10 1017@9781108780360 002-Merged
10 1017@9781108780360 002-Merged
Chapter Overview
The scholarship on intercultural communication is grounded in interdis-
ciplinary research in anthropology, communication studies, education,
linguistics, and, more recently, applied linguistics. Given this interdisci-
plinarity, it is useful to establish a shared understanding of how key
concepts such as culture and communication are used in this book.
Additionally, this chapter highlights the nature of culture as layered and
of communication as dynamically co-constructed by participants and the
social context in which their interaction takes place. Finally, the discussion
turns to prominent models of intercultural communication from a social-
scientific perspective, which offer fruitful points of analysis in language
pedagogy as well.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
10 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
[A] learned system of meanings that fosters a particular sense of shared
identity-hood and community-hood among its group members. It is a
complex frame of reference that consists of a pattern of traditions, beliefs,
values, norms, symbols, and meanings that are shared to varying degrees by
interacting members of an identity group. (p. 691; emphasis mine)
These definitions point to the cohesive power of culture, connecting
groups of individuals. Culture, in this sense, serves as a lens for making
sense of interactions with others, “a frame of reference for its members …
for making sense of the world” (Oetzel, 2009, p. 6). Yet, Spencer-Oatey
(2008) recognizes a duality in her definition of culture as “a fuzzy set of
basic assumptions and values, orientations to life, beliefs, policies, proce-
dures and behavioural conventions that are shared by a group of people and
that influence (but do not determine) each member’s behaviour and his/
her interpretations of the ‘meaning’ of other people’s behaviour” (p. 3).
That is, culture as a cohesive force helps individuals connect with each
other, but at the same time, these individuals have the freedom and
flexibility to adhere to shared culture practices in some ways and diverge
from them in others.
Layers of Culture
While cultural cohesion is grounded in shared experiences, culture is not
monolithic. Instead, cultures are layered constructs, and each individual
reflects affiliation with and is influenced by broader and narrower social
forces that shape our communicative practices. Broader cultural forces
might include supranational (beyond the nation) communities, such as
Doctors without Borders or Harry Potter fans around the globe. Nations –
the most common association with the term ‘culture’ – that determine
laws, as well as language and other social policies, also have a significant
impact on cultural practices. For example, it is usually the state that
determines procedures or traditions for electing, appointing or inheriting
leadership (e.g., democracies versus a monarchy), including verbal and
nonverbal communication that such procedures entail. Within each poli-
tically defined nation, there are mid-level cultural groups – e.g., based on
ethnic identity, social class, or geographic region – whose values and
practices may align more or less with dominant trends in the nation-state
(Bonvillain, 2020; Haslett, 2017; Myers-Scotton, 2006; Neuliep, 2018).
Religious groups that share linguistic rituals or an indigenous community
revitalizing its heritage language represent two examples of such mid-level
groups. Representing the next layer, smaller cultural groups influence
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
1 Key Concepts in Intercultural Communication 11
family-level coherence, building and maintaining traditions such as nam-
ing children, creating shared jokes, or celebrating symbols of a clan.
However, some practices are particular to an individual (e.g., idiosyncratic
use of emojis).
In his discussion of culture, Oetzel (2009) identifies four intercon-
nected levels, which are shown in Figure 1.1: (1) the individual (e.g.,
sense of self, personal attributes), (2) interpersonal relationships with
others (e.g., friendships, families), (3) organizations that scaffold every-
day life (e.g., healthcare, education, work, hobbies), and (4) broad
cultural forces (e.g., values, belief systems shared across larger society).
The outer layers influence each circle going inwards (top-down effects,
such as media informing individual preferences or behaviors), while the
inner circles may have outward impact (bottom-up effects, such as
leaders who affect local or world events).
Neuliep (2018) offers a similarly layered notion of contexts that shape
communication. In this model, communication is most broadly influenced
by the cultural context, defined as the “accumulated pattern of values,
beliefs, and behaviors shared by an identifiable group of people with a
broad cultural
context
organizations
& institutions
interpersonal
relationships
the individual
Figure 1.1 Layers of culture and impact (adapted from Oetzel, 2009).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
12 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
common history and verbal and nonverbal symbol systems” (p. 21). Within
broad cultural contexts exist microcultural contexts, the way smaller social
groups (e.g., ethnic groups) view and understand the world. The next layer
is the environmental context, referring to the ways in which the physical
location and immediate surroundings affect communication (e.g., whis-
pering during a religious ceremony but yelling at sports games). The most
immediate influence on interactions is each individual’s perceptual context,
“the individual characteristics of each interactant, including cognitions,
attitudes, dispositions, and motivations” (p. 22). Moreover, interactions
take place within sociorelational contexts, or “the relationship between the
interactants,” both familial and professional, such as friends, parents and
children, mentor and pupil (p. 22). It is within this context that inter-
actants use verbal and nonverbal communication to create and interpret
messages.
Oetzel’s layers and Neuliep’s contexts reflect multiple cultural and
individual factors that influence specific communicative events. These
factors impact our interactions in different ways at different times. What
is salient – relevant, impactful – varies across situations. For example,
whether an individual is shy or has a particular way of speaking, whether
the interlocutors are familiar with each other or not, and what each
person wants to achieve with the interaction plays an important role in
determining how an interaction progresses, and what its outcomes
might be.
Figure 1.2 illustrates how broader societal forces and highly individual,
local factors may converge: an American athlete, the captain of the U.S.
women’s soccer team1, will likely interact differently during training or at
an international press conference than she might with her friends at home;
she might also interact differently if she is stressed or has a disagreement on
a consequential issue, as opposed to discussing what movie to watch
Saturday night. In any of her interactions with others, a different aspect
of her identity might be most salient: her being on the American as
opposed to the German team may influence her attitude towards winning;
her being the captain of the team versus another player determines who
represents the team to the media; her being with friends rather than
strangers affects her language choices, her verbal and nonverbal behaviors.
When analyzing interactions, it is important to understand how various
contextual and cultural forces interplay to shape an interaction, and how
participants consequently enact their social personae, or identities, through
1
www.ussoccer.com/womens-national-team#tab-1
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
1 Key Concepts in Intercultural Communication 13
Coarse-Grained Factors
Americans Germans
The American
Other American
women’s soccer
women athletes
team
Fine-Grained Factors
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
14 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
language. It is equally important to understand the role that socialization
plays in the development of these identities (Monaghan & Goodman,
2007).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
1 Key Concepts in Intercultural Communication 15
Communication is socially constituted, rooted in social relationships and
produced in the conduct of social life … not only is communication socially
constituted but society is communicatively constituted, produced and
reproduced by communicative acts … the communicative forms and prac-
tices of a society ~ its ways of speaking, dressing, dancing, playing music,
and so on – are social means that are available to members for the accom-
plishment of social ends. (pp. 25–26)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
16 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
In this volume, communication is viewed as an intercultural and layered
process that reflects the broadest range of experiences individuals have: some
people traverse cultural boundaries and shift between cultural selves, others
meld cultural identities or choose not to adopt or adapt to another culture.
In order to learn about this process, a few key concepts need to be clarified
for establishing a shared understanding of intercultural communication.
2
www.npr.org/news/specials/nsawiretap/legality.html
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
1 Key Concepts in Intercultural Communication 17
PARTICIPANTS
Speakers Hearers/Listeners
Bystanders Eavesdroppers
interacting with her patient. These roles are often limited in the language
classroom but are at the heart of meaning-making in real-world commu-
nication. Therefore, L2/Lx learning must shift to provide opportunities for
creative and diverse language use as a living expression of the self
(Shohamy, 2007).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
18 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
Vocabulary
Cultural
Knowledge
Grammar
Nonverbal
Communication
Pragmatics
Paralinguistic Features
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
1 Key Concepts in Intercultural Communication 19
English to fill in a gap in speech, which indicates that the speaker is not yet
finished with his or her turn).
An important, and in L2/Lx pedagogy much understudied, component
of communication is nonverbal communication. Facial expressions, hand
gestures, body movements (kinesics), touch (haptics), personal space
(proxemics), time (chronemics), and our appearance (e.g., clothing or
hairstyle) all contribute to meaning, either alone or in combination with
verbal language.
Finally, cultural knowledge is crucial in intercultural communication.
Our knowledge of the world – history, past experiences, current issues or
everyday practices in our own and other cultures – helps us participate in
the social/cultural groups to which we belong (e.g., how to contribute in a
university seminar). Such knowledge also activates interpretive frameworks
that allow us to make sense of intercultural communication if they are
similar between cultures or hinder communication if they are dissimilar.
Theoretical models of intercultural communication, which we discuss
next, can provide useful frameworks for organizing the aspects of commu-
nication we discussed in this section.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
20 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
Table 1.1 Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck’s Values Orientation model
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
1 Key Concepts in Intercultural Communication 21
nature: rivers are rerouted and the Arctic is opened up for oil drilling3. The
human-over-nature paradigm reflects most Western countries since the
onset of the Enlightenment (Vining, Merrick, & Price, 2008). A nature-
over-human (biocentric) orientation, on the other hand, would accept
unlimited births (Martin & Nakayama, 2018), prefer organic food, or
engage in environmental activism. Cultures in which nature and people
are viewed as being in harmony with one another – neither dominant over
the other (e.g., adapting to nature by building homes on stilts) – include
many Native American societies and most Japanese people (Martin &
Nakayama, 2018).
Human relationships are also guided by value systems. Individualistic
societies (like many communities in the United States and Western
Europe) prefer individuals to take full responsibility for their actions
(e.g., financial situation), while hierarchical societies might be more likely
to see the state (or a patriarch or matriarch) as being responsible for the
well-being of its citizens (family or community members) (Hills, 2002). In
a collaterally oriented culture, members would make decisions with equal
weight and reach consensus, stressing “role obligations and in-group
interdependence, kinship bonds, and extended family bonds” (Ting-
Toomey, 2018, p. 194).
Doing, being-in-becoming, and being are the three possible responses to
motivations. The United States, by and large, is doing-oriented: it is not
enough to make it to the Olympic Games, it is crucial to earn a medal, lest
the country’s performance be called ‘lousy’ (Paine, 2018). A being-in-
becoming orientation may be difficult to measure in surveys, since personal
growth can mean myriads of possible paths that are important to a unique
human being. According to Martin and Nakayama (2018), “This orienta-
tion seems to be less prevalent than the other two, perhaps practiced only in
Zen Buddhism” (p. 101). Arguably, however, artists who wish to hone their
skills to meet their own aspirations towards a goal they had set for
themselves would also reflect this orientation. The third orientation –
being (expressive/emotional self-presentation) – emphasizes social struc-
tures and relationships (e.g., Chinese, Japanese, or Arab cultures, with the
obvious caveat that there is variation in each of these broad cultural labels).
In such cultures, an individual’s birth, family connections, or age would
outweigh their personal actions: “what [a person] is” carries greater sig-
nificance than “what he does” (Okabe, 1983, p. 24).
3
www.sciencemag.org/news/2018/04/trump-administration-takes-first-steps-toward-drilling-alaska-
s-arctic-refuge
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
22 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
Future, present, and past orientations form the basis of the final question
in Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck’s analytic model. Cultures that focus on the
future are likely to set out goals they want to meet in five or ten years, set up
appointments months or years in advance, and focus on saving up vacation
days or money for future adventures (e.g., the Boy Scout motto “Always be
prepared,” Canada and the United States at the national level). A present
orientation focuses on taking advantage of and enjoying the here and now
(e.g., Spain or Greece). As Kluckhohn (1953) herself observed, present-
oriented cultures “pay little attention to what has happened in the past, and
regard the future as a vague and most unpredictable period” (p. 348).
Cultures with past orientation, in contrast, emphasize tradition, a connec-
tion to the past, and the experience that older members of society can
contribute. Many European, Central and South American, and Asian
countries reflect this value orientation.
According to Hills (2002), Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck proposed a sixth
value orientation, space, which they did not know how to investigate at the
time of their original research. Hills himself suggested expanding the
original model with four further dimensions (quotes from p. 10):
1. Space: “Should space belong to individuals, to groups (especially the
family) or to everybody?”
2. The nature of work: “What should be the basic motivation for work?
To make a contribution to society, to have a sense of personal achieve-
ment, or to attain financial security?”
3. The relationship between genders: “How should society distribute
roles, power and responsibility between the genders?” (power and
responsibility to men, women, or equally to both)
4. The relationship between individuals and the state: “Should precedent
right and responsibility be accorded the nation or the individual?”
(individuals take full responsibility for themselves, with no support
from the state; the state provides full support to all members; the
individual and the state share rights and responsibilities in reasonable
proportions).
Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck were surprised by the relative similarities
among cultures around the world (five dimensions, fifteen value orienta-
tions), while at the same time, there was much diversity among cultures as
well (the permutations of five dimensions and fifteen orientations yield 243
possible variants). It is important to note that Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck
did not elevate any orientations above the others; differences were seen as
differences, not ‘right’ or ‘wrong’ ways of understanding the world,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
1 Key Concepts in Intercultural Communication 23
behaving, or being. Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck’s model is now almost sixty
years old and reflects their search for universal categories similar to the
contemporary foci in linguistics. Their model is also limited by its ground-
ing in Western value categories. Nonetheless, many of its tenets continue
to drive intercultural communication training in various professional
development arenas, therefore they can be useful entry points to investi-
gating cultural beliefs, products, and practices.
1. Individualism – Collectivism
In individualistic societies (e.g., the United States, Australia), the priority is given to the
needs of an individual, with little emphasis on group cohesion or the interests of a larger
social unit. In contrast, in collectivistic cultures (e.g., Ecuador, Taiwan), there is less
emphasis on individual achievement and wants and more on relationships and loyalty
(i.e., accomplishment is less important than membership in particular social groups).
Communication is more direct in individualistic cultures, while it may be mediated by
third parties in collectivist cultures.
2. High power distance – Low power distance
Although all cultures experience some degree of inequality, in high power distance
cultures, there is significant inequality between more and less powerful members. These
cultures tend to be very hierarchical, both at the family level and in society at large (e.g.,
Malaysia, Panama). Conversely, cultures with low power distance are comparatively
egalitarian, with little inequality between members of society. At work, supervisors are
more likely to accept questions and suggestions; at home, children are involved in
decision-making (e.g., New Zealand, Denmark).
3. Masculinity – Femininity
This dimension does not label countries masculine or feminine, rather it describes the
extent to which gender roles are distinguished from each other. More masculine
cultures (e.g., Japan, Austria) tend to have more rigidly assigned gender roles (e.g.,
women stay at home, men work; women perform low-level tasks, men are in positions
of power). Commensurately, traditional masculine attributes of assertiveness,
competitiveness, and material success are highly valued in such cultures. In contrast,
more feminine cultures (e.g., Sweden, Costa Rica) have fewer gender-specific roles and
reflect flexible ideas about work that men and women can undertake. These cultures
value characteristics that were traditionally associated with women: interpersonal
relationships, taking care of those in need, nurturing, or moderated ambition.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
24 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
(cont.)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
1 Key Concepts in Intercultural Communication 25
Hofstede’s model has been criticized for its reductionist view of culture
and its propensity to emphasize stereotypical views of culture (cf. Gerhart
& Fan, 2005; McSweeney, 2002). Nonetheless, these dimensions merit
discussion, since his model is still used to guide intercultural communica-
tion training in corporate cultures, when employees are relocated to an
office abroad or when diplomats are prepared for assignment. The dimen-
sions can serve as useful tools to analyze intercultural communication, as
long as we keep a few points in mind. First, each of these dimensions is a
continuum, not a binary category. Countries can reflect degrees of power
distance, distributed between the two distal points at the ends of each
continuum. Second, Hofstede’s model examines national patterns, central
tendencies, of beliefs and behaviors. There is little connection to individual
variation, and applying country-level averages to understand personal
preferences and choices is misleading (Oetzel, 2009; Spencer-Oatey &
Franklin, 2009). As Gudykunst (2004) observes, national-level trends can
have an impact on individual beliefs and behaviors by influencing social
norms, rules, and how members of a culture are socialized. However,
individuals may differ significantly from country averages on any dimen-
sion or on all of them (Kirkman, Lowe, & Gibson, 2006; Oetzel, 2009).
Third, Hofstede’s analyses do not take into consideration potential regio-
nal differences, such as between the wealthier areas in Northern Italy
(where Hofstede collected data) and less affluent Southern regions
(Nguyen-Phuong-Mai, 2017). However, we can use these dimensions to
study the behaviors of specific interlocutors in intercultural communication.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
26 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
(cont.)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
1 Key Concepts in Intercultural Communication 27
1. Knowledge:
a. Cultural self-awareness: The ability to recognize the complexity of
one’s own cultural practices and biases and how one’s experiences
shape these rules.
b. Knowledge of cultural worldview frameworks: The ability to
understand the complexity of another culture, such as its history,
values, communication styles, and so on.
2. Skills
a. Empathy: The ability to interpret intercultural experiences from
multiple perspectives, including one’s own cultural framework, as
well as that of another cultural group.
b. Verbal and nonverbal communication: The ability to interpret
and participate in interaction successfully, using culturally appro-
priate verbal and nonverbal communication (e.g., preferred phy-
sical contact, directness, or indirectness).
3. Attitudes
a. Curiosity: The ability and willingness to ask complex questions
about other cultures, recognizing that answers to these questions
vary, reflecting multiple perspectives.
b. Openness: The ability and willingness to interact with people
from diverse cultural backgrounds, suspending judgment limited
by one’s own cultural beliefs and practices.
This framework recognizes the importance of intercultural commu-
nicative competence in a globally interconnected world. It also puts
culture at the center of transformative learning. Thus, the rubric
offers “a systematic way to measure our capacity to identify our
own cultural patterns, compare and contrast them with others, and
adapt empathically and flexibly to unfamiliar ways of being”
(www.aacu.org/value/rubrics/intercultural-knowledge). While the
faculty experts acknowledge that intercultural communicative
competence is more complex than the components of the rubric,
this matrix is a valuable way to conceptualize and understand the
knowledge, attitudes, and skills required for becoming an intercultu-
rally competent individual.
