Wakefulness and Sleep PDF
Wakefulness and Sleep PDF
and Sleep
BY: MARK CRISTIAN BERNABE
IRISH NICOLE FERRER
MARIA ALYSHIA PARREÑAS
SLEEP
kkk
on
H
i
i m
m
Mi
Sleep is a subject that fascinates most people.
Many people today report that they do not get
enough sleep and yet there is still controversy
over the function of sleep and how much sleep is
‘enough’. Sleep is adapted depending on the
current environment and other competing
motivations.
The onset of sleep varies according to levels of
sleepiness, and the period of sleep can be
extended or shortened according to
circumstances. There are also consequences of
sleep deprivation, such as an increased likelihood
of having an accident while driving.
Sleep is a period of reduced activity during which
a person has their eyes closed and is not aware of
their immediate environment.
Provides a measure of overall brain activity. In
EEG, electrodes are attached to the scalp with a
special paste. These electrodes measure the
activity of large populations of neurons that are
recordable on the head surface.
The electrical signals detected by the scalp
electrodes are amplified and then passed to a
computer, and the resultant ‘brain waves’ can be
displayed. EEG can record spontaneous activity
and activity in response to a specific stimulus.
In 1924 Hans Berger, a German
neurologist, used ordinary radio
equipment to amplify the brain’s
electrical activity and depict it
graphically on a strip of paper. He
observed that the activity changed
in certain neural diseases, such as in
epilepsy. EEG recording procedures
are non-invasive, safe and painless.
EEG shows us that different levels of
responsiveness to external stimuli are associated
with specific patterns of brain activity. Therefore,
in humans, sleep is usually defined according to
electrophysiological criteria. Different states of
waking and sleep are also associated with
changes in the patterns.
DEFINING SLEEP
STAGES
kkk
on
H
i
i m
m
Mi
There are some areas of the brain that are more
active during sleep than waking. In addition, only
small reductions in cerebral blood flow have been
observed during sleep (Kety, 1956). Sleep
involves specific patterns of brain activity that we
will discover.
Electrical activity in the brain, muscles and eyes, which
can be measured by EEG, EMG and EOG, is characterized
by oscillating activity represented by waveforms that
vary in their frequency (the rhythm of the waves),
amplitude (the wave intensity or height) and shape.
When the pattern repeats, this is a regular or
synchronized waveform, and when there is no discernible
repeating pattern, this is an irregular waveform. We will
first describe the activity associated with the waking
state and then define sleep according to changes in this
pattern.
During the alert waking state, the EEG consists of what
is called beta activity. But when a person is relaxed but
still awake or resting with their eyes closed, the EEG
consists of alpha activity.
Alpha activity - Regular pattern, a rhythm of 8–13 cycles per second. Indicates
relaxed wakefulness with eyes closed. During waking there is also evidence of
muscle and eye movement activity in the EMG and EOG.
The transition from waking to sleep is defined by the
presence of a different kind of EEG activity: theta wave
activity. Theta wave activity occurs at a frequency of 3–
7Hz, has slightly higher amplitude than alpha waves, and
is associated with Stage 1 sleep.
Stage 1 sleep is known as the transitional stage and it
usually lasts for a short period. A person may open and
close their eyes and the eyes may roll upward and
downwards during this period.
After about ten minutes of theta wave activity, the EEG
changes again and very distinctive patterns interrupt the
theta rhythms. These patterns take the form of sleep
spindles, which are a burst of high-frequency activity
lasting between 0.5 and 1.0 second, and K-complexes,
which consist of a sharp negative deflection in the
waves followed immediately by slower positive
deflection.
The presence of sleep spindles and K-complexes defines
Stage 2 of sleep. This is still a light phase of sleep when
a person may still be woken easily by noises and
changes in the environment and they may still deny
being asleep if roused.
As a person continues to sleep, it becomes more
difficult to wake them, and this coincides with another
change to the EEG pattern. When someone goes into
deep sleep, it is known as slow wave sleep.
When delta wave activity becomes apparent or seen in
the EEG, this defines Stage 3 of sleep. When the
proportion of the EEG taken up with delta waves is more
than 50%, then this defines Stage 4 sleep.
Awake
Alpha
N1
Theta N2
N3
Delta
1 2 3 4 5 6
Hours
THE BIOLOGICAL
BASES OF WAKING AND
SLEEP STATES
kkk
on
H
i
i m
m
Mi
Circadian rhythms direct a wide variety of functions from daily
changes in wakefulness to body temperature, metabolism, and the
release of hormones.
Glutamatergic neurons - When these are active, they release glutamate, which keeps us
awake and alert.
Cholinergic neurons - Release acetylcholine when activated; they disrupt deep sleep which
makes the brain more active even during sleep.
GABAergic neurons - When activated, it releases GABA which slightly reduces the amount
of REM sleep.
Laterodorsal tegmental nuclei (LDT) - is a brainstem nucleus
classically involved in REM sleep and attention. It modulates
dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens.
