Review of Related Lit - Science Understanding
Review of Related Lit - Science Understanding
Over the past few decades, research in science education has yielded valuable insights
into how children form their comprehension of scientific concepts. In the forthcoming sections, I
will delve into three key facets of this developmental journey: the genesis of intuitive
understandings, the dynamics of science learning, and the phenomenon of conceptual co-
existence. Subsequently, I will explore the implications of these findings for science education.
Science learners construct their initial understanding of the physical world based on their
everyday encounters, forming what are termed as intuitive understandings. These intuitive
understandings are structured within framework theories, which serve as skeletal conceptual
structures. Notably, these framework theories often diverge from established scientific
paradigms, thereby influencing how students interpret scientific explanations. This can lead to
students must undergo a transformative process wherein they revise their representations and
explanations of the physical world, alongside refining their modes of reasoning. Importantly, as
students progress in their scientific knowledge, they develop new concepts and belief systems
that may coexist alongside their preexisting framework theories. This developmental process
unfolds gradually and follows a discernible learning progression. The education received by
acknowledged and justified by recent Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA)
reports in many countries, such as Spain (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and
Development [OECD], 2007, 2016), as noted by Sheldrake (2018). This acceptance can be
(Abrahams & Millar, 2008). These dimensions go beyond mere acquisition of knowledge; they
involve equipping students with the skills to adapt scientific information to meet the evolving
demands of society, where scientific knowledge plays an increasingly crucial role in daily life
In recent years, there has been a notable shift in the goals of secondary science
students—both STEM and non-STEM majors—as future consumers, voters, and policymakers.
This shift is driven by the need to enable students to critically examine and understand socio-
scientific issues and make informed decisions based on scientific evidence (Dauer et al., 2017;
This study advocates for a broader educational objective at the secondary level, which
includes not only acquiring knowledge of the scientific enterprise and its societal role but also
developing intellectual abilities related to scientific inquiry methods. These skills are essential
for students to effectively engage with scientific issues and contribute meaningfully to scientific
discourse. Some of these skills encompass accurately formulating research problems, generating
hypotheses creatively, drawing meaningful conclusions from scientific data, and understanding
the implications of research findings (Charney et al., 2007; Vossen et al., 2018).
Overall, this perspective underscores the importance of secondary science education in
equipping students with not only scientific knowledge but also the critical thinking and analytical
skills necessary for active participation in an increasingly complex and science-driven world.
In light of these insights, effective science education necessitates several key strategies.
Firstly, educators must foster students' awareness of their intuitive understandings, recognizing
them as foundational elements in the learning journey. Secondly, scientific information should be
imparted to students gradually, aligning with their individual learning progressions to optimize
comprehension and retention. Lastly, science education programs should prioritize the cultivation
of students' reasoning abilities and executive function skills, equipping them with the cognitive
By attending to these principles, science educators can enhance the efficacy and
Learning Science
When students embark on their journey of learning science, they do not start as blank
slates. Instead, they bring with them intuitive understandings of the physical world, which may
starkly differ from the scientific concepts and theories introduced in the classroom (Driver &
Easley, 1978; Clement, 1982; McCloskey, 1983; Novak, 1987). While researchers acknowledge
the presence of these intuitive understandings, there exists disagreement regarding their nature,
resemble unitary intuitive theories, often mirroring earlier scientific theories. Conversely, the
framework theory perspective suggests that students' conceptions are a collection of beliefs and
Support for the notion of students' conceptions as stable intuitive theories can be found in
McCloskey's (1983) work, demonstrating systematic beliefs about object motion reminiscent of
medieval impetus theory. However, not all student conceptions fit neatly into this framework.
Chi (2013) highlights false beliefs and mental models, along with constraints on reasoning,
processes. On the opposite end of the spectrum, diSessa (1993) argues for a "knowledge-in-
pieces" perspective, suggesting that students' initial understandings are internally inconsistent
and fragmented, characterized by p-prims. While this view accounts for inconsistencies in
instruction.
Vosniadou and her colleagues (Vosniadou & Brewer, 1992, 1994; Vosniadou, 2013;
Vosniadou & Skopeliti, 2017) advocate for distinguishing between students' pre- and post-
science instruction conceptions. Drawing on evidence from interviews with young children, they
argue that prior to formal instruction, children interpret experiences within the context of lay
culture, forming beliefs organized within loose yet coherent framework theories. These
perspectives, grounded in empirical evidence, offer insights into the diverse nature of students'
intuitive understandings in science education, highlighting the need for nuanced approaches to
instruction that consider both pre-existing conceptions and the impact of formal education on
conceptual development.