Summary
Since the study of intercultural communication first began in the 1950s, it
has been dominated by the social sciences, such as anthropology,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
28 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
communication, and sociology (Spencer-Oatey & Franklin, 2009).
Consequently, it has typically examined culture under a broad-lens micro-
scope: What do Americans do in certain situations? What do the Japanese
do differently? More recent work views interactions as multi-layered and
shaped by broader cultural practices as well as individual and context-
specific factors. Culture itself is a dynamic, multifaceted experience, and
analyses must account for changes within and between cultures and the
individuals who embody them (Oetzel, 2009).
In professional and personal spheres nowadays, it is essential to
learn to communicate effectively with others whose cultural and
linguistic backgrounds differ from ours. When we learn other lan-
guages, we are presented with a unique opportunity to reflect upon
our own multilayered cultural selves and practices, while we learn how
to make sense of and participate in intercultural communication. To
further this goal, the present volume reconceptualizes components of
language as essential tools in Lx learners’ intercultural communicative
repertoire, since “language is fundamental to intercultural compe-
tence” (Byram & Masuhara, 2013, p. 142).
1. What culture(s) do you belong to? How did you learn about the
practices, behaviors, and values of that/those culture(s)? Which practices
do you participate in, and which values do you share? Which practices do
you choose not to participate in, and which values do you not
share? Why?
2. How might your cultural affiliations change in different situations?
What factors influence your sense of cultural belonging?
3. Who determines what is ‘appropriate’ – in terms of beliefs and
practices – in a cultural group? What are some top-down and
bottom-up effects that may effect change in this group (see
discussions regarding Oetzel and Neuliep)?
4. Keep a journal for a day and note the different social personae you
inhabit in various interactions. What cultural affiliations are salient in
these interactions? How do you know? How does your communication
change to reflect various social identities?
5. Select a model of intercultural communication and analyze one of the
cultural groups you belong to (e.g., nation, hobby, family). Afterwards,
reflect on what practices, beliefs, and values were easy or difficult to
identify and why.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
1 Key Concepts in Intercultural Communication 29
6. Using the same model, analyze a cultural group that you are at least
somewhat familiar with but do not belong to. Afterwards, consider what
dimensions of the model were easy or difficult to identify. What kinds of
knowledge would you need to develop to be able to analyze this cultural
group more thoroughly, effectively, and accurately?
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:37:07, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.002
chapter 2
Chapter Overview
Our understanding of language learning and teaching, and what role we
believe the broader social context plays in the L2/Lx learning process, have
changed drastically over the past 100 years. This chapter offers a brief
review of key developments in language pedagogy, from the grammar
translation method to the communicative approach and beyond. This
review lays the groundwork for teaching languages with an intercultural
communication orientation, which is discussed in the second half of this
chapter and explored in depth in the pedagogical chapters in this volume.
30
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 31
• spoken language was the primary mode of communication,
• the process of L2 learning was similar to L1 acquisition,
• correct pronunciation was essential,
• L2 learning should reflect real-world language use,
• grammar should be taught implicitly, and
• the target language should be taught without relying on learners’ L1
(Richards & Rodgers, 2014).
This method required native or near-native levels of language knowl-
edge of instructors and the ability to teach without a textbook or coherent
syllabus. The learning process was also lengthy (children take years to
develop good language skills in their L1) and did not utilize cognitive
advantages that adult speakers could bring to the table, such as the ability
to analyze texts, to draw on linguistic and cultural background knowledge
or a well-formed L1.
The next major shift in L2 teaching occurred in the middle of the
twentieth century, when World War II and the Cold War increased the
need for communication across languages and cultures. Much of this
change was driven by the demand for translators and interpreters, code-
breakers and other specialized linguistic personnel in the military.
Influenced by contemporary psychological theory (behaviorism), language
learning was seen as habit formation: practice reinforced knowledge in
ways that “minimized the chances of producing mistakes” (Richards &
Rodgers, 2014, p. 26). Behaviorism informed the Audiolingual Method,
which used dialogic pattern drills focusing on structural components of
language (e.g., phonemes, morphemes). While this pedagogy aimed to
foster primarily spoken communication, successful L2 learning required
exceptional motivation and availability (lessons and practice could last
10–12 hours a day), and the ability to extrapolate from memorized chunks
of language to communicative contexts beyond the textbook dialogs
(Shrum & Glisan, 2015).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
32 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
endless variations of language due to the brain’s innate ability to organize
information – form rules – via abstract mental processing (Gass, Behney, &
Plonsky, 2013). In addition to reconceptualizing the way humans learn
language, Chomsky (1965) also proposed the notion of language competence,
the knowledge that native speakers are capable of applying to the comprehen-
sion and production of language. This construct was based on the abilities of
an ideal speaker-listener in a completely homogeneous speech community,
who knows his language perfectly and is unaffected by such grammatically
irrelevant conditions as memory limitation, distractions, shifts of attention
and interest, and errors (random or characteristic) in applying his knowl-
edge of the language in actual performance. (p. 3)
Chomsky contrasted competence with performance, the actual produc-
tion of language, during which humans are prone to making mistakes, such
as hesitations, false starts, and grammatical or other mishaps. As
a theoretical linguist, Chomsky did not intend to influence second lan-
guage learning, but his work had a long-lasting impact on the way we
understand second language acquisition (SLA).
Consequently, cognitively oriented scholarship in SLA research has
sought to understand the nature of knowledge that learners acquire, how
it responds to environmental stimulation (e.g., input types or instructional
intervention), and what they need to know to complete language tasks
(Doughty, 2003; Gass et al., 2013; Tóth & Davin, 2016). Learners’ perfor-
mance is typically compared to that of idealized or highly educated native
speakers, mostly on lexico-grammatical features of language (cf. Gass et al.,
2013). The resultant language pedagogy presents and practices the L2 via
activities that break down information into manageable chunks. Input-
presentation draws learners’ attention to target features, while negotiation
of meaning during interaction and error correction facilitates the mental
restructuring and solidification of knowledge. Learning is measured in
terms of linguistic output, with a primary focus on form and quantifiable
effects of pedagogical intervention (Atkinson, 2013; Ellis, 2001a, b; van
Patten & Williams, 2015). Missing from cognitively oriented pedagogy is
the role that the social context plays in learning.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 33
centrality of human-to-human interaction required for learning
(Vygotsky, 1978). While Vygotsky’s theory focused on learning in general,
it has been embraced in recent SLA scholarship as well. The basis of this
theory is that “humans use symbolic artifacts to establish an indirect, or
mediated, relationship between ourselves and the world” (Lantolf, 2001,
p. 1) and that human social and mental activity is culturally organized.
Accordingly, sociocultural theory considers learning to be prompted by the
social environment as a result of goal-directed intellectual and practical
activities (Hall & Walsh, 2002), and language is viewed as one of the
semiotic tools whose primary role is to shape and reflect cultural practices,
which are rooted in sociocultural and historical contexts (Muto, 2011).
Additionally, sociocultural theory views L2 learning as a process of
increasing autonomy, first directed by another, perhaps an expert, then
by the self (di Donato, 1994). This moves learners along the zone of
proximal development, which is the difference between the actual develop-
mental level of individuals at any moment and the potential skill they can
reach with the help of a more advanced interactant (Lantolf, 2011; Sato &
Viveros, 2016). L2 learning, then, requires social interaction, such as story-
telling or chatting, ideally with native speakers, highlighting the external
source of learning (Aimin, 2013). As Tóth and Davin (2016) note, “While
cognitive accounts envision an ‘inside-out’ process, where internal devel-
opment affects externalizable behavior, social perspectives emphasize an
‘outside-in’ process where engagement with the environment affects inter-
nal thoughts and activity” (p. 154). A more recent theoretical approach
bridges cognitive and social perspectives, integrating them as a unified
system.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
34 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
is presented in social contexts, and learners’ ability to use language inde-
pendently comes from related language experiences. Instructors can model
language use and provide guided practice, which steps should enable
learners to use the L2 in new and innovative ways (van Lier, 2004, 2008).
Explicit instruction is possible, as long as it directs learners’ attention to
specific grammatical, lexical, and content knowledge that they need for
accomplishing particular communicative goals. Communication with
others can be an effective source of social interaction, as can any artifact
(e.g., maps, movies) “that promotes interactive engagement with/in L2
environments” (Atkinson, 2013, p. 8).
Moreover, activities must engender a genuine exchange of information
among learners or between a source and the learner, engaging learners both
cognitively and socially (Atkinson, 2011, 2013). According to Tóth and
Davin (2016) three principles guide sociocognitively oriented language
teaching:
• Exercises should lead learners from more controlled language use
(modeling and guided practice) to independent and interactive self-
expression, using the L2 purposefully.
• Learning activities should reflect the symbolic complexity of real-world
language, both to interpret sources of input and to produce the L2 for
self-expression.
• Learners should gain “meaningful experience with knowledgeable
others” (p. 163), in order to develop the interdependent cognitive and
social processes necessary for successful L2 use.
Thus, according to sociocognitive theory, language is a social tool used
in situated activity to achieve social goals. Such a view would seem to be in
natural alignment with the notion of communicative competence,
a concept that has purportedly been behind L2 pedagogy in the past five
decades, although not always successfully. However, just as views of
language teaching have shifted over time, so have conceptualizations of
the competences learners need to develop in order to participate in inter-
cultural interactions.
Communicative Competence
In addition to new developments in cognitive psychology, the 1970s
witnessed the emergence of innovative theories in the social sciences as
well. While these new, socially oriented theories focused on L1 language use
and socialization, they were also very influential for L2 pedagogy, especially
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 35
the contrast between Hymes’ (1972) notion of communicative competence
and Chomsky’s view of cognitive competence. Hymes’ model of commu-
nicative competence recognized the importance of knowing how linguistic
elements of the language work (e.g., the vocabulary and linguistic struc-
tures), what sociolinguistic rules determine effective language use in inter-
action with others, and the social meaning of routine phrases (e.g., ‘I
hereby declare’) that carry specific meaning if uttered under specific con-
ditions, but sound strange if spoken by a random person.
Thus, instead of an abstract set of rules and patterns, Hymes’ framework
emphasized the relationship between the linguistic code and how it is used
to communicate ideas. According to this framework, the context in which
interaction takes place also determines what is ‘correct’ and what is not. As
Myers-Scotton (2006) states, communicative competence is the “ability to
recognize and use unmarked [i.e., commonly expected] ways of speaking in
a particular interaction – vary[ing] our way of speaking, depending on who
our audience is” (p. 29; emphasis mine).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
36 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
1. Grammatical competence: knowledge of the linguistic system, such
as grammar or the lexicon, including knowing how to use cases and
genders, conjugate verbs or put words in the right order.
• Example: I went to the store describes an event that happened in the
past, and bought apples describes multiple items.
2. Sociolinguistic competence: knowledge of social rules that guide
language use, including when to say what to whom, and what is
appropriate or not appropriate language in particular situations.
• Example: cul8er is appropriate for sending a text message (‘see you
later’) and Good morning, Professor for greeting an instructor during
office hours.
3. Discourse competence: knowledge of the way language fits together to
form entire texts, influenced by the cultural context and bound by
cultural practice.
• Example: Once upon a time . . . denotes the beginning of a fairy tale;
therefore indicates the consequence of an event or action.
4. Strategic competence: knowledge of skills that speakers use to initiate,
maintain, redirect, close, or repair interactions.
• Example: using a synonym for a forgotten word; asking for help;
using filler words (um . . .) to keep control of the conversation while
the speaker collects her thoughts.
Expanding on the work of Canale and Swain, Savignon (1997) suggested
that each of the four components of communicative competence are
emphasized differently at various levels of L2 proficiency. Specifically,
learners are initially likely to rely more on communication strategies to
fill in gaps of linguistic knowledge, such as using gestures or asking for
help. They may also draw from their understanding of the world, in terms
of sociocultural knowledge. However, at this stage, they have less control
over linguistic or discourse knowledge. Conversely, more proficient speak-
ers can draw on increasingly refined grammatical and sociolinguistic
sophistication to participate in broader varieties of discourses and use
communication strategies to complement their utterances rather than in
lieu of them.
In 1990 Bachman developed a more detailed model of communicative
competence, which was both similar to and different from Canale
and Swain’s original design. He separated language competence into
two main branches: organizational and pragmatic competence.
Organizational competence is comprised of grammatical and textual
competence (see Figure 2.1).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 37
• vocabulary
grammatical • morphology
competence • syntax
• phonology/graphology
organizational
competence
• cohesion
textual • rhetorical &
competence conversational
organization
• ideational functions
illocutionary • manipulative functions
competence • heuristic functions
• imaginative functions
pragmatic
competence
• sensitivity to dialects and
sociolinguistic language varieties
competence • sensitivity to registers
• sensitivity to natural and
idiomatic expressions
• cultural references and
figures of speech
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
38 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
That is, knowing “when to speak, when not, and . . . what to talk about
with whom, when, where, in what manner” (Hymes, 1972, p. 277).
Sociolinguistic competence includes four subskills. The first one pertains
to learners’ ability to distinguish and use different varieties of a language,
such as regional or social dialects, in contextually appropriate ways.
The second skill requires learners to identify and use the spoken or
written, formal or informal register appropriate for a particular interac-
tional context, knowing how to “formulate or interpret an utterance
which is not only linguistically accurate, but which is also phrased
in . . . a nativelike way . . . by speakers . . . who are native to the culture
of that dialect or variety” (Bachman, 1990, p. 97). The third subskill
requires learners to develop a sensitivity to natural and idiomatic expres-
sions. For example, they have to learn when and how to use phrases such
as three strikes, you’re out (a reference to baseball and a third missed strike
at a ball) in American English or Njia ya mwongo ni fupi1 (‘The route of
a liar is short ~ do not lie’) in KiSwahili. Interpretation of figures of
speech, such as the hyperbolic expression that weighs a ton for something
heavy also falls into this category. Finally, a large number of references
require knowledge of historical, political, or literary events to understand,
such as the meaning of I am Charlie Hebdo which was a rallying cry after
the January 7, 2015, shooting at the offices of the French satirical weekly
newspaper Charlie Hebdo2 in Paris. Notably absent from Bachman’s
model is strategic competence – how to fill gaps of knowledge, such as
missing vocabulary or background information – an essential skill for
navigating real-time interactions.
Since the late twentieth century, the definition of communicative com-
petence in applied linguistics has evolved and expanded as new under-
standings of language, language learning, and communication have
emerged, but it has remained a prominent driving force behind L2 peda-
gogy. Richards and Rodgers (2014: 89–90) summarize the main tenets of
communicative competence as follows:
1. Language is a system for the expression of meaning.
2. The primary function of language is to allow interaction and
communication.
3. The structure of language reflects its functional and communicative
uses.
1
www.kiswahili.net/3-reference-works/proverbs-and-riddles/proverbs-and-riddles-east-african.html
2
www.bbc.com/news/world-europe-30708237
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 39
4. The primary units of language are not merely its grammatical and
structural features, but categories of functional and communicative
meaning as exemplified in discourse.
5. Communicative competence entails knowing how to use language for
a range of different purposes and functions as well as knowing how to
a. vary language use according to the setting and participants (e.g.,
knowing when to use formal and informal speech or when to use
language appropriate for written and spoken communication),
b. produce and understand different types of texts (e.g., advertise-
ments, interviews),
c. maintain communication despite having limitations in one’s Lx
knowledge (e.g., using various communication strategies).
These components of communicative competence are not isolated;
instead, they are inherently integrated in real-world communication, as
Spielmann’s3 model illustrates (Figure 2.3).
Communicative
competence
sociocultural strategic
competence competence
referential textual
gestural
proxemic
pragmatic
discursive
socio-
linguistic rhetorical
linguistic
syntactic - morphological - semantic
phonological
semiotic competence
3
http://opsis.georgetown.domains/LaPageDeGuy/docs/FLE/images/CommCompetence.jpg
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
40 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
In Spielman’s model, semiotic competence interacts with sociocultural
competence, and they both overlap with strategic competence. Many
textbooks in the United States focus on the lower circle, semiotic compe-
tence, primarily represented as linguistic competence: knowing vocabu-
lary, grammatical conventions, word order. However, Spielmann rightly
adds other semiotic systems, including images and symbolism, to the
repertoire of knowledge that individuals need to know, such as the
Statue of Liberty signaling freedom for immigrants in the United States.
Thus, sociocultural competence is integrally connected to semiotic
competence. It includes both sociolinguistic and referential competence,
such as cultural connotations and conventions for politeness or impolite-
ness. Originally developed by van Ek (1986), sociocultural competence is
broader than sociolinguistic competence and includes background knowl-
edge about a culture. It entails, for example, knowing famous people,
history, and important facts about a culture, but also everyday practices
and perspectives held by its members. Van Ek also included social compe-
tence as a separate tenet: the ability to handle social interaction with others,
their motivation and attitude towards participating in another culture, self-
confidence, and empathy.
The next component in Spielmann’s model, strategic competence, over-
laps with semiotic and sociocultural competence. Writing a research paper,
whether in the L1 or the L2, for example, relies on knowledge of rhetorical
organization, both in terms of what to include and how to structure the
paper. Knowing when and how to initiate, maintain, and terminate inter-
actions also requires the knowledge of both linguistic and sociocultural
practices, as does when and how to ask for help when the speaker’s (socio)
linguistic competence is inadequate in a communicative situation.
In the center of Spielmann’s Venn diagram, the three main constructs
converge, as several communicative devices require semiotic, sociocultural,
and strategic knowledge: gestures (e.g., a shrug indicating I don’t know),
proxemics (e.g., how close to stand to another person), pragmatics (e.g.,
how to apologize), and discourse competence (e.g., using cohesion devices,
such as first, or consequently).
Celce-Murcia (2007) considers two more components to the models of
communicative competence presented above: formulaic and interactional
competence. The former entails unanalyzed chunks of language – such as
idiomatic expressions and common lexical collocations – that native speak-
ers rely on for fluent language production. The latter refers to the practical
implementation of sociocultural/sociolinguistic competence and includes
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 41
the ability to produce speech acts, manage conversations (e.g., opening and
closing sequences, topic shifts), and nonverbal communication.
