anchoring effect
anchoring effect
influenced by a reference point or "anchor" which can be completely irrelevant. Both numeric and non-
numeric anchoring have been reported in research. In numeric anchoring, once the value of the anchor
is set, subsequent arguments, estimates, etc. made by an individual may change from what they would
have otherwise been without the anchor. For example, an individual may be more likely to purchase a
car if it is placed alongside a more expensive model (the anchor). Prices discussed in negotiations that
are lower than the anchor may seem reasonable, perhaps even cheap to the buyer, even if said prices
are still relatively higher than the actual market value of the car.[1] Another example may be when
estimating the orbit of Mars, one might start with the Earth's orbit (365 days) and then adjust upward
until they reach a value that seems reasonable (usually less than 687 days, the correct answer).
The original description of the anchoring effect came from psychophysics. When judging stimuli along a
continuum, it was noticed that the first and last stimuli were used to compare the other stimuli (this is
also referred to as "end anchoring"). This was applied to attitudes by Muzafer Sherif et al. in their 1958
article "Assimilation and Contrast Effects of Anchoring Stimuli on Judgments".[2]
Experimental findings
[edit]
The anchoring and adjustment heuristic was first theorized by Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman.[3] In
one of their first studies, participants were asked to compute, within 5 seconds, the product of the
numbers one through to eight, either as 1 × 2 × 3 × 4 × 5 × 6 × 7 × 8 or reversed as 8 × 7 × 6 × 5 × 4 × 3 × 2
× 1. Because participants did not have enough time to calculate the full answer, they had to make an
estimate after their first few multiplications. When these first multiplications gave a small answer –
because the sequence started with small numbers – the median estimate was 512; when the sequence
started with the larger numbers, the median estimate was 2,250. (The correct answer is 40,320.) In
another study by Tversky and Kahneman, participants were asked to estimate the percentage of African
countries in the United Nations. Before estimating, the participants first observed a roulette wheel that
was predetermined to stop on either 10 or 65. Participants whose wheel stopped on 10 guessed lower
values (25% on average) than participants whose wheel stopped at 65 (45% on average).[3] The pattern
has held in other experiments for a wide variety of different subjects of estimation.
As a second example, in a study by Dan Ariely, an audience is first asked to write the last two digits of
their social security number and consider whether they would pay this number of dollars for items
whose value they did not know, such as wine, chocolate and computer equipment. They were then
asked to bid for these items, with the result that the audience members with higher two-digit numbers
would submit bids that were between 60 percent and 120 percent higher than those with the lower
social security numbers, which had become their anchor.[4] When asked if they believed the number was
informative of the value of the item, quite a few said yes.[5] Trying to avoid this confusion, a small
number of studies used procedures that were clearly random, such as Excel random generator
button[6] and die roll,[7] and failed to replicate anchoring effects.
The anchoring effect was also found to be present in a study[8] in the Journal of Real Estate Research in
relation to house prices. In this investigation, it was established that the 2-year and 9-year highs on
the Case-Shiller House Price Index could be used as anchors in predicting current house prices. The
findings were used to indicate that, in forecasting house prices, these 2-year and 9-years highs might be
relevant.
The anchoring effect was also found to be present in a study in the Journal of Behavioral Finance in
relation to stock purchase behavior.[9] The study found that when using an app-based stock brokerage, an
investor’s first stock purchase price serves as an anchor for future stock purchases. The findings indicate
that when investors start by making only a small stock purchase, they end up with less accumulated
investments in the long run.
Characteristics
[edit]
Difficulty of avoiding
[edit]
Various studies have shown that anchoring is very difficult to avoid. For example, in one study students
were given anchors that were wrong. They were asked whether Mahatma Gandhi died before or after
age 9, or before or after age 140. Clearly neither of these anchors can be correct, but when the two
groups were asked to suggest when they thought he had died, they guessed significantly differently
(average age of 50 vs. average age of 67).[10]
Other studies have tried to eliminate anchoring much more directly. In a study exploring the causes and
properties of anchoring, participants were exposed to an anchor and asked to guess how many
physicians were listed in the local phone book. In addition, they were explicitly informed that anchoring
would "contaminate" their responses, and that they should do their best to correct for that. A control
group received no anchor and no explanation. Regardless of how they were informed and whether they
were informed correctly, all of the experimental groups reported higher estimates than the control
group. Thus, despite being expressly aware of the anchoring effect, most participants were still unable to
avoid it.[11] A later study found that even when offered monetary incentives, most people are unable to
effectively adjust from an anchor.[12]
Although it has been found through many research and experiments that attempt to mitigate the
decision heuristic of anchoring bias is either marginally significant or not successful at all, it can be found
that the consider-the-opposite (COS strategy) has been the most reliable in mitigating the anchoring bias
(Adame, 2016). In short, the COS strategy is proposed to an individual by asking them to consider the
possibilities the opposite of their perceptions and beliefs. Therefore, depriving the individual of their
preexisting attitudes and limiting the decision bias. [13]
Durability of anchoring
[edit]
Anchoring effects are also shown to remain adequately present given the accessibility of knowledge
pertaining to the target. This, in turn, suggests that despite a delay in judgement towards a target, the
extent of anchoring effects have seen to remain unmitigated within a given time period. A series of three
experiments were conducted to test the longevity of anchoring effects. It was observed that despite a
delay of one week being introduced for half the sample population of each experiment, similar results of
immediate judgement and delayed judgement of the target were achieved. The experiments concluded
that external information experienced within the delayed judgement period shows little influence
relative to self-generated anchors even with commonly encountered targets (temperature) used in one
of the experiments, showing that anchoring effects may precede priming in duration especially when the
anchoring effects were formed during the task.[14] Further research to conclude an effect that is
effectively retained over a substantial period of time has proven inconsistent.[15][16][17]
[edit]
Given the old saying that 'Two Heads are Better than One', it is often presumed that groups come to a
more unbiased decision relative to individuals.[18] However, this assumption is supported with varied
findings that could not come to a general consensus.[19][20][21][22][23][24][25] Nevertheless, while some groups
are able to perform better than an individual member, they are found to be just as biased or even more
biased relative to their individual counterparts.[20][21] A possible cause would be the discriminatory
fashion in which information is communicated, processed and aggregated based on each individual's
anchored knowledge and belief.[26][27][28][29] This results in a diminished quality in the decision-making
process and consequently, amplifies the pre-existing anchored biases.
The cause of group anchoring remains obscure. Group anchors may have been established at the group
level or may simply be the culmination of several individual's personal anchors. Previous studies have
shown that when given an anchor before the experiment, individual members consolidated the
respective anchors to attain a decision in the direction of the anchor placed.[30] However, a distinction
between individual and group-based anchor biases does exist, with groups tending to ignore or disregard
external information due to the confidence in the joint decision-making process.[31][32][33] The presence of
pre-anchor preferences also impeded the extent to which external anchors affected the group decision,
as groups tend to allocate more weight to self-generated anchors, according to the 'competing anchor
hypothesis'.[34][30]
Recently, it has been suggested that the group member who speaks first often has an unproportionally
high impact on the final decision.[35][36]
A series of experiments were conducted to investigate anchoring bias in groups and possible solutions to
avoid or mitigate anchoring. The first experiment established that groups are indeed influenced by
anchors while the other two experiments highlighted methods to overcome group anchoring bias.
Utilized methods include the use of process accountability[37][38] and motivation through competition
instead of cooperation[39] to reduce the influence of anchors within groups.
Business intelligence
[edit]
A peer-reviewed study[40] sought to investigate the effect of business intelligence (BI) systems on the
anchoring effect. Business intelligence denotes an array of software and services used by businesses to
gather valuable insights into an organisation's performance.[41] The extent to which cognitive bias is
mitigated by using these systems was the overarching question in this study. While the independent
variable was the use of the BI system, the dependent variable was the outcome of the decision-making
process. The subjects were presented with a 'plausible' anchor and a 'spurious' anchor in a forecasting
decision. It was found that, while the BI system mitigated the negative effects of the spurious anchor, it
had no influence on the effects of the plausible anchor. This is important in a business context, because it
shows that humans are still susceptible to cognitive biases, even when using sophisticated technological
systems. One of the subsequent recommendations from the experimenters was to implement a
forewarning into BI systems as to the anchoring effect.
Causes
[edit]
Several theories have been put forth to explain what causes anchoring, and although some explanations
are more popular than others, there is no consensus as to which is best.[42] In a study on possible causes
of anchoring, two authors described anchoring as easy to demonstrate, but hard to explain.[10] At least
one group of researchers has argued that multiple causes are at play, and that what is called "anchoring"
is actually several different effects.[43]
Anchoring-and-adjusting
[edit]
In their original study, Tversky and Kahneman put forth a view later termed anchoring-as-adjustment.