The stance taken regarding the nature of students' intuitive understandings holds
significant implications for interpreting the process of science learning. If students' conceptions
are viewed as intuitive theories, then science learning cannot be simply seen as the accumulation
referred to as conceptual change. Posner et al. (1982) argued that conceptual change involves
replacing intuitive theories with correct scientific ones. This replacement is portrayed as a
rational process wherein students must recognize the fundamental assumptions and
In subsequent years, criticisms arose regarding the "classical approach." One contentious
issue was the notion of intuitive understandings being supplanted by scientific theories. Early
arguments for the coexistence of intuitive understandings and scientific concepts emerged (e.g.,
Caravita and Halldén, 1994), gaining empirical support in recent years, as will be discussed later.
science learning, the knowledge-in-pieces approach posits that science learning involves the
integration of various p-prims into coherent scientific theories under instructional influence
(diSessa, 1993, 2008). Smith et al. (1993) argued against cognitive conflict as an effective
instructional strategy, advocating instead for the productive use of intuitive understandings as
resources for learning. They proposed that these intuitive understandings evolve and integrate
into cohesive conceptual structures such as scientific theories through appropriate instruction,
We advocate for a perspective consistent with the framework theory approach to science
learning. According to this view, students initially organize their intuitive understandings into
loose yet relatively cohesive framework theories before exposure to science instruction. These
framework theories differ fundamentally from scientific theories in their explanations, concepts,
scientific theories, their prior framework theories influence their interpretation, often leading to
demonstrated through a text comprehension study that many elementary school students, when
presented with counter-intuitive scientific explanations, either ignored the scientific information
scientific information with prior knowledge. This process underscores that science learning is
gradual and that misconceptions are natural outcomes of constructive learning mechanisms,
Williams (2020), Garcia and Martinez (2021), Nguyen and Chang (2022), and O'Connor and
Murphy (2022) delve into the complex interplay between cognitive development and science
Baggini's (2018) study illuminates the connection between cognitive processes and
influence students' ability to comprehend scientific concepts. Smith (2019) further contributes to
science learning. Students who possess metacognitive skills are better equipped to regulate their
learning, leading to improved cognitive processes and a deeper grasp of scientific principles.
Johnson and Williams (2020) explore the efficacy of inquiry-based learning in fostering
cognitive engagement among high school students. Their findings suggest that active
participation in inquiry activities promotes critical thinking and problem-solving skills, thereby
Garcia and Martinez (2021) conduct a meta-analysis to examine the relationship between
cognitive processes and conceptual change in science education. Their review underscores how
Nguyen and Chang's (2022) systematic review investigates the impact of visual
representations on science learning outcomes among high school students. The study reveals that
well-designed visual aids enhance cognitive processes such as comprehension, retention, and
of digital technologies on science education. Their study discusses how digital tools and
between science understanding and cognitive processes among high school students. By
technologies, these studies offer guidance for educators, researchers, and policymakers seeking
While certain learning skills may naturally develop to some degree without formal
instruction, systematic teaching methods can significantly enhance their cultivation (Spektor-
Levy et al., 2009). Inquiry practices, which involve asking questions, problem-solving, and the
pursuit of knowledge, are not only driven by intellectual curiosity but also by the emotional
confirming or refuting ideas (Jaber & Hammer, 2016). Understanding these emotional aspects is
crucial in both professional disciplinary practices and science education, as they provide insight
effect relationships underlying natural phenomena, pose questions about complex issues relevant
to their lives, make predictions, and hone their logical reasoning skills from a scientific
perspective, thereby fostering the development of scientific competence (Levrini et al., 2019).
Germann and Aram (1996) outlined various tasks aimed at fostering inquiry skills in
students, such as formulating hypotheses, controlling variables, collecting and analyzing data,
and drawing conclusions, with a particular focus on the latter three processes. Their research
highlighted the challenges students face in accurately recording data, considering hypotheses
when drawing conclusions, and providing specific evidence to support their inferences.
Similarly, Toh and Woolnough (1993) examined students' integrated science processing skills,
including variable identification and control, data interpretation, hypothesis formulation, and
experimentation.
secondary students in hypothesis generation based on fossil evidence. Other research efforts have
alternative theories, construct models, present logical arguments and explanations, establish
connections between ideas, extend concepts, and pose relevant questions (Charney et al., 2007).
compulsory secondary students' research on corrosion, has demonstrated how students can
develop various scientific skills, such as generating research ideas, planning experiments, and
classrooms, many practical activities in science education often focus on lower-level skills, such
as manual dexterity, rather than fostering higher-order thinking abilities like hypothesis
formulation or experimental design (Cordón, 2009). This imbalance suggests that students'
development of scientific skills during their compulsory schooling years may fall short of
expectations.
Recent investigations have delved into the scientific skills essential for inquiry, such as
particularly among students pursuing scientific baccalaureate programs (Ferrés et al., 2015).