To recap, research on communicative competence has led to viewing
language in L2 pedagogy as an authentic communication system, whose
main components are linguistic, sociolinguistic, discourse, and, in most
models, strategic competence. A logical extension of these explorations was
intercultural communicative competence the way culture and communica-
tion are used within and between social groups.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
42 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
they participate in L2 speech communities successfully. A better outcome,
Byram (1997) argues,
is a learner with the ability to see and manage the relationships between
themselves and their own cultural beliefs, behaviours and meanings, as
expressed in a foreign language, and those of their interlocutors, expressed
in the same language – or even a combination of languages – which may be
interlocutors’ native language, or not. (p. 12)
Learning to navigate inter-group memberships – adapting to new cul-
tures and participating in our own – takes explicit learning and depends on
individual needs and experiences. Byram identified two roles in cultural
encounters. A tourist briefly leaves her home to visit another place but
returns home without changing in any significant way nor having had
much of an impact on the location she visited. In contrast, being a sojourner
involves adaptation by individuals, whether the contact with the new
culture is sought (e.g., study abroad) or is forced by circumstance (e.g.,
refugee status, natural disasters). Sojourners identify practices, beliefs, and
behaviors that are similar to and different from what they experienced at
home, and ideally develop the “capacity to critique and improve their own
and others’ conditions” (p. 2). Byram’s notion of intercultural communi-
cative competence pertains to sojourners, who need to establish and main-
tain relationships with people whose cultural affiliations they do not share.
With sojourners’ needs in mind, L2 pedagogy should help learners
develop into intercultural speakers (Kramsch, 1993; Risager, 2007), who
operate across multiple languages, language varieties, and cultural contexts.
Thus, intercultural speakers need to develop both linguistic and cultural
skills. According to Byram and Zarate (1997), linguistic skills entail (1)
linguistic competence, the ability to apply the rules of a standard variety of
the L2 and produce and interpret it in speech and writing, (2) sociolinguistic
competence, the ability to interpret the L2, produced by native and non-
native speakers, in line with the speaker’s intention or in negotiation with
him or her, and (3) discourse competence, the ability to produce and inter-
pret different genres of spoken or written texts in line with L2 conventions.
These linguistic competences must be complemented by cultural and
intercultural communicative competence reflected in five pedagogical
objectives (Byram, 1997; Byram & Wagner, 2018; Wagner & Bryam, 2017).
The first objective is the development of attitudes that enable individuals
to be curious about and open to other cultures, suspending what they
believe about those cultures and about their own. Second, learners need to
gain knowledge about other social groups, including their products and
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 43
practices, and how those groups interact at the societal and individual
levels. Effective intercultural communication also requires skills of inter-
preting and relating, which refers to learners’ ability to make sense of
documents or events from the perspective of the other culture and compare
them to documents and events in their own. Skills of discovery and inter-
action describe the ability to gain new cultural knowledge and apply it
during real-time, real-world interaction. The final objective relates to the
development of critical cultural awareness/political education, the ability to
analyze one’s own and other cultures based on explicit criteria. In order to
achieve these objectives, Byram argues, learning activities should address
several subskills, which are presented in Table 2.1.
Attitudes
• Engage with members of various social groups to learn about daily lives, beyond exotic
or touristic presentations.
• Discover diverse perspectives and interpretations of cultural phenomena.
• Actively question your own cultural beliefs, practices and habits.
• Be open to adapting to and interacting with other cultures.
• Learn conventions of verbal and nonverbal communication and rules for interaction.
Knowledge
• Understand current and historical relationships between the countries of the
interlocutors.
• Learn about opportunities for traveling to other countries and staying in touch with
members of that community.
• Learn about communication practices that may cause misunderstandings between
interlocutors.
• Know about the collective national memory of your own and the other country (e.g., myths,
historical sites, cultural products) from the perspective of both the in- and out-groups.
• Understand definitions of geographical space in your own and the other country and
how members of the in- and out-groups perceive them (e.g., regional dialects).
• Learn about how members of a social group are socialized formally and informally.
• Understand ways in which social identities, such as class, ethnicity, gender, profession,
or religion, are marked both in verbal and nonverbal communication.
• Learn about institutions that shape daily life in both countries, and how these institutions
are perceived (e.g., who determines the role of government in managing healthcare).
• Understand how social interaction is structured in both countries, in terms of formality
and informality, politeness, taboos, beliefs, etc.
Skills of interpreting and relating
• Understand ethnocentric perspectives and their origins when reading a text or obser-
ving an event; be able to explain the underlying issues.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
44 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
Table 2.1 (cont.)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 45
Byram (1997) also recognizes that most curricula cannot address all of
these skills and subskills and suggests instead that instructors set realistic
goals for their pedagogical contexts (e.g., age of learners, length and purpose
of the course). Whether the L2 learning takes place in a second or foreign
language setting may also identify suitable goals, since the L2 classroom,
fieldwork, and independent learning will likely foster the development of
distinct types and levels of intercultural communication abilities (Byram,
1997; Byram & Wagner, 2018; Wagner & Bryam, 2017). In the classroom,
for example, students can read about the different perceptions of their own
culture and of the societies whose language they are learning. In fieldwork,
instructors can organize excursions, watch videos, or complete introspective
analyses of the culture; this arrangement can offer learners sufficient inde-
pendence to engage with the other culture on their own alongside a common
space for analyzing and reflecting on their experiences. Finally, independent
learning is part of “life-long learning” (Byram, 1997, p. 69), where learners
engage with the other culture without the safety-net of a classroom. Ideally,
learners have an opportunity to develop the attitude, knowledge, and skills
required for successful intercultural communicative competence in a guided
setting prior to having to navigate such encounters on their own.
To summarize, Bryam’s approach rests on “three fundamental features”
(p. 70): (1) the ideal is an intercultural speaker, in contrast to a monolingual
native speaker, (2) L2 pedagogy should foster the development of intercultural
communication, and (3) learning should take place in and beyond the L2
classroom. Byram’s ideas have influenced the profession in significant ways and
lay the foundation for recent research on intercultural communication as well.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
46 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
affective ‘place’ multilingual speakers create as they navigate two or more
languages and cultures – as symbolic competence, a dynamic concept that
reflects the cognitive and affective strengths and tensions that arise from
belonging to multiple speech communities. Multilingual persons’ sym-
bolic competence entails the use of multiple semiotic systems to interpret
and create meaning and experience events from different perspectives.
Symbolic competence also allows multilingual speakers to traverse bound-
aries of both languages and break with conventions in novel, creative ways;
they can also enact different social personae in their various languages and
cultures.
In order to foster the development of symbolic competence, Kramsch
(2009) suggests viewing language learning “as a semiotic, historically and
culturally grounded, personal experience” (p. 2) grounded in specific social
contexts. To reflect the diversity of social contexts in which language is
used, L2 pedagogy should (1) focus on social interaction that reflects
variation across specific communicative contexts, (2) examine how multi-
ple modes of communication – different semiotic systems – are used to
convey meaning, and (3) encourage learners to shed conventional social
identities (e.g., nationality, gender, age) and adopt different subject posi-
tions across languages and social contexts.
The social context – and by extension navigating different identities –
includes tensions between conventional and subjective uses of language
and cultural practices. The former refers to the way that society has
traditionally used certain words, symbols, or meanings, while the latter
reflects a person’s idiosyncratic use of language. Learners have to navigate
these tensions by exploring and even moving beyond conventional uses of
language (i.e., creating meaning or subverting social expectations), while
remaining comprehensible by members of the community:
the multilingual subject is not defined by its boundless freedom and agency,
but, on the contrary, by the linguistic and discursive boundaries it abides by
in order to, now and then, transgress them. The ability to decide how to attach
oneself to the world does not come from a lack of boundaries, but from the choice
of which boundary to transgress. (Kramsch, 2009, p. 185; emphasis mine)
Several pedagogical guidelines for facilitating the development of sym-
bolic competence can be gleaned from Kramsch’s discussion. First, she
argues that classroom activities should address “cultural, political and
ideological issues of language, power, and identity,” perhaps by comparing
representations of the same event from multiple cultural perspectives
(p. 192). Second, activities should foster “sustained thinking or personal
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 47
insights,” possibly by focusing on fewer themes for longer periods of time
(pp. 202–203). Personal insights can be further promoted by allowing
learners to try on different identities, including race, class, gender, sexu-
ality, or ability (Norton, 2000); this type of engagement with the material
can increase learners’ self-determination and intrinsic motivation (Byram’s
[1997] savoir s’engager). Additionally, language play can enhance learners’
motivation and push their linguistic boundaries as they try out what is and
is not possible in the L2. She also recommends that instructors stay attuned
to their affective engagement with the L2 and its culture(s), as their feelings
may impact their pedagogical practices, as they navigate multiple cultural
selves.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
48 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
people communicate (Svalberg, 2007). Liddicoat and Scarino (2013)
describe a tripartite model of communication in which “linguistic struc-
tures provide elements for a communication system that, in turn, becomes
the resource through which social practices are created and accomplished”
(p. 17) within specific contexts of use; since it is impossible to learn all
cultural practices, pedagogical tasks should help learners develop the
analytic skills necessary for interpreting and creating meaning. By becom-
ing sensitive observers, individuals can learn to “participate in multiple
cultures, deploying practices in context-sensitive ways to construct action
in different social groups” (p. 22). Context-sensitivity includes the possi-
bility of learners choosing not to adopt the communicative practices,
beliefs, or behaviors of particular cultural communities; it also recognizes
the diversity of human experience within cultures, including variation in
education, gender, age, religion, ethnicity, or socioeconomic background
(Liddicoat, 2002; Norton, 2000). Ultimately, the objective of pedagogy
with an intercultural communication orientation is for learners to “to
know how to produce language that can be interpreted by native speakers,
but which at the same time acknowledges their own place as members of
another culture along with the identity issues that relate to their first
language cultural frame of reference as nonmembers of the target language
community” (Liddicoat & Scarino, 2013, p. 29).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 49
“become an ‘intercultural speaker’ who can mediate between different
cultures and different viewpoints” (p. 7).
Intercultural learning that fosters the development of diverse view-
points can occur in both the classroom and the real world. Classroom
activities can encourage ethnographic observations and the develop-
ment of declarative knowledge (explicit understandings of how culture
works), while real-world interactions offer opportunities for develop-
ing procedural knowledge, knowing how to participate in commu-
nities of practice (Hua, 2014). Both types of learning are important:
real-world interactions help newcomers become fully legitimate parti-
cipants in cultural practice, both in their own eyes and those of the
host community (Scollon, Scollon, & Jones, 2012), whereas explicit
instruction can help make sense of the new culture, whose practices
often seem indecipherable (Hua, 2014). This duality highlights the
“social nature of the process of socialisation, which is very often
overlooked. It also reminds us of the ‘unfinished’ and ongoing nature
of learning and socialisation” (Hua, 2014, p. 168).
In outlining a pedagogy that fosters the development of intercultural
communicative competence, Hua makes several key observations, high-
lighting that developing intercultural communicative competence is
a long-term process that requires both awareness about language and
culture (i.e., declarative knowledge) and real-world opportunities for put-
ting theory to practice (i.e., procedural knowledge). Moreover, Hua
argues, language pedagogy should support learners at their stage of devel-
opment, while also pushing them to try new experiences, using the class-
room to help “the learner make sense of their intercultural learning
experience” (p. 159). Like Kramsch (2009), Hua also emphasizes that
pedagogical tasks focusing on intercultural communication should guide
learners to analyze and understand contexts of communication (e.g., the
setting of the interaction, the participants, their goals for the interaction)
and culture-specific ways of communication (e.g., directness/indirectness).
Finally, she underscores the importance of language play as well, because it
can help learners explore in what ways and to what extent they wish to
adopt the practices of the host culture(s).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
50 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
competence, which itself has undergone growth since it was intro-
duced into applied linguistics. In spite of the important role inter-
cultural (communicative) competence has come to play in language
pedagogy, a clear definition has remained elusive (Deardorff, 2011).
Nonetheless, synthesizing the discussions in the second half of this
chapter, several characteristics emerge, pertaining to (1) knowledge
and skills, (2) linguistic repertoires (both declarative and procedural
knowledge), and (3) curricular issues. These themes suggest that an
intercultural communication-oriented pedagogy should help learners
do the following:
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 51
(cont.)
Summary
While most scholarship focuses on SLA as a predominantly psycho-
linguistic process, some recent theories acknowledge the social context
in which language is used. Living in a multilingual and multicultural
world requires such a social orientation and entails developing com-
municative competence. Since this construct was developed for study-
ing language use among native speakers, it has been expanded to
intercultural communicative competence in the L2/Lx learning con-
text, to reflect the existing linguistic, cultural, and referential knowl-
edge that (especially) adult learners have upon embarking on their
language learning journey (Byram, 1997; Hua, 2014; Kramsch, 2009;
Liddicoat & Scarino, 2013). Proposals for a pedagogy oriented towards
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
52 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
intercultural communicative competence emphasize the need for lear-
ners to be able to navigate multiple linguistic and cultural repertoires,
reflecting actively and critically on existing and new cultural practices
in which they participate. Moreover, such pedagogy should focus on
helping learners to become thoughtful observers of other cultural
behaviors, beliefs, and practices, developing the skills to analyze
socially contextualized interaction within and beyond the L2/Lx
classroom.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
2 Pedagogical Foundations of Teaching ICC for L2/Lx Use 53
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:55:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.003
chapter 3
Chapter Overview
In order to become more effective communicators, we – and our students –
need to understand how participants, their cultural and intercultural
selves, values, motivations, language knowledge and language use, among
other factors, shape interactions. We can improve our understanding by
analyzing interactions systematically: What are different ways in which we
communicate in our L1? And in our L2/Lx? How is communication in the
L2/Lx similar to or different from the way we communicate in our L1 in
specific interactions? What are some challenges we face when communi-
cating with people from different linguistic and cultural backgrounds?
What can facilitate smooth and effective communication? How do the
different layers of culture – the supranational, national, regional, cultural
group-based, local, and individual factors – contribute to the interaction
(Bonvillain, 2020; Oetzel, 2009; Saville-Troike, 2003)? Which aspects of
culture are most salient – relevant and impactful – in a particular situation?
Reflecting upon such questions helps learners recognize patterns both in
their L1 and L2/Lx and become more effective intercultural communicators.
To further this goal, in light of Byram and Masuhara’s (2013) recommenda-
tion, this chapter presents three models for analyzing communication as a
culturally situated process: the ethnography of communication, interac-
tional sociolinguistics, and multimodal analysis. Instructors can implement
these analytic approaches in the classroom, using various sources of authen-
tic data, to help students learn to understand communication in culturally
and situationally appropriate ways.
54
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 55
ethnographers, discourse or multimodal analysts. Instead, the goal is for
them to emerge as conscious consumers of and participants in commu-
nication, cultivating their ability to observe and understand communica-
tion as enacted locally by participants within broader societal contexts
(Mills, 2009; Kecskés, 2015; Locher, 2012). The analytic approaches
presented in this chapter can be implemented early in the L2/Lx learning
process and provide sufficient information to help foster the development
of intercultural communicative competence, both for this book’s immedi-
ate (language teachers) and its indirect audiences (language learners).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
56 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
3. Ends: shared expectations, what the participants want to accomplish
during the interaction.
4. Act sequence: the content of the communicative event, referential
information exchanged by interactants; the steps they take during
the interaction (e.g., turn-taking, overlapping speech).
5. Key: the tone of the event, for example, whether it is humorous or
serious.
6. Instrumentalities: the form of communication, including verbal and
nonverbal channels.
7. Norms of interpretation: shared knowledge for understanding the
structure of interaction.
8. Genre: the type of communicative event, such as greetings or narration.
In contexts where the L2/Lx is widely spoken, a range of ethnographic
data can be collected and analyzed by learners. Reports from participant
observations may include quick impressionistic notes, detailed fieldnotes
of interactions that the observer is an active participant in or ones that he or
she observes from the outside, as well as transcripts of interviews and
interactions (Monaghan, 2007). Quick handwritten notes serve the pur-
pose of reminding the observer of important aspects of a communicative
event. In contrast, fieldnotes are more analytic and present thorough
accounts of cultural activity; they utilize detailed descriptions of
the participants, the purpose of the interaction, the setting, the mood of
the exchange, as well as any questions that arise regarding the commu-
nicative event. Transcriptions are precise written records of an interaction
(preferably recorded). Whether analyzing real-world or text-based interac-
tions, students can begin by filling out Hymes’ SPEAKING grid (see
Appendix A for a sample analysis) and examining how communication is
cocreated by participants within specific social situations.
The salient aspects – what is actually important for and relevant to
understanding the interaction – must be included in the analyses, includ-
ing exact words, overlapping speech, and/or pauses, if they help make sense
of the exchange. These data are written up in an ethnographic report,
presenting a rich analysis of what you experienced and observed. A rich
analysis – thick descriptions (Geertz, 2007) – requires a thorough exam-
ination of the data, including the social context in which the interaction
took place, as the following example of two boys winking illustrates:
Consider … two boys rapidly contracting the eyelids of their right eyes. In
one, this is an involuntary twitch; in the other, a conspiratorial signal to a
friend. The two movements are, as movements, identical; from an I-am-a-
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 57
camera, “phenomenalistic” observation of them alone, one could not tell
which was twitch and which was wink, or indeed whether both or either was
twitch or wink. Yet the difference, however unphotographable, between a
twitch and a wink is vast; as anyone unfortunate enough to have had the first
taken for the second knows. The winker is communicating, and indeed
communicating in a quite precise and special way: (1) deliberately, (2) to
someone in particular, (3) to impart a particular message, (4) according to a
socially established code, and (5) without cognizance of the rest of the
company. As Ryle1 points out, the winker has done two things, contracted
his eyelids and winked, while the twitcher has done only one, contracted his
eyelids. Contracting your eyelids on purpose when there exists a public code
in which so doing counts as a conspiratorial signal is winking. That’s all
there is to it: a speck of behavior, a fleck of culture, and – voilà! – a gesture.