According to this theory, once an anchor is set, people adjust away from it to get to their final answer;
however, they adjust insufficiently, resulting in their final guess being closer to the anchor than it would
be otherwise.[44] Other researchers also found evidence supporting the anchoring-and-adjusting
explanation.[45] Factors that influence the capacity for judgmental correction, like alcohol intoxication and
performing a taxing cognitive load (rehearsing a long string of digits in working memory) tend to increase
anchoring effects.[46] If people know the direction in which they should adjust, incentivizing accuracy also
appears to reduce anchoring effects.[47]
This model is not without its critiques. Proponents of alternative theories have criticized this model,
claiming it is only applicable when the initial anchor is outside the range of acceptable answers. To use
an earlier example, since Mahatma Gandhi obviously did not die at age 9, then people will adjust from
there. If a reasonable number were given, though, there would be no adjustment. Therefore, this theory
cannot, according to its critics, explain all cases of anchoring effect.[48]
Selective accessibility
[edit]
An alternate explanation regarding selective accessibility is derived from a theory called "confirmatory
hypothesis testing". In short, selective accessibility proposes that when given an anchor, a judge (i.e. a
person making some judgment) will evaluate the hypothesis that the anchor is a suitable answer.
Assuming it is not, the judge moves on to another guess, but not before accessing all the relevant
attributes of the anchor itself. Then, when evaluating the new answer, the judge looks for ways in which
it is similar to the anchor, resulting in the anchoring effect.[48] Various studies have found empirical
support for this hypothesis.[5] This explanation assumes that the judge considers the anchor to be a
plausible value so that it is not immediately rejected, which would preclude considering its relevant
attributes.[48] For example, an online-experiment showed that ratings of previous members of the crowd
could act as an anchor. When displaying the results of previous ratings in the context of business model
idea evaluation, people incorporate the displayed anchor into their own decision-making process,
leading to a decreasing variance of ratings.[49]
Attitude change
[edit]
More recently, a third explanation of anchoring has been proposed concerning attitude change.
According to this theory, providing an anchor changes someone's attitudes to be more favorable to the
particular attributes of that anchor, biasing future answers to have similar characteristics as the anchor.
Leading proponents of this theory consider it to be an alternate explanation in line with prior research
on anchoring-and-adjusting and selective accessibility.[50][51]
Extremeness aversion
[edit]
When estimating something, predictors are less likely to choose an amount which is close to the
maximum allowable adjustment. According to the theory, predictors adjust less when an anchor is
chosen. As a result, the final prediction results are close to the anchor. [52]
Influencing factors
[edit]
Mood
[edit]
A wide range of research has linked sad or depressed moods with more extensive and accurate
evaluation of problems.[53] As a result of this, earlier studies hypothesized that people with more
depressed moods would tend to use anchoring less than those with happier moods. However, more
recent studies have shown the opposite effect: sad people are more likely to use anchoring than people
with happy or neutral mood.[54] In a study focusing on medical practitioners, it was found that physicians
that possess positive moods are less susceptible to anchoring bias, when compared to physicians with
neutral moods. This was specifically found to be because a positive mood leads to information
processing that is more systematic which leads to more efficient problem solving. This leads to a
decreased anchoring effect.[55]
Personal Experience
[edit]
Early research found that experts (those with high knowledge, experience, or expertise in some field)
were more resistant to the anchoring effect. However, anchoring happens unconsciously which means
that unless someone who is knowledgeable is warned prior, they are still susceptible to anchoring.
[11]
Since then, however, numerous studies have demonstrated that while experience can sometimes
reduce the effect, even experts are susceptible to anchoring. In a study concerning the effects of
anchoring on judicial decisions, researchers found that even experienced legal professionals were
affected by anchoring. This remained true even when the anchors provided were arbitrary and unrelated
to the case in question.[56] Also, this relates to goal setting, where more experienced individuals will set
goals based on their past experiences which consequently affects end results in negotiations.[57]
Expertise is when a judge has relevant knowledge. In a study using price estimation of cars, it was found
that relevant knowledge positively influenced anchoring. Expertise in cognitive bias is related to
experience however the two are not exclusively exhaustive. In a study using stock return estimates, it
was found that expertise decreases behavioural bias significantly. It was found that other factors like
cognitive ability and experience where there is no susceptibility to anchoring or a susceptibility as it
increases, tend to become factors that decrease the effects of anchoring when they are an expert.[58]
Personality
[edit]
Research has correlated susceptibility to anchoring with most of the Big Five personality traits. The
personality traits are as follows. Conscientiousness, a character that is orderly and responsible.