MCH neurons - These are active mostly during REM sleep. They help promote sleep
and are thought to reduce wakefulness, helping the brain and body rest.
Early studies of the controls of REM sleep by Jouvet and others showed
that large lesions made in the pons disrupted REM sleep, suggesting
that this brainstem area is important for REM sleep (Jouvet, 1962;
Webster and Jones, 1988).
The shift from waking to sleep state is controlled by a switching
mechanism that depends upon groups of neurons that inhibit each
other (Saper et al., 2001). The idea is based on electronic switches
called ‘flip-flops’ that enable fast and complete transitions
between different states.
CIRCADIAN INFLUENCES ON
SLEEP: THE IMPORTANCE
OF BIOLOGICAL RHYTHMS
kkk
on
H
i
i m
m
Mi
A circadian rhythm is an example of a biological rhythm.
Biological rhythms are periodic changes in a body’s
functioning. These rhythms help us distinguish between
day and night. There are different types of rhythms that are
defined by the amount of time it takes to complete one
cycle, known as period.
Sleep–wake and other daily patterns, including alertness,
mood, stress, heart function and immunity, corresponds to
a circadian rhythm. Several genes have been identified,
including the period (per), clock (clk) and timeless (tim)
genes. Variation in these genes has been linked to whether
someone is a morning-type person (lark) or a night-time-
preferring person (owl).
Humans are adapted to a diurnal pattern of behavior. Our
physiology is not the same at night as it is during the day.
Effectively, we have a biological day time and a biological
night time. The reason for this is that we have an internal
biological clock that keeps track of time.
The master body clock is responsible for orchestrating
biological rhythms, consists of a pair of nuclei, one nucleus
on either side of the brain, in the hypothalamus called the
suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) (Ralph et al., 1990). The SCN
does not make direct connections with sleep centers in the
brain. They communicate indirectly to the VLPO sleep-
promoting center.
Light cues are important for keeping the biological clock in
check and keeping it synchronized to the solar day. This is
because, in the absence of external cues to time of day,
the biological clock runs slightly longer than 24 hours.
Cues that reset the clock are called Zeitgebers, which
means ‘time-giver’ in German. It is a stimulus or event that
sets a biological clock.
Older adults tend to report a preference for earlier habitual
bedtime and getting-up time than do younger individuals (Duffy
and Czeisler, 2002). During adolescence, there is a shift in the
body clock which favors later sleep and waking times
(Roenneberg et al., 2004). This observation has led to the
suggestion that school start times should be delayed to avoid
the potentially detrimental effects of misalignment of sleep
patterns with the school day on academic performance
(Adolescent Sleep Working Group, 2014).
HOMEOSTATIC INFLUENCES
ON SLEEP: THE SEARCH FOR
SLEEP FACTORS
kkk
on
H
i
i m
m
Mi
Homeostatic theories suggest that there is an optimal level of
sleep that we require, so if we do not get enough sleep there are
mechanisms that work to compensate for this by increasing
sleep. Quite some time ago it was proposed that there are
sleep-promoting substances that accumulate in the body during
prolonged wakefulness. This idea was based on reports that
injections of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) from sleep-deprived dogs
induces sleep in recipient dogs (see Kubota, 1989).
The main candidate for a sleep factor is adenosine. Production
of adenosine in the brain is linked to energy utilization by
cells.Injection of an adenosine agonist into the basal forebrain
wake-promoting area or near the VLPO sleep-promoting area
causes sleep (Scammell et al., 2001).
EMOTIONAL AND COGNITIVE
INFLUENCES ON SLEEP
kkk
on
H
i
i m
m
Mi
Sleep can be resisted in response to external factors. Sleep can
be put off to deal with an emergency. The ability to stay awake
even when other factors are pushing us towards sleep probably
involves activation of prefrontal cortical areas, perhaps driven
by emotional responses generated in the amygdala (Nofzinger et
al., 2004). Sleep can also be promoted by the presence of cues
previously associated with sleep. In other words, we learn that
some environments are good for sleep and these cues can make
one feel sleepy.
Stressful situations reduce the propensity to sleep and increase
levels of arousal. The stress neurotransmitter corticotropin-
releasing hormone (CRH) has been found to stimulate orexin
neurons and it has been proposed that arousal associated with
the stress response is mediated by release of orexins (Paneda
et al., 2005). Hunger is also associated with increased arousal
and reduced sleep, and this is probably also due to increased
levels of orexins during food deprivation (Sakurai et al., 1998).
FUNCTIONS OF
SLEEP
kkk
on
H
i
i m
m
Mi
There has been some debate about whether sleep is necessary for the
building and restoration of material used during wakefulness.
Biochemicals and body tissues may be depleted during waking and then
restored by sleep. In support of this idea, growth hormone is released
during sleep. Studies are consistent with the idea that wakefulness
exerts some stress on the body and brain and that the sleeping state
restores optimal functioning. Another core function of sleep seems to be
in facilitating learning and memory. There is evidence that both slow
wave sleep and rapid eye movement sleep support memory consolidation
(Diekelmann and Born, 2010).