(Geertz, 2007, p. 28)
This vignette demonstrates the difference between merely noticing
communicative activities and analyzing them. Analysis seeks to understand
how particular communicative devices are used in social interaction and
why. Consider, for example, what happens when a friend visits you at
home:
• How would you greet this person?
• What social purpose do you hope to achieve with this greeting?
• How do the verbal and nonverbal aspects of your greeting work
together?
• Under what circumstances is this specific greeting appropriate (e.g.,
with whom, when)?
• When would it be unusual?
• What values and issues of identity or power inform your interpretation
of when this greeting is appropriate and when it is not?
• How do you learn and know which communicative acts are appropriate
in which communicative contexts?
In order to answer questions like this, as communicators, Ls/Lx learners –
and we, as instructors – need to learn to analyze various types of data.
Transcripts of an interaction teach learners about how participants jointly
construct communication within specific social contexts. Such data can be
complemented by metapragmatic information, if learners ask or interview
members of a culture whose practices learners seek to understand
(Monaghan & Goodman, 2007). However, it is important to recognize
1
This question of intentionality was originally posed by the philosopher G. Ryle (1968) ‘The Thinking
of Thoughts. What Is Le Penseur Doing?’, reprinted in Ryle (2009).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
58 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
that not all members of a culture can accurately analyze and describe their
own practices, which are mostly guided by procedural knowledge, knowing
how to do something without thinking about it. Consequently, it can be
difficult even for members of the in-group to provide declarative knowledge,
to explain why people use language in a certain way or what cultural values
and beliefs might affect their communication (Myers-Scotton, 2006). In
our daily interactions, we constantly make split-second judgments about
how we communicate both verbally and nonverbally, but we rarely have to
articulate the underlying rules that guide our sociocultural mental foot-
work. Yet, if we are serious about improving our ability to communicate
interculturally, we need to understand why we communicate how we do
and increase our declarative knowledge both about our L1 and our L2/Lxs.
Rampton (2007) notes that ethnographic analyses can be challenging,
since it entails “trying to get familiar with the strange” and “trying to get
analytic distance on what’s close at hand,” interrogating what we take for
granted in communication (pp. 590–591; emphasis original). He empha-
sizes that ethnography requires reflective, detailed, and sustained analysis
that looks at language use, starting with the details (i.e., bottom-up),
leading to interpretations of how language is used in and by social actors.
Implications from the language data regarding broader cultural practices
need to be drawn with caution. The people and language we analyze should
not be treated solely as members of a group, since individuals are not
simply categories. Instead, they reflect unique combinations of broader
and smaller cultural influences, as we saw in Chapter 1.
While it is not Rampton’s intention to make a link to intercultural
communicative competence, his description of ethnography actually
makes a good case for incorporating such analyses in the L2/Lx classroom.
Prior to engaging in analyses of other cultural groups, learners should
reflect on their own practices as this process “encourages sensitivity to
the risks of stereotyping: if you are researching people and institutions in
the area where you are based, the kind of people you are studying may well
turn up in your classes and/or read-and-reply to what you’ve written, and
this provides quite strong incentives to hedge your claims and clearly specify
their limits” (Rampton, 2007, p. 591; emphasis mine).
In other words, engaging in a process of identifying what the rules might
be and what their limits are (who observes them, in what specific situations,
when do they not apply, etc.) can guide learners to observe other cultures
with nuance beyond the taken-for-granted ‘nation = culture’ model.
Insights should be gleaned from multiple sources, to ensure that our
interpretation of communicative events and their cultural contexts is
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 59
accurate and objective, and that our own cultural practices do not limit or
unduly influence (i.e., bias) our analyses. Ideally, these analyses lead to the
ability to understand communication from an emic (insider) perspective,
instead of an etic (outsider) one; this is an essential part of intercultural
competence, of developing knowledge about and empathy towards others
(Byram, 1997; Fantini, 2007; Jackson, 2014). While the ethnography of
communication is excellent for viewing communication holistically, some-
times it is helpful to focus closely on how language is used to enact culture.
Interactional sociolinguistics is an accessible approach to analyzing lan-
guage in interaction, how it both reflects and shapes culture.
Interactional Sociolinguistics
While learners at earlier levels of L2 development might not be able to
reproduce the language they see, hear, or read in L2/Lx authentic materials,
they can learn to recognize cultural practices reflected in language.
Interactional sociolinguistics (IS), which was developed by the linguistic
anthropologist John Gumperz (1982), can shed light on how people create
meaning through interaction and how language interacts with its social
context. By examining discourse closely – usually transcribed audio- or
video-materials of natural or naturalistic language – IS helps reveal how we
decode and encode meaning during interaction, processes that are typically
subconscious but always culturally situated; that is, always in a specific
cultural context. Close analyses of interaction can help avoid overgener-
alization and identify what aspect of the context (e.g., social roles) might be
most salient in particular interactions. For example, when you are “being
interviewed for a job, sitting in class, having a meal, hanging around with
friends, talking about recent events, work, food, music and so forth – you
soon realize that local, institutional, activity- and discourse-specific iden-
tities may be a lot more compelling for the participants than, for example,
their Anglo, Pakistani or African Caribbean family backgrounds, and that
when ethnicity does become an issue, this happens in all sorts of different
ways – deconstructive, respectful, racist, some quite spectacular and others
hardly noticed” (Rampton, 2017, p. 4).
To understand communication in context, IS examines verbal and
nonverbal interaction turn by turn (complemented by conversation ana-
lysis), cross-checking it against broader cultural practices that help indivi-
duals “reconcile the material that they encounter in any given situation
with their prior [culturally informed] understanding” (Rampton, 2017,
p. 3). In interactions, individuals use language they deem appropriate to
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
60 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
convey the substance of their message, but also additional social informa-
tion, such as using backchanneling devices (e.g., aha or nodding) to show
that they are paying attention to their interlocutor or adjusting their level
of formality to match the situation (e.g., shifting to a more prestigious
accent, standing up straighter). This process is typically effortless and
subconscious for competent L1 users, as they continually evaluate the
interaction to ensure that their communication meets shared expected
patterns or to understand what variation from expectations might mean:
Does the higher volume means that my interlocutor is annoyed or only
that the ambient noise is louder too? Is the misuse of a word a joke or has
the message of my interactional partner shifted? The specific context –
understanding who the participants are, what the goals of their interaction
are, how the physical environment may affect their communication – is an
essential, intrinsic aspect of communication (Erickson, 2011).
The following excerpt illustrates how IS can be used to analyze interac-
tion. This excerpt depicts the beginning of an exchange between a clerk
and an American guest (who is accompanied by his wife and two teenage
sons) at the registration desk of a hotel that rents out apartments in Venice,
Italy. The clerk’s tasks entail welcoming the guest, obtaining documenta-
tion and payment, providing directions, handing over the keys to the
guests, and describing the rules for using the apartment. The guest’s
objectives are complementary: he needs to register, pay the tourist taxes,
pick up the key, and get directions to the apartment. The interaction lasted
about twelve minutes, and the first few minutes are transcribed here (for
classroom use, instructors should provide transcripts of focal segments of
interactions until learners are able to transcribe them themselves, probably
at the intermediate-high, advanced levels, depending on the type of inter-
action, such as the number of participants, rate of speech).
Excerpt (1)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 61
The interaction begins when the hotel clerk greets the four guests,
primarily addressing Guest 1, using a formulaic expression, which includes
a question that determines the content of the text turn. She greets the
guests in English, possibly because English is a lingua franca of tourism or
the appearance of the guests prompted her to address them in English.
Alternately, she could have referred to a list of check-ins for the day and
expected the American family. The guest’s response (beginning in line 2)
overlaps slightly with the clerk’s question, since he is familiar with the
greeting-query style of communication commonly used when checking in
at a hotel. The rising intonation of his questions (line 3) implies an indirect
confirmation check whether this is the right place to be checking in.
Similarly, the clerk’s rising intonation in her statement about making
copies of the passports is an indirect request for permission; the guests
know that this has to be done, and the question can be interpreted as a pro-
forma expression of courtesy. Her next question (lines 7–8) seeks to
confirm the number of guests and the location of the apartment. There
are several overlapping backchanneling devices (umm … or yeah) and
turns, for example in lines 2, 4, 6, and 10. They reveal two facts about
the interaction. First, the participants signal that they are paying attention
to their interlocutors. Second, they also signal that they can anticipate each
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
62 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
other’s comment, given the routinized format and content of this interac-
tion, relying on a shared knowledge of the registration process. The two
hesitation markers in line 13 (mhm … Yes, of course. But umm..) seem to
indicate something different: the clerk has to ask for money to cover city
taxes, and she may feel uncomfortable with this request, and these devices
help soften that request. In line 16, Guest 1 asks his wife for cash, and
although she responds to him, suggesting that he confirm the amount (line
17), it was the clerk who reached out to accept the money.
While the participants’ prior referential knowledge was helpful in ensur-
ing smooth communication, there were some features of the interaction
that were specific to the immediate context. The clerk spoke fluent English
and was clearly able to converse comfortably with native speakers of the
language. The guest had also traveled in Italy before and knew about the
tax that had to be paid in cash; this knowledge prevented the need for
lengthy explanations about an additional (for some, unexpected) cash
expense. The coequal turn-taking between the clerk and Guest 1 demon-
strated complementary and cooperative social roles in this particular
interaction. Finally, although the clerk’s emphatic comment in line 11 –
oh, I don’t NEED that – might have come across as somewhat direct in
other contexts, she was smiling during the exchange, and both participants
wanted to achieve a shared objective, therefore took accommodating
stances (attitudes) towards each other.
As we can see, the participants co-constructed the interaction in a
specific, unique way, situated in the local context (i.e., the specific people,
their specific linguistic and pragmatic choices, attitudes, etc.). The notion
that interaction is locally situated describes an analytic approach that
examines how broader cultural practices and beliefs contribute to interac-
tion, while accepting that it is the individual participants who enact culture
and their social identity as they communicate (Mills, 2009).
For L2/Lx learners, participating in interaction can be challenging,
especially at lower levels of proficiency, when referential knowledge (see
Chapter 9) is not available to fall back on and linguistic skills are not
sufficiently developed yet. The challenges include the lack of sufficient
access to interactional modelling between users of the L2/Lx, whether
native or effective nonnative language users. In addition to difficulties
with interpreting or employing varieties of language (e.g., lexicon, pro-
nunciation, grammar, sociolinguistic rules) that allow them to adapt to
different social meanings, inexperienced language users often also lack the
frameworks for interpreting the relevance of social roles, situation-specific
discourse, the physical setting of the interaction, or cultural references.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 63
Consequently, they have to draw more heavily on conscious analysis and
explicit metapragmatic discussions with their interlocutors (e.g., Is that the
right word? or I hope that makes sense) to cocreate meaning. Becoming
observant of both the specific language used in interaction and how that
language is shaped by and shapes the social context also helps improve
intercultural communicative competence.
The approaches discussed above can offer complementary interpreta-
tions: ethnographic analysis contributes a top-down (social context-driven)
view, while interactional sociolinguistics provides a bottom-up (text-
driven) perspective. Longitudinal, participant-observation-based analyses
resulting in a coherent, in-depth understanding of cultural practices are not
usually feasible in L2/Lx classrooms. Yet, careful examinations of multiple
unique interactions over time eventually provide learners with a broad
overview of another culture, and, more importantly, help them acquire the
skills necessary to interpret socially, culturally shaped communicative
practices in local contexts. While the ethnography of communication
and interactional sociolinguistics are well suited for analyzing spoken,
signed, or enacted language (e.g., nonverbal communication), multimodal
analyses offer a helpful supplement for understanding communication that
draws on multiple semiotic systems.
Multimodal Analysis
Real-world communication typically occurs across multiple integrated
channels of meaning-making (Abrams, 2016a; Helm & Dooly, 2017;
Herring, 2007, 2015; Jewitt, 2014; Jewitt, Bezemer & O’Halloran, 2016;
Kress & van Leeuwen, 2001; Norris & Maier, 2014), combining various
communicative resources, a community’s means for creating meaning:
• visual: including printed texts, fonts, colors, symbols, images, avatars,
graphics, clothing;
• sound: including speech, exclamations (e.g., ha!), noise, music; and
• actions: including proxemics (e.g., preferences for personal space),
posture or gestures.
Any of these modes or semiotic resources may be the focus of multimodal
analysis, depending on what the learner/analyst wishes to understand or
the epistemological stances of the researcher. Jewitt and her colleagues
(2016) identify three approaches to multimodal analysis: systemic-func-
tional multimodal discourse analysis, social semiotics, and conversation
analysis. The first and last approaches are quite technical and do not lend
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
64 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
themselves well to classroom adaptation. In contrast, social semiotics
(Kress & van Leeuwen, 2001) seems well suited for the purposes of this
volume, since it is more easily accessible to language instructors and their
students, helping them develop the skills necessary for systematic observa-
tion of intercultural communication. Moreover, this approach is repre-
sented predominantly in studies on second language learning, analyzing a
range of ‘texts,’ including printed materials, still images, popular media,
sociopolitical commentary, or computer-mediated communication
(Abrams, 2016a; Hampel, 2010; Hampel & Hauck, 2006; Hampel &
Pleines, 2013; Hauck, 2010). Although originally focusing on textual arte-
facts, this approach has been expanded to analyze dynamic interactions as
well, especially when used in conjunction with interactional sociolinguis-
tics and ethnography (Jewitt et al., 2016; Norris, 2004). Next, I outline the
basic tenets of this approach to multimodal analysis, followed by two
sample analyses, one of the interaction presented in Excerpt (1) above
and one of a poster.
Social semiotics, like all multimodal analyses, views language (i.e., speech
and writing) as only one of the many channels through which people convey
information to their interlocutors (Jewitt et al., 2016). Embodied modes
(e.g., body language, eye-gaze) and disembodied modes (e.g., the physical
location, clothing, artefacts) contribute to meaning-making (Norris, 2004).
The various modes (e.g., the verbal message and gestures) may reinforce
(nodding while saying yes) or contradict (sarcastic tone of voice when saying
Oh, I’m just fine) one another; alternately, they may fulfil complementary
roles, for example, when we ask would you like this bouquet of flowers or that
one? while pointing (Kress & van Leeuwen, 2001).
Importantly, understanding the specific context in which communica-
tion takes place is crucial for analyzing social action, interaction, and
identity, including the social relationships among participants, power,
and agency. Consequently, interpreting communication requires the abil-
ity to understand the totality of meaning-making, which can be provided
by rich ethnographic data (Flewitt, 2011): Who are the participants? What
and why are they communicating? How do their communicative choices
reflect their identities? How does history and culture influence the way
they communicate? How is multimodal communication encoded and
decoded in specific cultural settings? Understanding the totality of com-
munication, however, does not mean that every single facet of the text or
interaction has to be investigated equally at all times. Any analysis may
focus on one or more specific modes or aspects of communication, such as
how silence, laughter, clothing, bodily adornment, or eye-gaze is used by
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 65
the interactants to make meaning. Furthermore, in-depth analysis first
examines various modes separately, before bringing them together again to
identify how they “work together to construct the complexity of face to
face interactions” (Norris, 2004, pp. 12–13).
Excerpt (2)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
66 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 67
interaction. In line 2, the referent of this apartment is identified with the
printout of the rental confirmation. When analyzing Excerpt (2), the
clerk’s hesitations in line 14 (the numbers differ in this excerpt, due to
the additional peripheral participants) could be interpreted as discomfort
with the request, an interpretation that is reinforced by her gaze, because
she is looking down at the photocopies instead of at the guests, even
though during the rest of the conversation she looked at Guest 1, with
occasionally scanning of the other three guests. Finally, in line 21, the
physical layout of the space helps explain why the clerk says thank you to
Guest 2: the office is small, and the clerk intercepts the cash. These are only
a few observations about the brief excerpt of the interaction, and not all
analyses have to be equally detailed. When using such analyses for peda-
gogical purposes, instructors should guide learners’ attention to one or two
focal aspects of embodied communication – such as posture, gestures,
proxemics, head movement, eye-gaze, surrounding sounds (e.g., music,
ambient noise) – or how they work in combination. In addition to
embodied interpersonal communication, documents can be analyzed
multimodally as well, as the next section explains.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
68 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
Figure 3.1 Flyer by College Nine and College Ten Cocurricular Programs Unit, UCSC
(poster created by Daphne Mark, reprinted with permission)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 69
solidarity or distance between the text’s author(s) and its intended audi-
ence. The fact that neither the Cocurricular programs office or the location
for the events is included with a map indicates that the author and audience
are familiar with the physical context of the advertised events.
Beyond the first two layers, the poster communicates an underlying
textual meaning as well, indicating how the ideational and interperso-
nal messages interact to form a unified message. In this ad, the
colorful and thematically coherent images focus on fun and creative
expression, and are intended to elicit a sense of excitement in the
viewers regarding the event so that they want to participate and thus
help strengthen a sense of community among the students belonging
to these colleges. At the same time, the messages also connect the
event with values important to the university: social justice, celebrat-
ing students’ identities and self-care, which are useful life-skills that
students need as they transition to university. The design features
reiterate the focus on students: the sans serif font is youthful, evoking
a close and forward-looking relationship among the author and view-
ers, compared to more old-fashioned fonts.
As this poster partly illustrated, visual images, sounds, and actions
convey socially situated meaning (Jewitt, 2014); some are limited to parti-
cular social communities, others are shared across cultures. For example,
the rings of the Olympic Games are globally recognized symbols for an
international athletic competition. The winners’ podium conveys meaning
as well: the highest step signals the gold-medalist, the middle step is
reserved for the silver- and the lowest step for the bronze-medal awardees.
Music adds another communicative layer: the national anthems that
celebrate the winners might not be familiar internationally but have
symbolic and emotional significance for many members of the athlete’s
country. These elements jointly create cognitive and affective meaning to
the audiences of the Olympic Games.
Engaging with multimodal communication is crucial for L2/Lx learning
as well. Websites designed for authentic audiences contain verbal, non-
verbal, audio, and visual information that reflect how communication is
created for and by real-world users of the language (Kress & van Leeuwen,
2001). With the advent of the Internet and the rise in popularity of social
media platforms such as Facebook, Instagram, Snapchat, Twitter, and
others, people rarely use just one modality to communicate their ideas to
and with their interactants. L2/Lx learners also often use their technologi-
cal devices to stay informed about the world, interact with other L2/Lx
users, engage with and experience entertainment, and receive and process
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
70 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
course materials (González-Lloret & Ortega, 2014). At this point, it is
useful to note that computer and Internet access is a privilege, and not all
L2/Lx learners have the same opportunities for unlimited, immediate
access to authentic language sources across the globe; also, not all languages
enjoy the same breadth of representation on the Internet as English, for
example.
In spite of its wealth of meaning-making resources, multimodal com-
munication might be overwhelming for L2/Lx learners, particularly at early
stages of language learning. Therefore, such materials need careful
consideration. Hampel and Hauck (2006) acknowledge the significant
challenge that multimodal L2/Lx pedagogy represents by shifting from
the single spoken modality of traditional classrooms to communicating in
multiple modes:
Learners in general and language learners in particular have always been
required to shift semiotic material outside their modal ‘comfort zone’ and to
transpose information from oral input, written text and visual clues.
However, the varying degrees of embeddedness of modes in the new
media and the resulting modal complexity turn language acquisition in
virtual environments into a new challenge. (p. 11)
Furthermore, transcribing multimodal communication is challenging
(Helm & Dooly, 2017). It is selective and incomplete. It is impossible to
transcribe an entire day’s or movie’s worth of interactions, therefore, we
predetermine what segment of communication we end up analyzing.
Similarly, we cannot capture every gaze, gesture, and body movement,
just as it is unlikely that we can transcribe every nuance of spoken language.
To reduce the challenges, there are several steps that instructors and
students can take to make use of multimodal analyses for the benefit of
L2/Lx learning:
• identify what it is that you are trying to understand from the multi-
modal analysis (if body language, then focus only on those aspects of
communication),
• understand the context of use of the multimodal discourse (e.g., reflect
on the ethnographic situation: Who are the interlocutors? What is their
objective?),
• analyze different modes separately (depending on the analytic focus,
start with one mode, then add the other modes of communication),
• work in pairs or groups, asking different observers to capture various
layers of the multimodal message or interaction,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 71
• discuss transcription practices explicitly (e.g., how to transcribe enthu-
siasm, emphatic statements or more or less vigorous hand gestures), and
• incorporate sufficient time as homework or in-class activities to allow
for reexamining the data, in order to verify the accuracy of the tran-
script and to interpret it thoroughly.
With these safeguards, language tasks based on multimodal resources
can help learners develop the necessary skills to interpret the meaning of
different communicative modes coherently and allow for creative contri-
bution by the learners (Hampel & Hauck, 2006). While authentic websites
can provide opportunities for holistic L2/Lx learning, carefully designed
tasks need to make the culturally embedded authentic language and
potentially unfamiliar images and sounds more accessible to learners
(Chapters 4–9 include sample activity sets). Interconnected tasks can
draw learners’ attention step by step to the multiple semiotic systems
through which communication is realized. Tasks that follow these princi-
ples can help learners conduct rigorous and systematic analyses of authen-
tic L2/Lx sources that communicate meaning multimodally (Jewitt, 2014;
Kress & van Leeuwen, 2001).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
72 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
(Berardo, 2006, p. 62). In L2/Lx pedagogy, authentic materials can be
highly motivating for students and are useful for providing culturally and
sociolinguistically contextualized language. However, they need to be
selected with care, to match learning objectives, learners’ interests, and
their L2/Lx skills.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 73
5. Working with authentic materials requires learners and instructors to
become comfortable with not understanding everything; becoming
comfortable with an incomplete understanding of the L2/Lx can help
learners be more successful intercultural communicators as well
(Hofstede, Hofstede & Minkov, 2010).
6. Over time, students develop a sense of accomplishment as they learn to
“comprehend a real and authentic source from the target language”
(Gilmore, 2007, p. 107), but beforehand, their confidence can be fostered
by selecting material and tasks that are not so difficult as to frustrate the
learners, but not so easy as to suppress their interest in the learning
process.
7. Authentic materials need to match learners’ proficiency level in the
L2/Lx.
a. Beginning learners: In general, shorter, everyday topics presented
with audio-visual support may be useful at earlier stages of L2/Lx
development, because multimodality can help make input more
comprehensible and reduce learner frustration (Kilickaya, 2004).
Less complex storylines, adherence to ‘standard’ varieties of the
language and sources that do not rely heavily on abstract concepts
or cultural symbolism might be more suitable for this population.
Also at early stages of L2/Lx learning, some modification of
authentic texts may be warranted, such as selecting a smaller
segment of a longer text or highlighting key points (Gilmore,
2007), although making sources more accessible should not
come at the expense of losing the cultural benefits they provide.
Realistic videos are also better at this stage: people speaking will be
more comprehensible than cartoons, and nonverbal communica-
tion can help learners understand linguistic and cultural informa-
tion better.
b. Intermediate learners: This population can conduct increasingly
more sophisticated analyses of the connection between language
form and its socially contextualized meaning. For example, lear-
ners can use newspapers to compare the elements of news reports
and opinion pieces: What are the key arguments in each type of
writing and how are they structured? What evidence, if any, do the
authors use to support their points? Are there differences in how
the authors present their points in terms of language, modality, or
stylistic preferences? Selecting content that is familiar to and of
interest to learners can facilitate comprehension, because learners
can tap into available background knowledge, but intermediate-
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
74 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
level learners still require significant scaffolding for understanding
and interacting with authentic materials.
c. Advanced-level learners: With increasing proficiency, learners
should engage with more complex and abstract language and
ideas, learning about cultural beliefs and practices in progressively
more nuanced ways. Authentic sources and tasks should also
promote critical analyses of power and social dominance.
Learners could explore, for example, how different social groups
describe political events in which they participated (e.g., the
references, lexicon, or metaphors they use). Texts on sensitive
topics should allow for a thorough discussion of the underlying
issues and for learners’ questions to be answered adequately.
Instructors should not shy away from interesting or provocative
topics, but it can be difficult to discuss them in an L2/Lx when the
relevant linguistic and cultural knowledge is not yet readily acces-
sible for participants.
8. Interpreting authentic materials requires significant levels of L2/Lx
linguistic and cultural knowledge by the instructor, thus, educa-
tors need to commit to learning about the culture(s) they teach,
and from multiple perspectives (i.e., intracultural variation). The
authentic resources they select can also improve instructors’ own
knowledge of the L2/Lx cultures and demonstrate to learners that
developing intercultural communicative competence is a lifelong
process. Additionally, learners often contribute insights that the
instructor does not know or has not considered, perhaps some-
thing students learned in another course or experienced first-hand;
this is a positive aspect of a learning community. Student ques-
tions can also guide the class session in a positive and purposeful
direction.
9. It can be difficult – or unsustainably time consuming – to find
authentic sources that model particular language forms or cultural
concepts sufficiently. If the curriculum permits it, convert this chal-
lenge into a strength and focus onwhat is presented, namely, actual,
authentic language use. If, however, curricular constraints prevent
such flexibility, authentic materials should be combined with pedago-
gically prepared ones to help learners ease into their contact with the
L2/Lx. Glossaries or pretasks can provide lexical and content support
for authentic sources, simultaneously reducing the need for text mod-
ification and increasing learner autonomy.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 75
These pedagogical considerations can guide the selection of authentic
materials and ensure “the fitness of the material for the learning environ-
ment” or the exploitability of authentic materials (Gilmore, 2007, p. 98).
All types of source materials can be useful for an interculturally oriented
pedagogy; however, filmic materials can serve as particularly effective
sources of intercultural communication.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
76 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
Tognozzi, 2010) and can be used as springboards for L2/Lx analysis. Thus,
films can provide learners with relatively realistic linguistic and socio-
linguistic experiences. Finally, the multimodality of filmic materials
makes them better resources for L2/Lx learners than recorded real-world
interactions, which can be difficult to collect (either by students or
instructors), are often confusing, and have variable audio-visual quality
(Grant & Starks, 2001).
These issues should be considered when selecting filmic materials as
well, since each movie, comedy, documentary, or news report models
unique language. For example, while the film adaptations of Jane
Austen’s novels might be excellent for teaching culture, history, and
literary analysis at more advanced levels, the language (e.g., lexicon,
pragmatics) might be less useful at lower levels of L2/Lx development.
The choices are endless, but the selection of filmic resources should be
guided by specific learning objectives. Movies and TV series that depict
intercultural communication and identities explicitly, like The Boy with the
Topknot (U.K., 2017), East West 101 (Australia, 2007–2011), My Big Fat
Greek Wedding (U.S.A., 2002), Neuland (Switzerland, 2015), My Brilliant
Friend (Italy, 2018), or The Good Karma Hospital (U.K., 2017), are parti-
cularly conducive to fostering intercultural awareness and communicative
competence.
Summary
In this chapter, three analytic approaches were presented – the ethno-
graphy of communication, interactional sociolinguistics, and multi-
modal analysis – that lend themselves well to implementation in L2/
Lx pedagogy. Using these analyses allows students to examine inter-
actions closely and to learn how to participate in intercultural com-
munication themselves. The type of data needed and the appropriate
analytic approach depend on what aspect of communication learners
are seeking to understand. For questions regarding the relationship
between language and broader social practices, ethnography of com-
munication is a good fit; for a closer analysis of how language is used
for specific communicative purposes, interactional sociolinguistics is
perhaps better suited; for understanding posters or music videos,
multimodal analysis can be prioritized. However, these approaches
are often used in combination, such as in Excerpt (2), where interac-
tional sociolinguistics is combined with multimodal analysis.
Crucially, learners need not become fully trained ethnographers,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 77
discourse or multimodal analysts. Rather, each time they apply a small
aspect of the analytic frameworks presented in this chapter, they
improve their skills for becoming more conscious and aware consu-
mers of intercultural communication.
The chapter also proposed a number of possible data sources: con-
versations that learners can observe directly, interviews with members of
the in-group and more knowledgeable members of the out-group, as well
as a range of authentic materials that can serve as valuable resources for
modeling L2/Lx use. Chapters 4–9 examine further how to implement
the analyses presented in this chapter, using various authentic resources
and interactions, so that L2/Lx learners can develop the linguistic (e.g.,
lexicon, grammar, and pragmatics), discursive (e.g., poetry, exposition,
argumentation), and cultural knowledge required for intercultural com-
municative competence.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
78 Part I Theoretical and Methodological Foundations
Setting TV set, with an audience, cameras, the host and the guest sit on two
sides of a desk (this makes the interview a bit more formal, and
limits body language); audience in armchairs in the hall.
Scene Comedy, light-hearted entertainment
Participants • Speaker/listener: Jon Stewart (the host)
• Listener/speaker: Malala Yousafzai (a 16-year old girl, originally
from Pakistan, youngest Nobel Prize winner, author, activist
for girls’ education; survivor of a shooting by the Taliban)
• The audience on the set; the TV audience at home/in front of
computers
• Participants are ratified and legitimate, speaker/listener, audience
Ends • To entertain: first and foremost, a late-night comedy show is
meant to entertain the audience and engage in light-hearted
banter with the guest (e.g., his reference to New Jersey, self-
deprecating humor)
• To inform the audience about Malala’s story, her book, and
girls’ education in Pakistan
• To get people to support her cause (e.g., by buying her book or
donating to her foundation)
Act sequence • Introduction: JS introduces MY to the audience(s)
• JS asks questions, MY answers throughout the interview – all
questions are open-ended, sufficient time provided for
responses
• Audience laughs throughout, applauses between turns
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
3 The Learner as Analyst: Methods and Sources of Data Analysis 79
(cont.)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:56:33, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.004
chapter 4
Chapter Overview
One of the most important contributors to the comprehension and pro-
duction of the L2/Lx is the lexicon, the focus of the first pedagogical
chapter. After briefly reviewing relevant research regarding the develop-
ment of the L2/Lx lexicon, the discussion turns to issues in language
variation and communication strategies that L2/Lx users might draw on,
when they lack the necessary vocabulary to convey their intended meaning.
Since research into L2/Lx lexical acquisition is vast, a full review is impos-
sible in this space; therefore, this chapter focuses specifically on findings
that are most relevant for promoting intercultural communicative compe-
tence. The chapter concludes with a proposal of connecting L2/Lx research
on the lexicon to concepts in intercultural communicative competence,
including two sets of language teaching activities.
83
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
84 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
used words carry a significant amount of the communicative load in an L2/
Lx. The most frequent 100 words in English, for example, account for as
much as 50 percent of texts, while the 1000 most frequent words cover 70–
90 percent of most reading texts (Nation, 2013). Nation (2016) and Schmitt
(2014) suggest focusing on the highest-frequency words (ca 3000 words and
word families) in the first years of L2 instruction, while subsequent years
should push the development of a mid-frequent band of 6000 words.1
Notably, such frequency information is not available for all languages, in
which case the target vocabulary can be identified by determining students’
current needs in the L2/Lx context, such as traveling, interpersonal rela-
tionships, specific work requirements, or academic disciplines.
Once the target vocabulary has been identified, decoding and encoding
vocabulary requires three types of knowledge: form, meaning, and use
(Nation, 2001, 2013). Form refers to the sound and pronunciation of a
word, its spelling and components (e.g., stem, suffixes). Meaning includes
the concepts words signify (e.g., ‘What is a ghost?’), how additional
components change their meaning (e.g., haunt → haunted indicating the
past tense) and other lexical and cultural associations (e.g., Halloween).
Finally, use addresses the grammatical functions of lexical items (i.e., is it a
noun or an adverb?), frequent collocations (cooccurring word combina-
tions, such as haunted house), as well as the linguistic and sociolinguistic
constraints surrounding them (e.g., register, appropriate contexts).
Using Nation’s framework, Table 4.1 presents a sample analysis of the
word destructive used in an article on the political situation in Syria,
published in the American newspaper The Washington Post2.
Learners’ lexical knowledge can be described by its breadth (i.e., how
many different words a learner knows) and its depth (i.e., detailed knowl-
edge of the meaning, form, and use of words, and the networks they create
with other words). Both the breadth and depth of lexical knowledge play
important roles in language comprehension and production, although
depth of knowledge seems to facilitate both receptive and productive skills,
because it enhances effective strategy use and successful lexical inferencing
(Gass et al., 2013; Hu & Nassaji, 2014). In addition to breadth and depth of
knowledge, a distinction needs to be made between receptive and
1
This subset may or may not overlap with general high-frequency words from everyday language use.
Also, word-frequency lists are often based on written corpora (compilations of texts) and that some of
the distinctions between moderately and very low-frequency words can be arbitrary.
2
www.washingtonpost.com/world/national-security/after-syria-attack-us-and-russia-tensions-rise-bu
t-military-confrontation-fears-ease/2018/04/14/d7a48d32-3fdb-11e8-a7d1-e4efec6389f0_story.html?u
tm_term=.f40396b0e071
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 85
Table 4.1 Lexical components of destructive
3
www.wordfrequency.info/free.asp
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
86 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
collocations such as water → drinking water or fish out of water (Gass et al.,
2013). In contrast, learners at lower developmental levels mostly make
associations based on phonological similarity, resulting in links between
profond (‘deep’) and professeur (‘professor’), for example, because of the
shared initial syllable (Meara, 1978).
Additionally, lexical knowledge includes recognizing polysemy and
homoformity. Polysemy refers to words with multiple meanings (Gass
et al., 2013), such as leaf, which can denote the green part of a tree, a
page in a book, or an extension to a table. These three versions of leaf are
homonyms, because they are spelled and pronounced the same, but refer to
unrelated concepts (Nation, 2016). Homographs, in contrast, are spelled the
same but are pronounced differently (e.g., desert ‘arid region’ versus desert
‘to leave behind’), while homophones sound the same but are spelled
differently (e.g., cell versus sell). Polysemy and homoformity may cause
miscommunication, but also trigger puns, wordplay, and jokes. Including
them in L2/Lx pedagogy can help learners explore and push the boundaries
of the language, an important aspect of building linguistic and intercul-
tural communicative competence.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 87
between attending to a word’s meaning or its form (e.g., singular/plural,
tense, aspect) helps learners become aware of the form–meaning connection,
which is required for the accurate interpretation of lexical information. This
approach leads to deeper lexical processing and better retention.
A particularly effective approach to teaching vocabulary is in word
groups, collocations, phrasal verbs, and idiomatic expressions (e.g., let me
think about it or You’re kidding) (Boers, Eyckmans, & Stengers, 2007;
Chun & Payne, 2004; Gass et al., 2013; Moeller et al., 2009). Using “fixed
expressions and formulas has an important economizing role in speech
production,” because processing words in groups instead of as individual
words reduces the learning burden (Kecskés, 2019, p. 50). Consequently,
they facilitate both comprehension and production. Hinkel (2017) also
highlights the importance of teaching idiomatic expressions and conven-
tionalized phrases, which are closely linked to specific languages and
cultures. Idiomatic expressions and conventionalized phrases can be diffi-
cult to operationalize – they range from single-word to multi-word expres-
sions (e.g., what?!?! or burning the midnight oil) and can be rigid or
somewhat flexible – they are ubiquitous in both spoken and written
language, in both formal and informal contexts, and can take diverse
forms:
1. frequently recurring and culture-specific expressions with opaque
meanings (e.g., cost an arm and a leg; better late than never)
2. collocations, that is, words that often occur together but with flexible
and variable components (e.g., take place/part/a test/a break; ready to
go/start/close [verb]; give advice/suggestion(s))
3. fixed phrases with specific and well-defined meanings, as well as
phrasal verbs (e.g., break in/out/down/up/into; out of place)
4. figurative expressions (such as metaphors) (e.g., The world is my oyster;
couch potato; heart of gold; heart of stone; melting pot)
5. conversational routines and pre-patterned speech (e.g., Excuse me,
could you tell me where xxx is?; I am sorry I am late; No problem/No
worries)
6. set and rigidly ordered phrases (in which components are fixed in a
certain order) (e.g., here we go; ahead of time; what in the world)
7. proverbs (e.g., two wrongs don’t make a right; no man is an island)
8. culturally bound sayings (e.g., a fish out of water; right as rain; count
your chickens before they hatch)
(Hinkel, 2017, pp. 49–50; reduced examples; emphases mine)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
88 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
These formulaic expressions can have literal and figurative meanings – she’s
with me either implies that someone is physically by my side or that she
shares my opinion – with figurative meaning often being a stumbling block
for L2/Lx learners. Since such expressions number in the thousands (Shin
& Nation, 2008), it is best to focus on the most frequently used phrases
during regular L2/Lx instruction (Arnon & Snider, 2010), with repeated
exposures in all language skills (Hinkel, 2017).
An investment in teaching vocabulary has great positive impact, since
lexical development is exponential: known words serve as springboards for
linking to and networking with increasingly wider ranges of new, related,
and associated words. This includes learners’ existing multilingual lexical
repertoires, since the L1 and L2/Lx lexicons are in communication with
each other (Gass et al., 2013; Jiang, 2004). Thus, exploring cross-linguistic
word associations can support vocabulary development. Cognates – words
with similar historical roots across languages, e.g., Schwester in German and
sister in English – can help activate phonological, orthographic, semantic,
and syntactic aspects of both languages, potentially promoting L2/Lx
lexical acquisition. Explicit connections to other languages that learners
know can be especially helpful at advanced levels of L2/Lx proficiency,
since the academic lexicon, which is similar to specialized lexicons of
professions (e.g., medicine, law, mathematics), draws heavily on words
with Greek and Latin roots, which can serve as built-in scaffolding for
reading and viewing activities that emphasize “meaning-focused input,
language-focused learning, meaning-focused output, and fluency develop-
ment” (Nation, 2001, p. 196).
At the same time, L1–L2/Lx connections can also be problematic. For
example, false cognates, which are words that have divergent meanings in
different languages (e.g., Gift, means ‘poison’ in German but ‘present’ in
English) are misleading. Similarly, semantically overlapping but still distinct
words across languages, such as friendship, which describes a different range
of relationships in French than in English, may cause confusion
(Furstenberg, Levet, English, & Maillet, 2001). Even when lexical items
are shared across languages, they often connote different objects; for many
Americans, the core meaning of bread refers to a sliced white bread, while in
Iran, the word naan (‘bread’) depicts a flat bread. There may be missing
concepts across languages as well. The Japanese word kawaii in general terms
means ‘cute,’ ‘adorable,’ or ‘delicate’ but has no direct English equivalent,
and no English counterpart carries the same affective connotations
(Burdelski & Mitsuhashi, 2010). Collocations also do not readily transfer
from the L1 into the L2/Lx; for example, while you brush your teeth in
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 89
English, you clean them in German (Zähne putzen) and wash them in
Hungarian (fogat mosni). Since textbooks often show individual lexical
items, authentic sources that provide larger meaningful contexts can help
learners avoid incorrect L1–L2/Lx pairings and see words as components of
larger ideational units. Similarly, semantic mind-maps or tasks that ask
learners to paraphrase existing texts can help connect related L2/Lx words
to each other without reliance upon or interference from the L1 (Moeller et
al., 2009).
In addition to cognitive aspects of lexical development, affective issues
should be considered as well. Laufer and Hulstijn (2001) found, for
example, that the more personally interested students are in learning a
word, the more likely they are able to do so. Thus, words that help them
communicate with friends (internal motivation) are learned easier than
words assigned by the teacher (external motivation). Learners invest more
time researching and trying to understand words they are curious about,
pushing them to evaluate their hypotheses regarding the meaning of those
words, confirming, revising, or refining their interpretation as appropriate.
As Byram (1997) proposed, developing proficiency in the L2/Lx is
crucial for intercultural communicative competence; since lexical knowl-
edge is at the heart of meaning-making, it should serve as the primary pillar
in an intercultural communication oriented pedagogy. Furthermore, lex-
ical knowledge should be connected to other components of Byram’s
model, as we explore in the next sections.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
90 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
reflect on the social consequences of linguistic variation, the point of
departure for exploring how to teach vocabulary with an intercultural
communication orientation.
Language Variation
The way people use language reflects geographic, situational, and indivi-
dual variation that telegraphs a speaker’s region of origin, profession, and
other social affiliations. The first indicator, regional variation, is based on
phonological, morphological, syntactic, and lexical differences between
dialects and languages (Myers-Scotton, 2006). Sometimes the distinction
between language and dialect is arbitrary and greatly influenced by socio-
political factors. Dutch and German are oft-quoted examples where regio-
nal varieties (e.g., Bavarian, spoken in southern Germany and Plattdeutsch
spoken in northern Germany) might be more different phonologically and
morphologically, and less mutually comprehensible, than Dutch and
certain German dialects (Fagan, 2009).
Due to historical sociopolitical factors, one or two language varieties
might emerge as privileged. These standard varieties typically represent
the language to outsiders (e.g., used in dictionaries), reflect the speech of
people with education, power, and prestige, and are used by national
media. In comparison, other varieties of the language are viewed as ‘non-
standard,’ because they diverge from the ‘standard’ phonologically, lexi-
cally, morphologically, and/or syntactically. Standard varieties of
languages coexist with regional dialects in diglossic situations, in which
each variety is deployed in different contexts of use (e.g., the standard
variety might be used at work and the dialect at home). Although, from a
linguistic standpoint, all languages are equally valid and valuable, non-
standard varieties sometimes enjoy less symbolic capital (i.e., value placed
on them by society) than ‘standard’ languages; these differences often
maintain existing social and power structures (Bourdieu, 1993; Lippi-
Green, 2012).
Languages can also have multiple standard varieties. These languages
are called pluricentric and include Arabic, Chinese, English, French,
German, Portuguese, and Spanish (Fagan, 2009; Myers-Scotton,
2006). Given the stature of English as a world language, spoken by the
most speakers around the globe as a native or additional language, it has
received significant attention for its variation. Kachru (2008) coined the
term World Englishes to describe its spread and distribution, grouped in
three concentric circles:
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 91
1. The inner circle represents countries in which English is spoken as a
native language, including Australia, Canada, Ireland, New Zealand,
the United Kingdom, and the United States. The standard varieties
spoken in these countries are traditionally seen as socially and eco-
nomically more powerful and prestigious (~380 million users of
English).
2. The outer circle includes countries with a colonial past, where English
is spoken by broad segments of society as a first, second, or additional
language learned from childhood, such as some countries in the
Caribbean, India, Kenya, or the Philippines (~350–450 million
people).
3. In the outermost expanding circle, we find countries without a colonial
past, where English is spoken as an L2/Lx. Given the important
economic, political, and social role that English plays around the
globe, this circle includes all remaining countries (~750 million
individuals).
The varieties spoken in the three circles have enjoyed different levels of
prestige over time and still reflect current and historical relationships
among countries, cultural groups, and individuals. Raising learners’ aware-
ness of the value society places on different language varieties pertains to
Byram’s (1997) notion of knowledge and should be included in an inter-
culturally oriented L2/Lx pedagogy.
There is an additional reason for addressing pluricentricity: the surface-
level similarity between these language varieties may (mis)lead speakers
into a false sense of security, even though smooth communication is not
guaranteed. Lexical mismatches (e.g., flat v. apartment between British and
U.S. American English) or divergent pronunciation (e.g., the Arabic
expression for ‘good morning’ is pronounced as SabaaH iklheer in Cairo
but as SbaH klher in Fez; Brustad, Al-Batal, & Al-Tonsi, 2014) may trip up
interactants. Awareness of these distinctions not only facilitates commu-
nication among speakers of different languages, but also of different
varieties of the same language.
In addition to regional differences, variation reflects gender, education,
ethnicity, and spheres of activity (e.g., hobbies). We adopt these social
dialects intentionally or inadvertently and signal social group membership
to our interlocutors (Myers-Scotton, 2006); we may use more or less
formal language, specific vocabulary (e.g., pertaining to a profession), or
phonological features. For example, while giving a speech at a conference,
we use sophisticated and technical vocabulary (e.g., examine the dataset)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
92 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
and careful grammar. In informal communicative events, in contrast,
formal language might come across as overly reserved or distancing, so
we tend to use the vernacular (informal) style (Labov, 1972). This style
features high-frequency words, contractions, creative spelling (e.g., amirite
for am I right in texts), and colloquial expressions such as couch potato (for a
person who watches too much television).
Gender identity also affects linguistic variation, from the need for
distinct feminine and masculine suffixes at the end of nouns signifying
professions (e.g., ingeniero (m) – ingeniera (f) [engineer] in Spanish), to
discursive differences in policy-making in the U.N. regarding violence
about women (Shepherd, 2010). Gender-based differences can be found
in pronunciation as well. In Cairene and Jordanian Arabic, for example,
gender, age, and class manifest phonologically (Abudalbuh, 2011; Clopper,
2009; Mavisakalyan, 2015; Sadiqi, 2008), helping women convert “femin-
ism as a political, economic and cultural movement into a linguistic
movement where women opt to distinguish themselves linguistically
from men” (Abudalbuh, 2011, p. 43).
Language variation also has a temporal aspect. Due to constant and
rapid changes in technology, globalization, and world events, new words
are coined and existing words are expanded or narrowed to signify novel
meanings. Examples abound, especially surrounding technological
innovation:
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 93
“the phrase best represents ‘the public discourse and preoccupations’ of
2016.”4
As native speakers and experienced nonnative users of languages over
time, we develop the competence necessary to recognize language varieties,
connect them to the social personae they represent, and use these varieties
in different communicative contexts ourselves. Most language users are
eventually able to shift between social dialects (sociolects), styles, and
registers, as appropriate for specific social situations. The ability to decode
and encode social identity through language is an important skill for L2/Lx
learners and users, but this ability is complicated by developmental
variation.
4
www.wired.com/2017/01/word-of-year-dumpster-fire/
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
94 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
French or Italian in the United States – or if a language user wants to
convey a certain personality, like Henry Kissinger maintaining his accent
to emphasize his image as a German professor and thus be taken more
seriously, there might be little impetus to adapt to L2/Lx pronunciation
(Myers-Scotton, 2006). Moreover, speaking an L2/Lx too well can lead to
difficulties. As Piller (2016) relates, in 2009, a boat carrying Sri Lankan
asylum seekers to Australia was intercepted; their leader, who had trained
at a U.S. call-center spoke English with an American accent and used the
anglicized name ‘Alex.’ The Sri Lankan High Commissioner questioned
the group’s refugee status, due to Alex’s excellent English skills, putting
their asylum claim in jeopardy.
In contrast, in some bilingual communities, learners do not ever develop
sufficient proficiency in the L2/Lx and retain significant nonnative-like
accents and language skills. This is the situation for many ethnic
Hungarians in the Hungarian-majority part of Romania (Transylvania),
where students only study Romanian at school, in very limited contexts of
use, and often fail to learn it well enough to pass graduation requirements
(Piller, 2016). Their lack of fluency subsequently limits students’ access to
higher education or employment.
The situation in multilingual speech communities is not always dire,
and members of these communities easily shift between languages,
although not randomly. One of the determiners of language choice is the
domain of interaction, which Fishman (1972) divided into five categories:
(1) home, (2) work, (3) neighborhood, (4) church/religion, and (5) public
affairs. For example, someone may speak English in educational settings
but Haitian Creole with her family and friends, although participants at
home may also code switch – use two languages in the same conversation – a
phenomenon frequently observed in bilingual communication (Myers-
Scotton, 2006). Multilingual people also tend to have strengths in one
language for some of these domains and use their other language(s) for the
other domains.
These are some of the issues that multilingual speakers negotiate as they
traverse diverse linguistic and cultural realms. When their lexical knowl-
edge is insufficient for a particular interaction, they may have to resort to
communication strategies to make themselves understood.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 95
one of the most difficult things to learn in a new language. The challenges
are numerous. We might not have learned a particular word yet or cannot
think of it due to processing demands (e.g., because we are paying atten-
tion to grammar, pragmatics, or other interpersonal needs). Such lexical
difficulties occur most often at lower levels of L2/Lx development, when
language users have not yet learned conventions of usage or a speech
community’s preferred ways of saying things (Kecskés, 2019). Within the
linguistic system, vocabulary is of primary importance, so it is especially
important for learners to develop skills to compensate for missing or
misunderstood words to facilitate communication. In 1997, Yule and
Tarone identified a number of strategies for filling gaps in lexical knowl-
edge, organized into achievement/compensation, reduction, and interac-
tion strategies (see Table 4.2). The interactive and reduction strategies are
most useful to language learners early on, while achievement and compen-
satory strategies require some amount of L2/Lx competence, and are used
frequently in L1 communication as well, enhancing or livening up a
speaker’s message.
Using these strategies effectively can help ameliorate communication
breakdown or repair miscommunication, especially at lower levels of L2/Lx
development when the need to compensate for lexical knowledge is higher
(Littlemore, 2003; Rosas Maldonado, 2016). Teaching both the strategies
themselves and the metalanguage to describe them can promote reflective
learning and should be included in intercultural communication-oriented
pedagogy.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
Table 4.2 Communication strategies (examples based on Yule & Tarone, 1997, p. 20)
Appeal for assistance Asking for help (how do you say . . .), comprehension check
Mime Acting out swimming
Gesture Using thumbs-up to signal ‘good’ or pointing to an icon on a map,
Interactive
if the speaker does not know the word for ‘castle’
Message abandonment Speaker does not know how to say three o’clock so they respond
with I don’t know when asked the time
Topic avoidance Speaker lacks the technical vocabulary required for explaining
Reduction
what is wrong with the car
Approximation Saying blue and green instead of teal
Semantic contiguity
Analogy It is like the color of the sea
Antonymy Not good for discouraging
speakers)
used by L2/Lx
Metonymy Using one word to represent a different idea, such as crown for the
Royal Family in the U.K.
Conceptual
color, shape – high-
Achievement / Compensatory
Repetition Repeating a word, perhaps with a rising intonation to signal
uncertainty
Lx competence)
Exemplification Providing one or more examples, such as car, bus, or subway to
explain transportation
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
Borrowing Using a word from the L1, retaining the L1 pronunciation, within
the surrounding L2/Lx message
Foreignizing Using the L1 word for an item with L2/Lx phonology, hoping that
it is transferable (e.g., theater → *teater in Greek, which is close
enough to the actual Greek word θέατρο, hopefully
recognizable for the listener
Code
Literal translation Literal translation of an L1 word into the L2, expecting a one-to-
one transfer, such as confiture de circulation (i.e., ‘traffic
marmalade’ in French) when trying to express traffic jam
Word coinage Creation of *hétután (‘next week’) in Hungarian, following the
Summary
As this chapter has demonstrated, lexical knowledge is essential for com-
municating with members of other cultures. By emphasizing vocabulary in
their teaching, L2/Lx instructors contribute significantly to learners’ ability
to communicate with members of other cultures and speakers of other
languages. However, there are additional ways in which lexical knowledge
can be mapped onto intercultural communication, in terms of the guide-
lines presented in Chapter 2 (Byram, 1997; Hua, 2014; Kramsch, 2009;
Liddicoat & Scarino, 2013) and the concepts discussed in Chapter 1 (e.g.,
Hall, 1976; Hofstede, et al., 2010; Kluckhohn & Strodtbeck, 1961). The
following synthesis models possible interrelationships among these threads
of research, illustrating what an intercultural communication-oriented
pedagogy might entail:
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 99
In the next section, two teaching activities are presented that reflect
some of the pedagogical goals. They also promote deeper levels of analysis
and reflection at increased levels of proficiency.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
100 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Sample Teaching Activities
ACTIVITY SET 1
L2/Lx: Greek
Learner group: Novice/A1 learners
Source: Advertisement from a Greek supermarket chain called
Bazaar
Duration: 1–2 lessons
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, students will be able to:
1. read and identify
• the labels for individual items (e.g., milk, ham),
• their superordinate category (e.g., dairy, vegetables),
• the culturally appropriate way to measure items (e.g., kilogram,
bunch),
• the cost of various items and actual prices (e.g., the ad lists the price
of a kilogram of meat, but individual packages vary in weight),
include the currency used in Greece;
2. negotiate and create a grocery list they would purchase for a specific
occasion (e.g., birthday party), using the correct vocabulary, within an
anticipated budget;
3. reflect on different items that may be purchased at a grocery store (versus
pharmacy or open-air market) in Greece compared to students’ home
culture.
Analytic Approach
Multimodal analysis. Students learn to interpret the complementary
semiotic systems of images, words (including multilingual labels, which
can tap into learners’ world knowledge and multilingual skills to
identify objects), numbers, colors, and their meanings (e.g., bottle caps
often indicate sparkling or plain water), as well as how information is
organized.
Steps in the Task
Pre-Reading
1. Looking at images, students identify the authentic source material (i.e.,
flyer/advertisement for a supermarket).
2. Students search for and find cognates, using their knowledge of Greek
orthography and referential knowledge (e.g., recognizing images of
fruit).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 101
Reading
Working in pairs or small groups, students sort the groceries into
superordinate categories, what they like/dislike or what they have/need. As a
team, they use skills of interpreting and discovery as they clarify the
meaning of new lexical items in a culturally contextualized environment,
overcome mismatches in expected and actual content, and explore costs
associated with food in Greece.
Post-Reading
1. Students create a list of items they need for their event and ensure that
their purchases fit their budget – if available, budgets for several socio-
economic groups could be shared with learners to understand better the
sociocultural context (e.g., how much a teacher or a street vendor earns
each month).
• Provide learners with formulaic phrases they need for the task
We (don’t) need . . .
We still have plenty of . . .