Neuroticism, an individual that has an uneasy nature and is unstable. Extraversion which is where a
person is sociable and outgoing in nature. Openness to experience which is an intelligent and creative
personality trait. Agreeableness, where someone is polite and trusting. People high in agreeableness and
conscientiousness and neuroticism are more likely to be affected by anchoring, while those high in
extraversion and openness to experience are less likely to be affected. Studies have shown that those
high in Openness to New Experiences and Agreeableness are more susceptible to anchoring.[59][60]
Cognitive ability
[edit]
The impact of cognitive ability on anchoring is contested. A recent study on willingness to pay for
consumer goods found that anchoring decreased in those with greater cognitive ability, though it did not
disappear.[61] Another study, however, found that cognitive ability had no significant effect on how likely
people were to use anchoring.[62] In a poker-like experiment that included people of differing academic
achievement and psychometric reasoning scoring, it has been found that anchoring is not related to
education level. It also found that numerical reasoning and reflection scores had a negative association
with anchoring susceptibility.[63]
Overconfidence
[edit]
Although overconfidence emanates from the heuristic and refers more specifically to a behavioural
tendency to take their initial assessment and put more emphasis on it during making their initial
assessment leading to cognitive conceit.[64] Cognitive conceit or overconfidence arises from other factors
like personal cognitive attributes such as knowledge and decision-making ability, decreasing the
probability to pursue external sources of confirmation. This factor has also been shown to arise with
tasks with greater difficulty. Even within subject matter experts, they were also prey to such behaviour of
overconfidence and should more so, actively reduce such behaviour. Following the study of estimations
under uncertain, despite several attempts to curb overconfidence proving unsuccessful,[65] Tversky and
Kahneman (1971) suggest an effective solution to overconfidence is for subjects to explicitly establish
anchors to help reduce overconfidence in their estimates.[66]
Applications
[edit]
Anchoring in negotiation
[edit]
In the negotiation process anchoring serves to determine an accepted starting point for the subsequent
negotiations. As soon as one side states their first price offer, the (subjective) anchor is set. The
counterbid (counter-anchor) is the second-anchor.[67]
In addition to the initial research conducted by Tversky and Kahneman, multiple other studies have
shown that anchoring can greatly influence the estimated value of an object.[68] For instance, although
negotiators can generally appraise an offer based on multiple characteristics, studies have shown that
they tend to focus on only one aspect. In this way, a deliberate starting point can strongly affect the
range of possible counteroffers.[44] The process of offer and counteroffer results in a mutually beneficial
arrangement. However, multiple studies have shown that initial offers have a stronger influence on the
outcome of negotiations than subsequent counteroffers.[69]
An example of the power of anchoring has been conducted during the Strategic Negotiation Process
Workshops. During the workshop, a group of participants is divided into two sections: buyers and sellers.
Each side receives identical information about the other party before going into a one-on-one
negotiation. Following this exercise, both sides debrief about their experiences. The results show that
where the participants anchor the negotiation had a significant effect on their success.[70][page needed]
Anchoring affects everyone, even people who are highly knowledgeable in a field. Northcraft and Neale
conducted a study to measure the difference in the estimated value of a house between students and
real-estate agents. In this experiment, both groups were shown a house and then given different listing
prices. After making their offer, each group was then asked to discuss what factors influenced their
decisions. In the follow-up interviews, the real-estate agents denied being influenced by the initial price,
but the results showed that both groups were equally influenced by that anchor.[71]
Anchoring can have more subtle effects on negotiations as well. Janiszewski and Uy investigated the
effects of precision of an anchor. Participants read an initial price for a beach house, then gave the price
they thought it was worth. They received either a general, seemingly nonspecific anchor (e.g., $800,000)
or a more precise and specific anchor (e.g., $799,800). Participants with a general anchor adjusted their
estimate more than those given a precise anchor ($751,867 vs $784,671). The authors propose that this
effect comes from difference in scale; in other words, the anchor affects not only the starting value, but
also the starting scale. When given a general anchor of $20, people will adjust in large increments ($19,
$21, etc.), but when given a more specific anchor like $19.85, people will adjust on a lower scale ($19.75,
$19.95, etc.).[72] Thus, a more specific initial price will tend to result in a final price closer to the initial
one.