The function of sleep may involve a combination of
restoration, predator avoidance, energy conservation
and memory consolidation. Although many people
can get by with small amounts of sleep, we all sleep,
suggesting that sleep has some essential function.
SLEEP DISTURBANCES AND
IMPLICATIONS FOR
HEALTH
INTRODUCTION
NARCOLEPSY
PARASOMNIAS
INSOMNIA
INSOMNIA
Insomnia is a common sleep problem that causes difficulty falling and staying asleep.
Additionally, you can wake up too early and find it difficult to fall back asleep. When you
wake up, you can still feel exhausted. Your attitude and energy levels can be negatively
impacted by insomnia.
Your quality of life, productivity at work, and health may also be impacted.
INSOMNIA
How does this compare with how much sleep our grandparents or great-
grandparents got?
DO YOU GET
ENOUGH SLEEP?
Most people are likely to say they could use more sleep.
However, the amount of sleep needed varies between
individuals, and it is important to assess one’s sleep needs
accurately. Current lifestyle trends and technology usage
before bed often interfere with the body’s natural need for
rest.
WHAT DO YOU THINK
IS THE AVERAGE
SLEEPING HOURS?
WHAT DO YOU THINK IS THE AVERAGE
SLEEPING HOURS?
Adults in the UK and USA typically sleep around 7 hours per night,
according to surveys (Groeger et al., 2004; Krueger and Friedman,
2009). However, this average conceals significant variation: while
some people report sleeping less than 6 hours, others sleep more
than 9 hours.
The relationship between work hours and sleep duration has been
noted, with longer workdays often resulting in less sleep (Basner
et al., 2007).
For adults, the average sleep duration is 7 hours, the reality for
many people deviates from this.
Studies show that when people don’t get enough sleep, their leptin
levels decrease, and ghrelin levels increase, leading to overeating and
potential weight gain.
People who sleep fewer than 6 hours per night are more likely to have a
higher body mass index (BMI) compared to those who get 7-9 hours of
sleep.
BMI- Body mass index is a measure of body fat based on height and
weight that applies to adult men and women.
This relationship between sleep loss and obesity may be driven by changes
in metabolism and increased caloric intake due to altered hormone levels
(Ayas et al., 2003; Taheri et al., 2004).
HEALTH EFFECTS OF SLEEP DEPRIVATION
Different studies use different methods to measure sleep, which can lead to
inconsistencies in the findings. For example, some studies rely solely on
self-reported sleep data, while others use objective measures like EEGs or
actigraphy. This variability in methods may help explain why some studies
find a strong link between sleep and health outcomes, while others do not
(Magee et al., 2008).
CONTROL FOR
CONFOUNDING VARIABLES
One of the challenges in sleep research is accounting for confounding
variables, which can distort the relationship between sleep and health
outcomes.
For example:
Alcohol consumption is associated with both short sleep duration
and obesity.
ALCOHOL
POOR DIET POOR EXERCISE
CONSUMPTION
Lifestyle factors:
Poor diet
Exercise
Alcohol consumption
LIfe style can influence both sleep patterns and health outcomes. In
many cases, the relationship between short sleep and poor health
may be mediated by these factors.
It could be that people who consume alcohol a lot are more likely to
be obese and alcohol use just so happens also to be associated with
short sleeping. Controlling for alcohol consumption would then mean
that the relationship between short sleep duration and obesity
disappears, but not controlling for it in the analysis would give a
spurious result.
REVERSE
CAUSATION
Reverse causation is another issue in sleep research.
In sleep research, reverse causation is the possibility that the
outcome of a study precedes the exposure, which is the opposite of
the typical cause-and-effect relationship. This can occur when the
effect of something is actually its cause.
It is possible that poor health leads to short sleep, rather than the other
way around.
For example:
People with chronic illnesses such as obesity or heart disease may
struggle with sleep problems like sleep apnea, which reduces sleep
duration.
One recent study found that short sleeping was only associated with
greater risk of mortality for adults who had existing health problems at
baseline.
This suggests that the relationship between short sleeping and mortality
may be explained by preexisting disease, at least for adults (Magee et
al., 2013).
SLEEP QUALITY
RATHER THAN
QUANTITY?
IS SLEEP QUALITY MORE IMPORTANT THAN QUANTITY?
A person might sleep for a long time but this sleep might be
interrupted, leading to reductions in the amount of REM sleep.
Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep is the stage of sleep.
increased hunger
impaired glucose metabolism
tendency toward binge eating.
Over time, these issues can contribute to weight gain and other health
problems, highlighting the importance of sleep quality in maintaining
overall well-being
SUMMING UP
It is possible that there is some risk associated with sleeping
around five hours a night over a long period for physiological
function (St-Onge, 2013)