic, procedural).
2. Overemphasis on Rehearsal: The MSM implies rehearsal is the primary mechanism for
transferring information to LTM, but other factors, such as emotional significance or
distinctiveness, also play a role.
3. Neglects Interactions: The MSM fails to account for how information flows back from LTM to
STM (e.g., during problem-solving).
Conclusion 1
The Multi-Store Model of Memory (proposed by Atkinson and Shiffrin) provides a foundational
framework for understanding human memory by emphasizing the roles of sensory, short-term,
and long-term memory stores. Its simplicity and clear structure have been instrumental in
advancing research on memory processes. However, the model has limitations, including its
oversimplification of memory processes, lack of emphasis on the interactions between memory
stores, and failure to account for complex phenomena such as the influence of emotions or the
role of implicit memory.
Conclusion 2
While it remains a valuable starting point for studying memory, more recent models,
such as the Working Memory Model and theories of long-term memory, offer a more
nuanced and comprehensive understanding of how memory functions in real-world
contexts. Thus, the Multi-Store Model is best viewed as an essential step in the
evolution of cognitive psychology, providing a basis for future refinement and
exploration
The Working Memory Model (WMM), proposed by Baddeley and Hitch (1974),
presents a more sophisticated understanding of short-term memory (STM) compared
to the Multi-Store Model. It describes STM as a dynamic system with multiple
components:
1. Central Executive: Acts as a control system, directing attention and coordinating information
from the slave systems.
2. Phonological Loop: Handles auditory information, with two subcomponents:
1. Phonological Store: Stores spoken words briefly.
2. Articulatory Process: Rehearses verbal information to keep it active.
3. Visuospatial Sketchpad: Deals with visual and spatial information.
4. Episodic Buffer (added later): Integrates information from the other components and links
working memory to long-term memory (LTM).
1. Empirical Support: Dual-task studies (e.g., Baddeley et al., 1975) demonstrate that
performing two tasks using different components (e.g., visual and verbal) is easier than using
the same component, supporting the idea of separate subsystems.
2. Accounts for Complexity: Unlike the Multi-Store Model, the WMM explains how STM
processes multiple types of information simultaneously.
3. Real-World Relevance: The WMM explains everyday activities, such as reading (phonological
loop) or navigating (visuospatial sketchpad).
4. Neuroscientific Evidence: Brain imaging studies show distinct brain regions active during
tasks involving the phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad.
Conclusion
The Working Memory Model represents a significant improvement over earlier
models like the Multi-Store Model, offering a detailed and dynamic view of short-
term memory. However, its lack of clarity about the central executive and some
oversimplifications highlight the need for further refinement and integration with
broader theories of memory
1. Insight into Memory Errors: The theory effectively explains why memory is prone to
distortion, such as the inclusion of false details or the omission of unfamiliar elements.
2. Application to Real Life: It has practical relevance in understanding eyewitness testimony,
false memories, and the influence of schemas in everyday memory.
3. Cultural Influence: Bartlett’s work underscores the role of cultural and social context in
shaping memory, making it particularly valuable in cross-cultural psychology.
4. Support from Modern Research: Subsequent studies, such as those by Loftus and Palmer
(1974) on eyewitness testimony, support the idea that memory is influenced by external
factors like language or suggestion.
1. Methodological Limitations: Bartlett’s War of the Ghosts study lacked standardization, and
his findings relied heavily on qualitative analysis, which could introduce bias.
2. Underemphasis on Accuracy: While the theory focuses on distortions, it does not adequately
address instances when memory is highly accurate, especially for emotionally significant
events (e.g., flashbulb memories).
3. Vagueness in Mechanisms: The theory lacks specificity regarding how schemas operate or
are formed, making it difficult to predict when distortions are likely to occur.
4. Overgeneralization: Not all memory is reconstructive; for example, procedural and semantic
memories are often more stable and resistant to distortion.
Conclusion: