PERCEPTION-cognitive Psych
PERCEPTION-cognitive Psych
Perception is the process by which we interpret sensory information to make sense of the
world around us. When you look around, whether out a window or across a room, you don't
just see a random assortment of objects. Instead, you perceive patterns, recognize objects,
identify people, and perhaps even notice events happening. This process, while automatic for
us, is quite complex. Computer scientists working on artificial intelligence have found that
replicating human perception is a huge challenge. Neuroscientists have discovered that the
brain areas responsible for processing visual information take up a large portion of the brain’s
cortex. The main issue in perception is understanding how we attach meaning to the sensory
input we receive. What we perceive isn't just raw data; it's our brain's interpretation of that
data, transforming it into something meaningful and useful.
When you look at an object, like a tree or a person, your brain rapidly processes specific
information about it, such as its location, shape, size, texture, and in some cases, its name.
Some psychologists, like James Gibson, argue that we may also instantly gather information
about an object’s function, or how it is typically used. The key question for cognitive
psychologists is how we manage to interpret all this information so quickly and accurately.
Perception, particularly visual perception, is one of the most studied areas in psychology. It
involves interpreting sensory information to identify objects, events, and states. For instance,
when you see a tree, you don’t just observe the image of it but also recognize it as a tree
based on past experiences.
Cognitive psychologists want to understand the processes that allow us to perceive objects.
They ask how much of our perception relies on prior learning, and how much is based on
what we directly experience. They also look at how perception differs from sensation, which
is simply the reception of sensory information, and from other cognitive processes like
reasoning or categorization.
In the classic approach to visual perception, the process begins with distal stimuli—objects or
events in the world, such as a tree or a car. These objects emit light, which travels to our eyes.
When light hits the retina at the back of the eye, it forms an image—called the retinal image.
This image is two-dimensional, upside down, and reversed left to right, yet our brain
interprets it correctly and helps us understand the world around us.
Perception is the process through which we interpret sensory information and assign meaning
to it. When you see an object, like a tree, your brain processes the sensory input (the proximal
stimulus) to form a percept, which is your interpretation of what you're seeing. For example,
despite the image of the tree being upside-down and reversed on the retina, your brain
interprets it as a tree that exists in three-dimensional space, with the tree closer to you than
objects further away, like shrubs. This process is what we call perception, and it’s different
from the raw sensory input we receive.
An interesting concept related to perception is size constancy. When you bring your hand
closer to your face, the size of the image of your hand on your retina changes, but you don’t
perceive it as shrinking. This demonstrates that perception is not directly based on the raw
retinal image but involves interpreting and maintaining consistent perceptions of size despite
changes in the visual input.
Pattern recognition, a key part of perception, involves recognizing an object as belonging to a
category, such as identifying a shrub as a "shrub." This process is essential to how we
interpret the world, as most perception relies on classifying objects, events, or situations.
To explain how we perceive objects, researchers look at different theories. The Gestalt
approach suggests that perception involves organizing visual stimuli into distinct objects and
backgrounds. Some models of perception suggest that sensory input is processed in a bottom-
up fashion, where simple features are built up into more complex patterns. However,
cognitive psychologists argue that top-down processes—such as expectations, prior
knowledge, and context—interact with bottom-up processing, influencing how we perceive
the world.
J. J. Gibson’s theory of “direct perception” takes a different view, suggesting that the
environment provides all the information needed for perception, meaning the brain doesn’t
need to do much processing at all. Instead, we just "pick up" the available information
directly from the environment. This theory contrasts with others that emphasize extensive
processing of sensory information.
Lastly, research on neuropsychological conditions has shown that people can have intact
visual abilities but still struggle with perception, highlighting the complexity of the
perceptual process.
In perception, psychologists use two key terms to refer to the different aspects of stimuli: the
distal stimulus and the proximal stimulus. The distal stimulus is the actual object or event
in the environment, such as the cell phone on your desk. The proximal stimulus refers to the
information that reaches your sensory receptors, like the image of the cell phone on your
retina.
The retina, located at the back of the eye, is made up of various types of neurons that detect
and transmit visual information from the outside world. Even though the information in the
proximal stimulus is often imperfect or incomplete, our brain can still recognize the identity
of the distal stimulus. For example, we can recognize our cell phone from an unusual angle or
when it's partially obscured by an object, like a book bag.
Interestingly, our visual system can recognize objects incredibly quickly, often within a
fraction of a second. Studies show that in as little as 1/10 of a second, we can identify objects
in a scene. But how is this possible? Our visual system benefits from sensory memory,
specifically iconic memory, which is a type of visual sensory memory that holds a brief
image of what we've just seen. This memory system helps us retain visual information briefly,
allowing us to process and recognize objects even if they are only briefly presented.
Visual information registered on the retina (the proximal stimulus) must travel through the
visual pathway, a series of neurons that connect the retina to the primary visual cortex
located in the occipital lobe of the brain. This area is responsible for the initial, basic
processing of visual stimuli. If you place your hand at the back of your head, just above your
neck, the primary visual cortex lies just beneath your skull in that area.
However, the primary visual cortex is only the first step in processing visual information.
Beyond it, researchers have identified over thirty additional areas of the cortex involved in
visual perception. These regions are activated when we recognize more complex objects.
Despite significant research, scientists are still working to determine the specific functions of
these areas and how they contribute to object recognition. For example, in the later part of the
chapter, when discussing face recognition, we will focus on these more advanced cortical
regions that are particularly involved in identifying faces
In the context of visual perception, informational medium and proximal stimulus (or
object stimulation) refer to key concepts that describe the way sensory information is
transmitted from the environment to our sensory systems, particularly our visual system.
1. Informational Medium: This refers to the medium through which sensory
information is transmitted from the environment to the perceiver. For vision, the
informational medium is typically light. Light carries visual information from the
environment (the distal stimulus, or the actual object in the world) to the retina of the
eye, where it is processed. The light can be reflected or emitted from objects and is
key to visual perception.
For example, when light reflects off an object, it carries information about the object’s color,
shape, and texture. The light waves act as the informational medium, carrying the patterns
that are eventually interpreted by the brain as visual stimuli.
2. Proximal Stimulus (or Proximal Stimulation): This is the sensory input that directly
reaches the sensory receptors (like the retina for vision) after passing through the
informational medium. It is the distorted or processed version of the distal stimulus
(the actual object), and it is this stimulus that the brain processes to construct our
perception.
In visual perception, the proximal stimulus is the image formed on the retina. For example,
when you look at a tree outside, the light reflecting off the tree travels through the air (the
informational medium) and strikes your retina, forming a two-dimensional image of the tree.
This image is a distorted version of the tree—it is upside down and reversed, and it might not
show the true size or shape of the tree due to various factors like distance or lighting.
3. The distal stimulus refers to the actual object or event in the external world that is
being perceived. It is the real-world source of the sensory information that our brains
interpret. In visual perception, the distal stimulus is the object itself, such as a tree, a
car, or a person standing in front of you.
For example, if you are looking at a red apple, the distal stimulus is the apple itself—its
color, shape, texture, and size, as it exists in the world around you.
4.A perceptual object is the mental representation or interpretation that we form of a
distal stimulus (the real object or event) after it has been processed by our sensory
systems. It is how we perceive or "see" the object in our minds, based on the sensory
input (the proximal stimulus) we receive.
For example, when you look at a red apple (the distal stimulus), the light from the apple
enters your eyes and forms a pattern on your retina (the proximal stimulus). Your brain then
processes this sensory information and interprets it, allowing you to recognize the object as a
perceptual object — a red apple. This perceptual object includes all the features you
recognize about the apple, such as its color, shape, size, and even its function or meaning
(e.g., "This is something I can eat").
Gestalt principles refer to a set of rules that describe how we tend to organize visual
elements into groups or unified wholes. These principles, developed by psychologists in the
early 20th century, explain how we perceive patterns, structures, and forms in the world
around us. Here are some key Gestalt principles:
1. Proximity: Objects that are close to each other are perceived as a group or pattern.
For example, if dots are placed close together, we tend to see them as a set, even if
they're just separate points.
2. Similarity: Items that are similar in shape, size, color, or other characteristics are
perceived as part of a group. For instance, a group of circles and squares might be
seen as two separate sets, even if they are arranged in the same space, simply because
of the visual differences between the two shapes.
3. Continuity: We tend to perceive continuous patterns, lines, or shapes, even if they are
interrupted. This principle suggests that our brain likes to see smooth, uninterrupted
flows rather than broken lines. For example, a series of dashed lines might be
perceived as a continuous line.
4. Closure: When parts of an image are missing or incomplete, we tend to fill in the
gaps and perceive a whole image. For example, a circle with a small gap in it will still
be perceived as a complete circle, even if part of it is missing.
5. Figure-Ground: This principle refers to how we distinguish objects (figures) from
their background (ground). The figure is what we focus on, while the ground is the
background. The famous vase-face illusion is an example, where we can perceive
either a vase or two faces, depending on how we focus on the figure-ground
relationship.
6. Symmetry: We tend to perceive symmetrical elements as part of a unified group or
pattern. Symmetry provides a sense of balance and order, making it easier for us to
organize visual information.
7. Common Fate: Objects moving in the same direction are perceived as a group. For
example, a flock of birds flying together in the same direction is seen as a single unit,
even though each bird is an individual object.
8. Prägnanz (Simplicity): This principle suggests that we perceive the simplest, most
stable form of a visual stimulus. When presented with a complex image, we tend to
interpret it in the simplest form possible, such as perceiving a complex figure as a set
of basic geometric shapes.
THEORIES OF PERCEPTION
The study of object recognition has led to various theories about how we identify and
interpret objects in our environment. Here’s a breakdown of the three primary theories:
BOTTOM UP APPROACH OR THRORIES (Data-Driven)
1)TEMPLATE THEORY
he template-matching theory suggests that perception works by comparing incoming
stimuli to stored templates (pre-existing patterns) in our memory. This concept is akin to how
check-sorting machines at banks process numbers on checks by matching them to pre-stored
templates to identify the correct information. Similarly, when we perceive an object or
pattern, we compare it to a template and recognize it if the incoming pattern closely matches
one of the templates.
1. Template Matching in Technology:
o In check-sorting systems, templates help identify account numbers by
comparing the incoming numbers to stored patterns, ensuring that the correct
bank and account are selected.
o A template can be thought of as a "stencil" that allows us to recognize a
pattern as long as it fits closely to the template.
2. Perceptual Process:
o In this theory, when we encounter a new object or stimulus, we compare it to a
variety of stored templates.
o If a match is found, we identify the stimulus. If there’s a close match between
several templates, additional processing is needed to determine the exact
match.
3. Problems with Template Matching:
o Impossibly Large Number of Templates: For template matching to explain
perception, we would need to store an enormous number of templates for all
objects and stimuli, which is highly impractical.
o Recognition of New Objects: The model struggles to explain how we can
recognize new objects or things we haven't encountered before (e.g., new
technologies like DVDs or smartphones).
o Variation in Stimuli: We recognize patterns despite great variability in how
they are presented. For example, the sentence "I like cog. psych." might look
vastly different in each person’s handwriting, yet we can still recognize it as
the same sentence. Template matching would require a separate template for
each possible variation, which is not feasible.
4. Generalization Issue:
o People can recognize objects as the same even when they are presented in
different orientations or forms. For example, a chair might be seen from a
different angle or be upside down, but we still recognize it as a chair. Template
matching would have difficulty explaining this flexibility because it implies
we need a different template for each variation.
5. Adjustment Before Matching:
o The theory doesn’t address how we know when to adjust an object before
attempting to match it to a template (e.g., rotating an object to match it with a
template). This creates ambiguity, as we may not know the object’s exact
orientation or form at the beginning of the process.
FEATURE ANALYSIS THEORY
Feature Analysis Theory is a psychological theory of perception that proposes we recognize
objects by identifying their individual components, or features, rather than processing them
as whole objects. This model contrasts with more holistic approaches to perception, like
template matching, which involves comparing an object to a stored template.
1. Features: Basic components or distinctive elements of an object (e.g., lines, edges,
angles). According to this theory, when we view an object, we first break it down into
its individual features and then combine these features to recognize the whole object.
2. Feature Detectors: The brain has specialized neurons or cells (such as those in the
visual cortex) that respond to specific features of a stimulus, such as lines, edges, and
orientations. These detectors allow us to detect and identify these individual
components before assembling them into a complete perception of the object.
3. Featural Representation: The theory suggests that an object is represented by a
collection of features that are recognized and stored in memory. This helps explain
how we recognize objects even when they are presented in various forms,
orientations, or sizes.
Theoretical Models and Evidence:
1. Neurophysiological Evidence:
o Research by Hubel and Wiesel (1962, 1968) demonstrated that cells in the
visual cortex of animals respond to specific visual stimuli. Some cells are line
detectors that respond to lines of particular orientations (horizontal, vertical,
or diagonal), while others may respond to more complex features.
o Edge detectors respond to the boundary between light and dark areas, and
motion detectors may react to movement, helping the brain break down and
recognize different components of an object.
o These specialized cells in the brain provide support for the featural analysis
model, suggesting that perception involves identifying specific features in a
stimulus.
2. Recognition by Components (Biederman):
o Irving Biederman (1987) extended feature analysis theory with his
Recognition by Components (RBC) theory. He proposed that objects are
recognized by breaking them down into a set of geons (geometric components
such as cubes, cylinders, and spheres). These geons can be combined in
various ways to form complex objects.
o Biederman argued that there are 36 basic geons, and just like phonemes in
language (the basic units of sound), combining these geons allows us to
recognize a wide variety of objects.
Strengths of Feature Analysis Theory:
1. Flexibility: Feature analysis allows us to recognize objects in various forms or
orientations (e.g., recognizing a letter ‘A’ even if it’s slanted or in a different font).
2. Neurological Evidence: There is strong evidence from brain studies, such as the
discovery of specialized neurons (feature detectors) that respond to specific aspects of
visual stimuli.
Limitations:
1. Complexity of Real-World Objects: While feature analysis works well for simple
objects or letters, it may not explain how we recognize complex objects or scenes in
the real world. For instance, recognizing a horse involves more than detecting basic
geometric shapes; the complexity of natural objects can be overwhelming for feature-
based models.
2. Relationships Among Features: Feature analysis primarily focuses on individual
features but doesn’t always account for the relationships between these features. For
instance, how the parts of an object fit together is also crucial for recognition. The
theory must explain how features are combined to form meaningful objects in the
context of their spatial relationships.