These studies revealed that many students struggle to grasp the fundamental elements of
scientific inquiry and fail to apply their knowledge to make predictions, explanations, data
analyses, or draw meaningful conclusions (Ferrés, 2017; Ferrés et al., 2015). Notably, students
face challenges in articulating research questions, often finding it more difficult than designing
formulating hypotheses and making simple predictions remains blurred for many, reflecting a
disconnect between their everyday knowledge and scientific thinking (Friedler & Tamir, 1990).
experiments, with limited understanding of how variables impact research outcomes (D’Costa &
Schlueter, 2013; Grunwald & Hartman, 2010). However, inquiry-based learning approaches have
shown promise in addressing these challenges, fostering inquiry and critical thinking skills
(Furtak et al., 2012). Studies indicate that engaging students in authentic scientific inquiry
enhances their ability to generate hypotheses, consider alternatives, implement models, construct
logical arguments, connect ideas, extend concepts, and ask pertinent questions (Charney et al.,
2007). Meaningful participation in real scientific practices can thus enhance students'
multivariable causal analysis, and argumentation have been shown to advance students' scientific
skills and deepen their epistemological understanding of science (Kuhn et al., 2017). Thus,
integrating authentic scientific practices into classroom activities holds promise for improving
students' scientific proficiency and fostering a deeper appreciation for the scientific process.
The discipline of science has long been regarded as a cornerstone of education, providing
students with essential knowledge and skills to understand scientific principles, formulate
hypotheses, and analyze and solve real-world problems (Bhagat, 2018). Mastery of scientific
processes empowers students to comprehend scientific facts, concepts, and even contribute to
new discoveries through experimental inquiry, thereby enhancing their academic performance
(Maranan, 2017). However, despite the critical role of science education, poor achievement in
during national examinations in primary schools, where failures, poor performance, and high
dropout rates persist (Mabula, 2012). Despite efforts to foster competitiveness in science
some regions experiencing improvements while others face ongoing challenges. For instance, in
Tanzania and other parts of East Africa, academic achievement in science subjects has shown
positive trends over time (Shimbi, 2016 as cited by Jidamwa, 2012). However, in Uganda,
science courses remain a challenge, with poor performance noted in national examinations such
students include a lack of qualified teachers, inadequate teaching and learning environments, and
insufficient instructional materials (Komba & Kira, 2013). Additionally, socioeconomic factors,
parental involvement, class size, and curriculum issues contribute to low science performance
On a national level, surveys reveal mixed results regarding science proficiency among
achieved basic competency in science in 2015, while 38 percent reached proficient levels
(Rabino, 2014). However, many students demonstrated only a basic understanding of scientific
concepts, highlighting gaps in science education. Constraints such as lack of parental support,
inadequate teaching methods, insufficient instructional materials, and teacher training further
hinder student performance (Rabino, 2014). Addressing these multifaceted challenges is crucial
for improving science education outcomes and fostering scientific literacy among students.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Allan, N. P., Hume, L. E., Allan, D. M., Farrington, A. L., & Lonigan, C. J. (2014). Relations
between inhibitory control and the development of academic skills in preschool and
kindergarten: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 50, 2368–2379.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037493
Babai, R., Sekal, R., & Stavy, R. (2010). Persistence of the intuitive conception of living things
in adolescence. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 19, 20–26.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-009-9174-2
Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: A
theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research,
63, 1–49. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543063001001
Fugelsang, J., & Mareschal, D. (2014). The development and application of scientific reasoning.
In D. Mareschal, B. Butterworth, & A. Tolmie (Eds.), Educational Neuroscience (pp. 237–267).
Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.
Kelemen, D., Rottman, J., & Seston, R. (2013). Professional physical scientists display tenacious
teleological tendencies: Purpose-based reasoning as a cognitive default. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 142, 1074–1083. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030399
Masson, S., Potvin, P., Riopel, M., & Brault-Foisy, L.-M. (2014). Differences in brain activation
between novices and experts in science during a task involving a common misconception in
electricity. Mind, Brain, and Education, 8, 44–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12043
Shtulman, A., & Valcarel, J. (2012). Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not supplant
earlier intuitions. Cognition, 124, 209–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.04.005
Vosniadou, S., & Skopeliti, I. (2014). Conceptual change from the framework theory side of the
fence. Science & Education, 23, 1427–1445. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-013-9640-3
Johnson, L. E., & Williams, M. K. (2020). Inquiry-Based Learning and Cognitive Engagement:
A Synthesis of Research. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38(4), 321-335.
Nguyen, T. T., & Chang, H. C. (2022). The Impact of Visual Representations on Science
Learning: A Systematic Review. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 50(1), 55-68.
O'Connor, R. C., & Murphy, E. J. (2022). The Impact of Digital Technologies on Science
Education: A Review of Literature. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 55(4), 345-
360.