Seeking The Causes of Behaviour: How Do We Attribute Causality, Why Is It Important?
Seeking The Causes of Behaviour: How Do We Attribute Causality, Why Is It Important?
tions and are likely to act in different ways from those who do not. Heider believed
that people are intuitive psychologists who construct causal theories of human Internal (or
behaviour, and because such theories have the same form as systematic scientific dispositional)
social psychological theories, people are actually intuitive or naive psychologists. attribution
Heider made a lasting distinction between personal factors (e.g. personality, abil Process of assigning
the cause of our own
ity) and environmental factors (e.g. situations, social pressure) in the way that we
or others’ behaviour to
account for the causes for behaviour. The former are examples of an internal (or internal or dispositional
dispositional) attribution and the latter of an external (or situational) attribution. factors.
So, for example, it might be useful to know whether someone you meet at a party
External (or
who seems aloof and distant is an aloof and distant person or is acting in that
situational) attribution
way because she is not enjoying that particular party. Heider believed that because Assigning the cause
internal causes, or intentions, are hidden from us, we can infer their presence only of our own or others’
if there are no clear external causes. However, as we see below, people tend to be behaviour to external or
biased in preferring internal to external attributions even in the face of evidence for environmental factors.
Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2017 – 9781488619106 – Vaughan & Hogg /Human Social Behaviour
Vaughan, G. (2017). Human social behaviour (custom edition). Pearson Education Australia.
Created from deakin on 2025-03-24 17:01:07.
46 Chapter 2 SOCIAL THINKING
Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2017 – 9781488619106 – Vaughan & Hogg /Human Social Behaviour
Vaughan, G. (2017). Human social behaviour (custom edition). Pearson Education Australia.
Created from deakin on 2025-03-24 17:01:07.
SEEKING THE CAUSES OF BEHAVIOUR 47
Internal External
Figure 2.4
Achievement attributions as a function of locus, stability and controllability.
How we attribute someone’s task achievement depends on:
• Locus – is the performance caused by the actor (internal) or the situation (external)?
• Stability – is the internal cause a stable or unstable one?
• Controllability – to what extent is future task performance under the actor’s control?
kept sneezing throughout, and he was sitting next to poor Helga. So let us look to
the future: in future examinations Bevan might not be present (an unstable factor),
or Helga could choose to sit well away from Bevan if he turns up (a controllable
factor). In total, there are eight different ways of explaining task performance.
Copyright © 2017. Pearson Education Australia. All rights reserved.
Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2017 – 9781488619106 – Vaughan & Hogg /Human Social Behaviour
Vaughan, G. (2017). Human social behaviour (custom edition). Pearson Education Australia.
Created from deakin on 2025-03-24 17:01:07.
48 Chapter 2 SOCIAL THINKING
Weiner’s model is a dynamic one, in that people first assess whether someone has
succeeded or failed and accordingly experience positive or negative emotion. They
then make a causal attribution for the performance; further, people can experience
specific emotions (e.g. pride for doing well due to ability) and expectations that
influence future performance.
Weiner’s model is relatively well supported by experiments that provide
participants with performance outcomes and locus, stability and controllability
information, often under role-playing conditions (e.g. de Jong, Koomen &
Mellenbergh, 1988). However, critics have suggested that the controllability dimen
sion may be less important than was first thought. They have also wondered to
what extent people outside controlled laboratory conditions really analyse achieve
ment in this way.
Self-perception
If you can attribute an act internally to a person’s disposition you now know some
thing about that person – his or her personality. Daryl Bem (1972) pinpointed an
Self-perception interesting implication of this in his self-perception theory. He argued that: (1) we
theory make attributions for our own behaviour in the same way as we make attributions for
Bem’s idea that we gain others’ behaviour; and (2) it is through internal attribution of our own behaviour that
knowledge of ourselves we gain knowledge about ourselves, our self-concept and identity (see Chapter 3).
only by making
self-attributions: for
example, we infer our Explaining our emotions
own attitudes from our Making attributions also plays a role in defining emotions. Our emotions have
own behaviour. two distinct components: a state of physiological arousal, and cognitions that we
use to label the arousal as an emotion, such as fear or excitement. Although the
arousal and label usually go hand-in-hand and our thoughts can generate the asso
ciated arousal, in some cases unexplained arousal could be experienced as different
emotions depending on what kind of attributions we make for what we are experi
encing. A major contributor to theory and research in this area is Stanley Schachter
Copyright © 2017. Pearson Education Australia. All rights reserved.
(1964; for a review of his work see Reisenzein, 1983). One of his experiments dealt
with ‘emotional lability’. See Box 2.3 and Figure 2.5 to see the components in the
process of attributing an emotion in this experiment.
Being emotionally labile can help in therapy. Valins and Nisbett (1972) won
dered if the process of making attributions could be used to treat emotional
disorders. For example, might someone who is chronically anxious learn to
re-label the arousal as happiness, transform depression into contentment, or
attribute shyness to external factors rather than their own social anxiety? While
some experiments suggest this could work (e.g. Olson, 1988), in general what
is a mis-attribution effect is limited to the laboratory, unreliable and short-lived
(Forsterling, 1988; Parkinson, 1985).
Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2017 – 9781488619106 – Vaughan & Hogg /Human Social Behaviour
Vaughan, G. (2017). Human social behaviour (custom edition). Pearson Education Australia.
Created from deakin on 2025-03-24 17:01:07.
SEEKING THE CAUSES OF BEHAVIOUR 49
In the late nineteenth century the famous psycholo- heart rate), but were not informed what the drug was
gist William James turned the usual account of how we or what would happen. The aim was to show that the
experience an emotion on its head. As ordinary folk, drug-induced arousal would be interpreted differently
we might believe that our mental images cause the according to the context, of which there were two.
body to react, and define our feelings as an emotion. In the first context, a confederate in the same room
However, James argued that first the body responds engaged in silly antics and made paper aeroplanes,
automatically to a stimulus, and then we interpret which led the volunteers to report feeling euphoric. In
our bodily responses on the basis of what is going on the second context, the confederate ripped up papers
around us: if we see a bear, we run, and a little later and stomped around the room, which led the students
our pounding heart tells us that we are afraid. to report feeling angry.
One of Stanley Schachter’s experiments dealing with Given that the arousal brought on by the drug was
‘emotional lability’ brought this idea into the laboratory unexpected, the confederate’s actions provided suf-
and gave it an attributional flavour. The key condition ficient cues to attach a label to what the volunteers
was one in which adrenalin was administered to male thought was actually an ‘emotion’.
volunteers causing them to feel aroused (an increase in
Arousal
Adrenalin
(increase in
injected
heart rate)
Figure 2.5
Attributing a likely cause to an experimentally induced emotion.
Source: Based on Schachter & Singer (1962).
Styles of attribution
We all engage in attributions, but it appears that we differ in our attributional Attributional style
style. According to the eminent clinical psychologist Julian Rotter (1966), those of An individual
us who are internals tend to make internal attributions; believing we have a great (personality)
deal of personal control over our destiny – things happen because we make them predisposition to
make a certain type of
happen. Those of us who are externals tend to make external attributions; believ
causal attribution for
ing that we have little control over what happens to us – things simply occur by behaviour.
chance, luck or the actions of powerful external agents. We can also differ in the
Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2017 – 9781488619106 – Vaughan & Hogg /Human Social Behaviour
Vaughan, G. (2017). Human social behaviour (custom edition). Pearson Education Australia.
Created from deakin on 2025-03-24 17:01:07.