I (don’t) like . . .
Do you like . . . / Don’t you like . . .
2. In small groups, students can act out shopping in short skits, including:
• asking for help from a salesperson: can you show me where [item x] is?
or where can I find [item x]? using chunked phrases;
• learning and exchanging information about whether they
need a shopping cart or a basket,
need a plastic bag or say thanks, we have one, and
be paying by bankcard or in cash;
• identifying useful communication strategies: pointing to items
whose name they do not know, guessing from context, gesturing, etc.
[Other students should take notes on each group’s skit, what they
need and how they plan to pay, to foster listening
comprehension.]
Analysis
These pre- during- and post-reading tasks provide sufficient repetition
of targeted words, focusing on meaning in a rich cultural context. The
multimodal presentation and practice foster deeper cognitive processing
and generate a fuller semantic domain of shopping (i.e., paradigmatic
organization), connecting it to real-world products. Additionally, the
multimodal environment – images of food and advertising tools,
multilingual text, numbers, cultural information, etc. – engages
students both cognitively and affectively, increasing their L2/Lx lexical
knowledge and improving their attitude (curiosity) towards the new
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
102 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
ACTIVITY SET 2
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, students will be able to do the following:
1. Recognize several features of the regional varieties of English used by the
actors, for now focusing on the lexicon: phonology, form, and meaning,
including features that overlap with other varieties learners know and
those that are local. Because three of the main characters are Australian,
while the others are from New Zealand, the material lends itself well to
detailed comparisons of language varieties used by multilingual
speakers.
2. Identify styles of language used in the movie: casual and formal,
including contexts-of-use, and social dialects (e.g., young people,
members of the local population).
3. Identify language and terminology – and the underlying cultural
implications – used to define kinship, friendships, acquaintances, and
professional relationships, as well as topics that are discussed in these
communities.
4. Identify language used to describe and celebrate life events (e.g., gra-
duation from school, getting a job, a cricket match) or to grieve (e.g.,
loss of friendship, death).
5. Describe several societal dimensions (e.g., power distance
relationships, masculine/feminine), orientation towards time, high-
versus low-context communication, or personal space; the number of
features to be analyzed depends on how many
episodes can be watched.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 103
Analytic Approaches
Ethnography of communication and interactional sociolinguistics.
Viewing
1. Watch one or more episodes in segments of 15–20 minutes (pausing at a
natural break in the story line, such as a shift of events), expanding the
list of participants and settings introduced:
• check back with the class after each segment, to ensure compre-
hension (it serves as a foundation for analysis).
2. Divide the class into four groups, each taking notes on one of the
following details in Episode 1 and subsequent episodes, using evidence
from the film:
• ends: shared interactional expectations, rules for interaction, the
objectives participants want to accomplish;
• act sequence: the content of the communicative event (i.e., referential
information exchanged) and how participants interact (e.g., turn-
taking, collaborative or competitive overlap);
• key: the tone of the event, whether it is humorous or serious
• instrumentalities: message form, the type of communication used
(for this task, the group should focus on specific lexical areas, such as
idioms, metaphors or puns).
3. Half-way through the episode, each group presents its finding to the rest
of the class, so that the second half is viewed with the information
serving as an interpretive lens for studying the lexicon.
Post-Viewing
1. Groups might want to re-watch selected scenes and discuss the norms of
interpretation focusing specifically on the lexicon (e.g., Were there
difficulties between the participants in terms of vocabulary?) and the
genre (e.g., What are the characteristics of communication in this
television series? What types of language – regional varieties, styles,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
104 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
registers – are used and for what purposes? How does the language of
young people vary from that of older characters? What other social
dialects were you able to identify?)
2. Learners, working in small groups, write short scenes, which can be
presented orally or in writing, and identify:
• the setting,
• the participants,
• the purpose of the interaction,
• the act sequence,
• the key,
• the instrumentalities (re-creating the appropriate style, form, con-
tent, and use of language we would expect these characters to speak),
• applying norms of interpretation, and
• framing their scene according to the TV series’ genre.
Analysis
Extended texts, such as films, not only offer repeated exposure to the same
vocabulary, deepening learners’ lexical knowledge, but also detailed
information about contexts of use for that vocabulary. The visual support
available in films, as well as adapting the content into different modalities or
formats (e.g., creating a skit) further stabilizes that knowledge. Additionally,
integrating vocabulary practice with expanding culturally relevant world
knowledge – such as the organization of families or friendships in different
cultures – supports the development of intercultural communicative
competence.
It is good to keep in mind that no one activity set will result in intercultural
communicative competence as regards the lexicon. Rather, this process requires
numerous iterations in the curriculum over time. Furthermore, these tasks can
supplement existing curricula and need not become the entire curriculum; they
are flexible in their adoption.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 105
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
chapter 4
Chapter Overview
One of the most important contributors to the comprehension and pro-
duction of the L2/Lx is the lexicon, the focus of the first pedagogical
chapter. After briefly reviewing relevant research regarding the develop-
ment of the L2/Lx lexicon, the discussion turns to issues in language
variation and communication strategies that L2/Lx users might draw on,
when they lack the necessary vocabulary to convey their intended meaning.
Since research into L2/Lx lexical acquisition is vast, a full review is impos-
sible in this space; therefore, this chapter focuses specifically on findings
that are most relevant for promoting intercultural communicative compe-
tence. The chapter concludes with a proposal of connecting L2/Lx research
on the lexicon to concepts in intercultural communicative competence,
including two sets of language teaching activities.
83
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
84 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
used words carry a significant amount of the communicative load in an L2/
Lx. The most frequent 100 words in English, for example, account for as
much as 50 percent of texts, while the 1000 most frequent words cover 70–
90 percent of most reading texts (Nation, 2013). Nation (2016) and Schmitt
(2014) suggest focusing on the highest-frequency words (ca 3000 words and
word families) in the first years of L2 instruction, while subsequent years
should push the development of a mid-frequent band of 6000 words.1
Notably, such frequency information is not available for all languages, in
which case the target vocabulary can be identified by determining students’
current needs in the L2/Lx context, such as traveling, interpersonal rela-
tionships, specific work requirements, or academic disciplines.
Once the target vocabulary has been identified, decoding and encoding
vocabulary requires three types of knowledge: form, meaning, and use
(Nation, 2001, 2013). Form refers to the sound and pronunciation of a
word, its spelling and components (e.g., stem, suffixes). Meaning includes
the concepts words signify (e.g., ‘What is a ghost?’), how additional
components change their meaning (e.g., haunt → haunted indicating the
past tense) and other lexical and cultural associations (e.g., Halloween).
Finally, use addresses the grammatical functions of lexical items (i.e., is it a
noun or an adverb?), frequent collocations (cooccurring word combina-
tions, such as haunted house), as well as the linguistic and sociolinguistic
constraints surrounding them (e.g., register, appropriate contexts).
Using Nation’s framework, Table 4.1 presents a sample analysis of the
word destructive used in an article on the political situation in Syria,
published in the American newspaper The Washington Post2.
Learners’ lexical knowledge can be described by its breadth (i.e., how
many different words a learner knows) and its depth (i.e., detailed knowl-
edge of the meaning, form, and use of words, and the networks they create
with other words). Both the breadth and depth of lexical knowledge play
important roles in language comprehension and production, although
depth of knowledge seems to facilitate both receptive and productive skills,
because it enhances effective strategy use and successful lexical inferencing
(Gass et al., 2013; Hu & Nassaji, 2014). In addition to breadth and depth of
knowledge, a distinction needs to be made between receptive and
1
This subset may or may not overlap with general high-frequency words from everyday language use.
Also, word-frequency lists are often based on written corpora (compilations of texts) and that some of
the distinctions between moderately and very low-frequency words can be arbitrary.
2
www.washingtonpost.com/world/national-security/after-syria-attack-us-and-russia-tensions-rise-bu
t-military-confrontation-fears-ease/2018/04/14/d7a48d32-3fdb-11e8-a7d1-e4efec6389f0_story.html?u
tm_term=.f40396b0e071
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 85
Table 4.1 Lexical components of destructive
3
www.wordfrequency.info/free.asp
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
86 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
collocations such as water → drinking water or fish out of water (Gass et al.,
2013). In contrast, learners at lower developmental levels mostly make
associations based on phonological similarity, resulting in links between
profond (‘deep’) and professeur (‘professor’), for example, because of the
shared initial syllable (Meara, 1978).
Additionally, lexical knowledge includes recognizing polysemy and
homoformity. Polysemy refers to words with multiple meanings (Gass
et al., 2013), such as leaf, which can denote the green part of a tree, a
page in a book, or an extension to a table. These three versions of leaf are
homonyms, because they are spelled and pronounced the same, but refer to
unrelated concepts (Nation, 2016). Homographs, in contrast, are spelled the
same but are pronounced differently (e.g., desert ‘arid region’ versus desert
‘to leave behind’), while homophones sound the same but are spelled
differently (e.g., cell versus sell). Polysemy and homoformity may cause
miscommunication, but also trigger puns, wordplay, and jokes. Including
them in L2/Lx pedagogy can help learners explore and push the boundaries
of the language, an important aspect of building linguistic and intercul-
tural communicative competence.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 87
between attending to a word’s meaning or its form (e.g., singular/plural,
tense, aspect) helps learners become aware of the form–meaning connection,
which is required for the accurate interpretation of lexical information. This
approach leads to deeper lexical processing and better retention.
A particularly effective approach to teaching vocabulary is in word
groups, collocations, phrasal verbs, and idiomatic expressions (e.g., let me
think about it or You’re kidding) (Boers, Eyckmans, & Stengers, 2007;
Chun & Payne, 2004; Gass et al., 2013; Moeller et al., 2009). Using “fixed
expressions and formulas has an important economizing role in speech
production,” because processing words in groups instead of as individual
words reduces the learning burden (Kecskés, 2019, p. 50). Consequently,
they facilitate both comprehension and production. Hinkel (2017) also
highlights the importance of teaching idiomatic expressions and conven-
tionalized phrases, which are closely linked to specific languages and
cultures. Idiomatic expressions and conventionalized phrases can be diffi-
cult to operationalize – they range from single-word to multi-word expres-
sions (e.g., what?!?! or burning the midnight oil) and can be rigid or
somewhat flexible – they are ubiquitous in both spoken and written
language, in both formal and informal contexts, and can take diverse
forms:
1. frequently recurring and culture-specific expressions with opaque
meanings (e.g., cost an arm and a leg; better late than never)
2. collocations, that is, words that often occur together but with flexible
and variable components (e.g., take place/part/a test/a break; ready to
go/start/close [verb]; give advice/suggestion(s))
3. fixed phrases with specific and well-defined meanings, as well as
phrasal verbs (e.g., break in/out/down/up/into; out of place)
4. figurative expressions (such as metaphors) (e.g., The world is my oyster;
couch potato; heart of gold; heart of stone; melting pot)
5. conversational routines and pre-patterned speech (e.g., Excuse me,
could you tell me where xxx is?; I am sorry I am late; No problem/No
worries)
6. set and rigidly ordered phrases (in which components are fixed in a
certain order) (e.g., here we go; ahead of time; what in the world)
7. proverbs (e.g., two wrongs don’t make a right; no man is an island)
8. culturally bound sayings (e.g., a fish out of water; right as rain; count
your chickens before they hatch)
(Hinkel, 2017, pp. 49–50; reduced examples; emphases mine)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
88 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
These formulaic expressions can have literal and figurative meanings – she’s
with me either implies that someone is physically by my side or that she
shares my opinion – with figurative meaning often being a stumbling block
for L2/Lx learners. Since such expressions number in the thousands (Shin
& Nation, 2008), it is best to focus on the most frequently used phrases
during regular L2/Lx instruction (Arnon & Snider, 2010), with repeated
exposures in all language skills (Hinkel, 2017).
An investment in teaching vocabulary has great positive impact, since
lexical development is exponential: known words serve as springboards for
linking to and networking with increasingly wider ranges of new, related,
and associated words. This includes learners’ existing multilingual lexical
repertoires, since the L1 and L2/Lx lexicons are in communication with
each other (Gass et al., 2013; Jiang, 2004). Thus, exploring cross-linguistic
word associations can support vocabulary development. Cognates – words
with similar historical roots across languages, e.g., Schwester in German and
sister in English – can help activate phonological, orthographic, semantic,
and syntactic aspects of both languages, potentially promoting L2/Lx
lexical acquisition. Explicit connections to other languages that learners
know can be especially helpful at advanced levels of L2/Lx proficiency,
since the academic lexicon, which is similar to specialized lexicons of
professions (e.g., medicine, law, mathematics), draws heavily on words
with Greek and Latin roots, which can serve as built-in scaffolding for
reading and viewing activities that emphasize “meaning-focused input,
language-focused learning, meaning-focused output, and fluency develop-
ment” (Nation, 2001, p. 196).
At the same time, L1–L2/Lx connections can also be problematic. For
example, false cognates, which are words that have divergent meanings in
different languages (e.g., Gift, means ‘poison’ in German but ‘present’ in
English) are misleading. Similarly, semantically overlapping but still distinct
words across languages, such as friendship, which describes a different range
of relationships in French than in English, may cause confusion
(Furstenberg, Levet, English, & Maillet, 2001). Even when lexical items
are shared across languages, they often connote different objects; for many
Americans, the core meaning of bread refers to a sliced white bread, while in
Iran, the word naan (‘bread’) depicts a flat bread. There may be missing
concepts across languages as well. The Japanese word kawaii in general terms
means ‘cute,’ ‘adorable,’ or ‘delicate’ but has no direct English equivalent,
and no English counterpart carries the same affective connotations
(Burdelski & Mitsuhashi, 2010). Collocations also do not readily transfer
from the L1 into the L2/Lx; for example, while you brush your teeth in
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 89
English, you clean them in German (Zähne putzen) and wash them in
Hungarian (fogat mosni). Since textbooks often show individual lexical
items, authentic sources that provide larger meaningful contexts can help
learners avoid incorrect L1–L2/Lx pairings and see words as components of
larger ideational units. Similarly, semantic mind-maps or tasks that ask
learners to paraphrase existing texts can help connect related L2/Lx words
to each other without reliance upon or interference from the L1 (Moeller et
al., 2009).
In addition to cognitive aspects of lexical development, affective issues
should be considered as well. Laufer and Hulstijn (2001) found, for
example, that the more personally interested students are in learning a
word, the more likely they are able to do so. Thus, words that help them
communicate with friends (internal motivation) are learned easier than
words assigned by the teacher (external motivation). Learners invest more
time researching and trying to understand words they are curious about,
pushing them to evaluate their hypotheses regarding the meaning of those
words, confirming, revising, or refining their interpretation as appropriate.
As Byram (1997) proposed, developing proficiency in the L2/Lx is
crucial for intercultural communicative competence; since lexical knowl-
edge is at the heart of meaning-making, it should serve as the primary pillar
in an intercultural communication oriented pedagogy. Furthermore, lex-
ical knowledge should be connected to other components of Byram’s
model, as we explore in the next sections.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
90 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
reflect on the social consequences of linguistic variation, the point of
departure for exploring how to teach vocabulary with an intercultural
communication orientation.
Language Variation
The way people use language reflects geographic, situational, and indivi-
dual variation that telegraphs a speaker’s region of origin, profession, and
other social affiliations. The first indicator, regional variation, is based on
phonological, morphological, syntactic, and lexical differences between
dialects and languages (Myers-Scotton, 2006). Sometimes the distinction
between language and dialect is arbitrary and greatly influenced by socio-
political factors. Dutch and German are oft-quoted examples where regio-
nal varieties (e.g., Bavarian, spoken in southern Germany and Plattdeutsch
spoken in northern Germany) might be more different phonologically and
morphologically, and less mutually comprehensible, than Dutch and
certain German dialects (Fagan, 2009).
Due to historical sociopolitical factors, one or two language varieties
might emerge as privileged. These standard varieties typically represent
the language to outsiders (e.g., used in dictionaries), reflect the speech of
people with education, power, and prestige, and are used by national
media. In comparison, other varieties of the language are viewed as ‘non-
standard,’ because they diverge from the ‘standard’ phonologically, lexi-
cally, morphologically, and/or syntactically. Standard varieties of
languages coexist with regional dialects in diglossic situations, in which
each variety is deployed in different contexts of use (e.g., the standard
variety might be used at work and the dialect at home). Although, from a
linguistic standpoint, all languages are equally valid and valuable, non-
standard varieties sometimes enjoy less symbolic capital (i.e., value placed
on them by society) than ‘standard’ languages; these differences often
maintain existing social and power structures (Bourdieu, 1993; Lippi-
Green, 2012).
Languages can also have multiple standard varieties. These languages
are called pluricentric and include Arabic, Chinese, English, French,
German, Portuguese, and Spanish (Fagan, 2009; Myers-Scotton,
2006). Given the stature of English as a world language, spoken by the
most speakers around the globe as a native or additional language, it has
received significant attention for its variation. Kachru (2008) coined the
term World Englishes to describe its spread and distribution, grouped in
three concentric circles:
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 91
1. The inner circle represents countries in which English is spoken as a
native language, including Australia, Canada, Ireland, New Zealand,
the United Kingdom, and the United States. The standard varieties
spoken in these countries are traditionally seen as socially and eco-
nomically more powerful and prestigious (~380 million users of
English).
2. The outer circle includes countries with a colonial past, where English
is spoken by broad segments of society as a first, second, or additional
language learned from childhood, such as some countries in the
Caribbean, India, Kenya, or the Philippines (~350–450 million
people).
3. In the outermost expanding circle, we find countries without a colonial
past, where English is spoken as an L2/Lx. Given the important
economic, political, and social role that English plays around the
globe, this circle includes all remaining countries (~750 million
individuals).
The varieties spoken in the three circles have enjoyed different levels of
prestige over time and still reflect current and historical relationships
among countries, cultural groups, and individuals. Raising learners’ aware-
ness of the value society places on different language varieties pertains to
Byram’s (1997) notion of knowledge and should be included in an inter-
culturally oriented L2/Lx pedagogy.