As for the question of setting the first or second anchor, the party setting the second anchor has the
advantage in that the counter-anchor determines the point midway between both anchors.[67] Due to a
possible lack of knowledge the party setting the first anchor can also set it too low, i.e. against their own
interests. Generally negotiators who set the first anchor also tend to be less satisfied with the
negotiation outcome, than negotiators who set the counter-anchor.[73] This may be due to the regret or
sense that they did not achieve or rather maximise the full potential of the negotiations. However,
studies suggest that negotiators who set the first offer frequently achieve economically more
advantageous results.[74]
[edit]
Tversky and Kahneman[75] suggest that the anchoring effect is the product of anchoring and adjustment
heuristics whereby estimates are made starting from an anchor value which is then adjusted in until the
individual has reached an answer. Kahneman suggests that anchoring occurs from derivations from
anchor-consistent knowledge. In their paper on anchoring bias[75] Kahneman and Tversky showed that
people judgements could be skewed either higher or lower when presented with random numbers
either high or low before their prediction.
Predrag Teovanović’s [76] study ‘Individual Differences in Anchoring Effect: Evidence for the Role of
Insufficient Adjustment’ analysed individual measures of individual differences like intelligence, cognitive
reflection and basic personality traits to find the effect the anchoring bias had on individual decisions.
Results found that there was a negative correlation between intelligence and anchoring in a group of
more reflective subjects, indicating that individual differences in cognitive processing has important
effects on the emergence of the anchoring effect. The experiment took 236 participants and graded
cognitive attributes like intelligence, cognitive reflection and personality traits.
A further study completed by Adrian Furnham, Hua Chu Boo and Alistair McClelland[77] asked participants
to complete four anchoring tasks with each task consisting of a higher or lower anchor. The participants
completed a personality test measuring cognitive processes as well as intelligence and individual
determinants like extroversion and introversion. Furnham found that there was a relationship between
high levels of conscientiousness and extraversion with anchoring biases.
Sebastian Schindler’s[78] study which analysed the significance of the anchoring effect against the 5
personality traits in a sample of 1000 participants found that there was evidence against the systematic
influence of personality traits on the susceptibility towards the anchoring effect. All manipulations in the
1000 participant study shifted the median response towards the anchor and evidence against a
systematic relationship.
Anchoring in pricing
[edit]
According to the theory, consumers’ shopping experiences are influenced by factors such as time
restriction and specific environment. Enterprises design would set anchor values for consumers in order
to get them to buy the products. When persuading consumers to purchase a particular product, sellers
might use anchoring. Sellers often influence consumers’ price perception by anchoring a high reference
price and that is an anchor value.[79] Following are three ways to set the anchor value for consumers.
[edit]
Sellers usually sort the prices of products from high to low and this method is common seen on the
menus of restaurants.[80] The high prices at the top of the menu act as anchor values in this situation.
Consumers will have an expectation that the products are all expensive when knowing the relatively high
prices of products on the top of the list. As a result, they will be pleased to see the cheaper products at
the middle and bottom of the list and regard these prices as acceptable or cheaper than expected.
Therefore, they are more likely to buy these products.[81]
Decoy
[edit]
Decoy effect is defined as a situation where people tend to have a change in preference between two
choices when they are showed with a third choice. The third choice is called a decoy which is designed to
induce consumers to change their preferences. The decoy is usually considered as inferior. For example,
it might be more expensive than option A while having lower quality than option B. In this case, the
anchor is the decoy.[82]
One decoy effect example is the bundle sales. For example, many restaurants often sell set meals to their
consumers, while simultaneously having the meals’ components sold separately. The prices of the meals’
components are the decoy pricing and act as an anchor which enables to make the set meal more
valuable to consumers. With the decoy effect it generates, the anchor increases consumers’ willingness
to pay for the set meals, or the mixed bundles.[83]
Incidental prices
[edit]
Incidental price is defined as the prices offered or showed by a seller for products which the consumers
are not interested in. According to the theory, the incidental price serves as an anchor which increases
consumers’ willingness to pay. This effect has been widely used in areas such as auctions, online vendors
and retailers.[84]
See also