There is an additional reason for addressing pluricentricity: the surface-
level similarity between these language varieties may (mis)lead speakers
into a false sense of security, even though smooth communication is not
guaranteed. Lexical mismatches (e.g., flat v. apartment between British and
U.S. American English) or divergent pronunciation (e.g., the Arabic
expression for ‘good morning’ is pronounced as SabaaH iklheer in Cairo
but as SbaH klher in Fez; Brustad, Al-Batal, & Al-Tonsi, 2014) may trip up
interactants. Awareness of these distinctions not only facilitates commu-
nication among speakers of different languages, but also of different
varieties of the same language.
In addition to regional differences, variation reflects gender, education,
ethnicity, and spheres of activity (e.g., hobbies). We adopt these social
dialects intentionally or inadvertently and signal social group membership
to our interlocutors (Myers-Scotton, 2006); we may use more or less
formal language, specific vocabulary (e.g., pertaining to a profession), or
phonological features. For example, while giving a speech at a conference,
we use sophisticated and technical vocabulary (e.g., examine the dataset)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
92 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
and careful grammar. In informal communicative events, in contrast,
formal language might come across as overly reserved or distancing, so
we tend to use the vernacular (informal) style (Labov, 1972). This style
features high-frequency words, contractions, creative spelling (e.g., amirite
for am I right in texts), and colloquial expressions such as couch potato (for a
person who watches too much television).
Gender identity also affects linguistic variation, from the need for
distinct feminine and masculine suffixes at the end of nouns signifying
professions (e.g., ingeniero (m) – ingeniera (f) [engineer] in Spanish), to
discursive differences in policy-making in the U.N. regarding violence
about women (Shepherd, 2010). Gender-based differences can be found
in pronunciation as well. In Cairene and Jordanian Arabic, for example,
gender, age, and class manifest phonologically (Abudalbuh, 2011; Clopper,
2009; Mavisakalyan, 2015; Sadiqi, 2008), helping women convert “femin-
ism as a political, economic and cultural movement into a linguistic
movement where women opt to distinguish themselves linguistically
from men” (Abudalbuh, 2011, p. 43).
Language variation also has a temporal aspect. Due to constant and
rapid changes in technology, globalization, and world events, new words
are coined and existing words are expanded or narrowed to signify novel
meanings. Examples abound, especially surrounding technological
innovation:
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 93
“the phrase best represents ‘the public discourse and preoccupations’ of
2016.”4
As native speakers and experienced nonnative users of languages over
time, we develop the competence necessary to recognize language varieties,
connect them to the social personae they represent, and use these varieties
in different communicative contexts ourselves. Most language users are
eventually able to shift between social dialects (sociolects), styles, and
registers, as appropriate for specific social situations. The ability to decode
and encode social identity through language is an important skill for L2/Lx
learners and users, but this ability is complicated by developmental
variation.
4
www.wired.com/2017/01/word-of-year-dumpster-fire/
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
94 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
French or Italian in the United States – or if a language user wants to
convey a certain personality, like Henry Kissinger maintaining his accent
to emphasize his image as a German professor and thus be taken more
seriously, there might be little impetus to adapt to L2/Lx pronunciation
(Myers-Scotton, 2006). Moreover, speaking an L2/Lx too well can lead to
difficulties. As Piller (2016) relates, in 2009, a boat carrying Sri Lankan
asylum seekers to Australia was intercepted; their leader, who had trained
at a U.S. call-center spoke English with an American accent and used the
anglicized name ‘Alex.’ The Sri Lankan High Commissioner questioned
the group’s refugee status, due to Alex’s excellent English skills, putting
their asylum claim in jeopardy.
In contrast, in some bilingual communities, learners do not ever develop
sufficient proficiency in the L2/Lx and retain significant nonnative-like
accents and language skills. This is the situation for many ethnic
Hungarians in the Hungarian-majority part of Romania (Transylvania),
where students only study Romanian at school, in very limited contexts of
use, and often fail to learn it well enough to pass graduation requirements
(Piller, 2016). Their lack of fluency subsequently limits students’ access to
higher education or employment.
The situation in multilingual speech communities is not always dire,
and members of these communities easily shift between languages,
although not randomly. One of the determiners of language choice is the
domain of interaction, which Fishman (1972) divided into five categories:
(1) home, (2) work, (3) neighborhood, (4) church/religion, and (5) public
affairs. For example, someone may speak English in educational settings
but Haitian Creole with her family and friends, although participants at
home may also code switch – use two languages in the same conversation – a
phenomenon frequently observed in bilingual communication (Myers-
Scotton, 2006). Multilingual people also tend to have strengths in one
language for some of these domains and use their other language(s) for the
other domains.
These are some of the issues that multilingual speakers negotiate as they
traverse diverse linguistic and cultural realms. When their lexical knowl-
edge is insufficient for a particular interaction, they may have to resort to
communication strategies to make themselves understood.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 95
one of the most difficult things to learn in a new language. The challenges
are numerous. We might not have learned a particular word yet or cannot
think of it due to processing demands (e.g., because we are paying atten-
tion to grammar, pragmatics, or other interpersonal needs). Such lexical
difficulties occur most often at lower levels of L2/Lx development, when
language users have not yet learned conventions of usage or a speech
community’s preferred ways of saying things (Kecskés, 2019). Within the
linguistic system, vocabulary is of primary importance, so it is especially
important for learners to develop skills to compensate for missing or
misunderstood words to facilitate communication. In 1997, Yule and
Tarone identified a number of strategies for filling gaps in lexical knowl-
edge, organized into achievement/compensation, reduction, and interac-
tion strategies (see Table 4.2). The interactive and reduction strategies are
most useful to language learners early on, while achievement and compen-
satory strategies require some amount of L2/Lx competence, and are used
frequently in L1 communication as well, enhancing or livening up a
speaker’s message.
Using these strategies effectively can help ameliorate communication
breakdown or repair miscommunication, especially at lower levels of L2/Lx
development when the need to compensate for lexical knowledge is higher
(Littlemore, 2003; Rosas Maldonado, 2016). Teaching both the strategies
themselves and the metalanguage to describe them can promote reflective
learning and should be included in intercultural communication-oriented
pedagogy.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
Table 4.2 Communication strategies (examples based on Yule & Tarone, 1997, p. 20)
Appeal for assistance Asking for help (how do you say . . .), comprehension check
Mime Acting out swimming
Gesture Using thumbs-up to signal ‘good’ or pointing to an icon on a map,
Interactive
if the speaker does not know the word for ‘castle’
Message abandonment Speaker does not know how to say three o’clock so they respond
with I don’t know when asked the time
Topic avoidance Speaker lacks the technical vocabulary required for explaining
Reduction
what is wrong with the car
Approximation Saying blue and green instead of teal
Semantic contiguity
Analogy It is like the color of the sea
Antonymy Not good for discouraging
speakers)
used by L2/Lx
Metonymy Using one word to represent a different idea, such as crown for the
Royal Family in the U.K.
Conceptual
color, shape – high-
Achievement / Compensatory
Repetition Repeating a word, perhaps with a rising intonation to signal
uncertainty
Lx competence)
Exemplification Providing one or more examples, such as car, bus, or subway to
explain transportation
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
Borrowing Using a word from the L1, retaining the L1 pronunciation, within
the surrounding L2/Lx message
Foreignizing Using the L1 word for an item with L2/Lx phonology, hoping that
it is transferable (e.g., theater → *teater in Greek, which is close
enough to the actual Greek word θέατρο, hopefully
recognizable for the listener
Code
Literal translation Literal translation of an L1 word into the L2, expecting a one-to-
one transfer, such as confiture de circulation (i.e., ‘traffic
marmalade’ in French) when trying to express traffic jam
Word coinage Creation of *hétután (‘next week’) in Hungarian, following the
Summary
As this chapter has demonstrated, lexical knowledge is essential for com-
municating with members of other cultures. By emphasizing vocabulary in
their teaching, L2/Lx instructors contribute significantly to learners’ ability
to communicate with members of other cultures and speakers of other
languages. However, there are additional ways in which lexical knowledge
can be mapped onto intercultural communication, in terms of the guide-
lines presented in Chapter 2 (Byram, 1997; Hua, 2014; Kramsch, 2009;
Liddicoat & Scarino, 2013) and the concepts discussed in Chapter 1 (e.g.,
Hall, 1976; Hofstede, et al., 2010; Kluckhohn & Strodtbeck, 1961). The
following synthesis models possible interrelationships among these threads
of research, illustrating what an intercultural communication-oriented
pedagogy might entail:
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 99
In the next section, two teaching activities are presented that reflect
some of the pedagogical goals. They also promote deeper levels of analysis
and reflection at increased levels of proficiency.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
100 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Sample Teaching Activities
ACTIVITY SET 1
L2/Lx: Greek
Learner group: Novice/A1 learners
Source: Advertisement from a Greek supermarket chain called
Bazaar
Duration: 1–2 lessons
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, students will be able to:
1. read and identify
• the labels for individual items (e.g., milk, ham),
• their superordinate category (e.g., dairy, vegetables),
• the culturally appropriate way to measure items (e.g., kilogram,
bunch),
• the cost of various items and actual prices (e.g., the ad lists the price
of a kilogram of meat, but individual packages vary in weight),
include the currency used in Greece;
2. negotiate and create a grocery list they would purchase for a specific
occasion (e.g., birthday party), using the correct vocabulary, within an
anticipated budget;
3. reflect on different items that may be purchased at a grocery store (versus
pharmacy or open-air market) in Greece compared to students’ home
culture.
Analytic Approach
Multimodal analysis. Students learn to interpret the complementary
semiotic systems of images, words (including multilingual labels, which
can tap into learners’ world knowledge and multilingual skills to
identify objects), numbers, colors, and their meanings (e.g., bottle caps
often indicate sparkling or plain water), as well as how information is
organized.
Steps in the Task
Pre-Reading
1. Looking at images, students identify the authentic source material (i.e.,
flyer/advertisement for a supermarket).
2. Students search for and find cognates, using their knowledge of Greek
orthography and referential knowledge (e.g., recognizing images of
fruit).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 101
Reading
Working in pairs or small groups, students sort the groceries into
superordinate categories, what they like/dislike or what they have/need. As a
team, they use skills of interpreting and discovery as they clarify the
meaning of new lexical items in a culturally contextualized environment,
overcome mismatches in expected and actual content, and explore costs
associated with food in Greece.
Post-Reading
1. Students create a list of items they need for their event and ensure that
their purchases fit their budget – if available, budgets for several socio-
economic groups could be shared with learners to understand better the
sociocultural context (e.g., how much a teacher or a street vendor earns
each month).
• Provide learners with formulaic phrases they need for the task
We (don’t) need . . .
We still have plenty of . . .
I (don’t) like . . .
Do you like . . . / Don’t you like . . .
2. In small groups, students can act out shopping in short skits, including:
• asking for help from a salesperson: can you show me where [item x] is?
or where can I find [item x]? using chunked phrases;
• learning and exchanging information about whether they
need a shopping cart or a basket,
need a plastic bag or say thanks, we have one, and
be paying by bankcard or in cash;
• identifying useful communication strategies: pointing to items
whose name they do not know, guessing from context, gesturing, etc.
[Other students should take notes on each group’s skit, what they
need and how they plan to pay, to foster listening
comprehension.]
Analysis
These pre- during- and post-reading tasks provide sufficient repetition
of targeted words, focusing on meaning in a rich cultural context. The
multimodal presentation and practice foster deeper cognitive processing
and generate a fuller semantic domain of shopping (i.e., paradigmatic
organization), connecting it to real-world products. Additionally, the
multimodal environment – images of food and advertising tools,
multilingual text, numbers, cultural information, etc. – engages
students both cognitively and affectively, increasing their L2/Lx lexical
knowledge and improving their attitude (curiosity) towards the new
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
102 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
ACTIVITY SET 2
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, students will be able to do the following:
1. Recognize several features of the regional varieties of English used by the
actors, for now focusing on the lexicon: phonology, form, and meaning,
including features that overlap with other varieties learners know and
those that are local. Because three of the main characters are Australian,
while the others are from New Zealand, the material lends itself well to
detailed comparisons of language varieties used by multilingual
speakers.
2. Identify styles of language used in the movie: casual and formal,
including contexts-of-use, and social dialects (e.g., young people,
members of the local population).
3. Identify language and terminology – and the underlying cultural
implications – used to define kinship, friendships, acquaintances, and
professional relationships, as well as topics that are discussed in these
communities.
4. Identify language used to describe and celebrate life events (e.g., gra-
duation from school, getting a job, a cricket match) or to grieve (e.g.,
loss of friendship, death).
5. Describe several societal dimensions (e.g., power distance
relationships, masculine/feminine), orientation towards time, high-
versus low-context communication, or personal space; the number of
features to be analyzed depends on how many
episodes can be watched.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 103
Analytic Approaches
Ethnography of communication and interactional sociolinguistics.
Viewing
1. Watch one or more episodes in segments of 15–20 minutes (pausing at a
natural break in the story line, such as a shift of events), expanding the
list of participants and settings introduced:
• check back with the class after each segment, to ensure compre-
hension (it serves as a foundation for analysis).
2. Divide the class into four groups, each taking notes on one of the
following details in Episode 1 and subsequent episodes, using evidence
from the film:
• ends: shared interactional expectations, rules for interaction, the
objectives participants want to accomplish;
• act sequence: the content of the communicative event (i.e., referential
information exchanged) and how participants interact (e.g., turn-
taking, collaborative or competitive overlap);
• key: the tone of the event, whether it is humorous or serious
• instrumentalities: message form, the type of communication used
(for this task, the group should focus on specific lexical areas, such as
idioms, metaphors or puns).
3. Half-way through the episode, each group presents its finding to the rest
of the class, so that the second half is viewed with the information
serving as an interpretive lens for studying the lexicon.
Post-Viewing
1. Groups might want to re-watch selected scenes and discuss the norms of
interpretation focusing specifically on the lexicon (e.g., Were there
difficulties between the participants in terms of vocabulary?) and the
genre (e.g., What are the characteristics of communication in this
television series? What types of language – regional varieties, styles,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
104 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
registers – are used and for what purposes? How does the language of
young people vary from that of older characters? What other social
dialects were you able to identify?)
2. Learners, working in small groups, write short scenes, which can be
presented orally or in writing, and identify:
• the setting,
• the participants,
• the purpose of the interaction,
• the act sequence,
• the key,
• the instrumentalities (re-creating the appropriate style, form, con-
tent, and use of language we would expect these characters to speak),
• applying norms of interpretation, and
• framing their scene according to the TV series’ genre.
Analysis
Extended texts, such as films, not only offer repeated exposure to the same
vocabulary, deepening learners’ lexical knowledge, but also detailed
information about contexts of use for that vocabulary. The visual support
available in films, as well as adapting the content into different modalities or
formats (e.g., creating a skit) further stabilizes that knowledge. Additionally,
integrating vocabulary practice with expanding culturally relevant world
knowledge – such as the organization of families or friendships in different
cultures – supports the development of intercultural communicative
competence.
It is good to keep in mind that no one activity set will result in intercultural
communicative competence as regards the lexicon. Rather, this process requires
numerous iterations in the curriculum over time. Furthermore, these tasks can
supplement existing curricula and need not become the entire curriculum; they
are flexible in their adoption.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
4 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Vocabulary 105
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 05:10:45, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.005
chapter 5
Chapter Overview
This chapter explores the relationships between grammar and intercultural
communicative competence. Specifically, it discusses what we mean by
grammar, recent scholarship regarding the nature of L2 grammatical
development, and key concepts in teaching L2 grammar. This discussion
is followed by a reevaluation of the native speaker ideal, morphosyntactic
language variation, the local situatedness of grammar, and deploying
grammar for creating social meaning. The chapter concludes with practical
suggestions for teaching L2/Lx grammar with an intercultural communi-
cation orientation.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 107
English, while the latter is missing a complement, because the verb to buy
usually requires a direct object.
Like morphology, syntax is language specific; what is acceptable in one
language might not be so in another. Because English has few morphemes
that indicate syntactic relationships, its word order is relatively inflexible.
Other languages (e.g., Finnish) with complex derivational and inflectional
systems in which pre-, in- or suffixes express relationships among compo-
nents of a sentence are more flexible. In English, for example the following
two sentences have distinct meanings determined by word order:
Anneli loves Jaakko. Jaako loves Anneli.
In Finnish, however, word order merely shifts emphasis (i.e., that it is
Jaakko and not someone else, whom Anneli loves) but does not change the
meaning of the sentence, since the partitive -a suffix clearly identifies
Jaakko as the object of Anneli’s affections:
Anneli rakastaa Jaakkoa. Jaakkoa rakastaa Anneli.
While psycholinguistic research informs us about the way the brain
organizes grammatical information, and grammar books tell us what is
considered ‘right’ and what is ‘wrong,’ neither explains how grammar is
implemented in social interaction. Yet, what is deemed correct can be
fairly subjective and context dependent. A casual conversation might
sound ‘stuffy’ with rigidly correct grammar, whereas a professional
presentation might sound ‘sloppy’ if the speaker or language user does
not attend carefully to grammar. Two concepts are relevant here: pre-
scriptive grammar proposes how a language should be used, while descrip-
tive grammar describes how a language is actually used by people,
including native speakers of the language in real interactions (Gass
et al., 2013). For example, me and my friends is not ‘proper’ English if
used as the subject of a verb, but this phrase is used extensively among
young people in casual conversation in the United States, where the
grammatically accurate my friends and I would sound stilted. Navigating
grammatical expectations and rules can be tricky even for native speak-
ers. This is especially true in the L2/Lx, over which most learners and
users have incomplete control, due to developmental factors.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
108 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
languages. A key hypothesis in first-language acquisition – that there is
a critical period, usually around age 10–12, before which children can learn
a language rather easily, but after which language learning is more challen-
ging if not impossible (Lenneberg, 1967) – has been influential in second
language acquisition (SLA) as well. The arguments behind this claim are
that children have superior neural plasticity (flexibility), are more willing to
experiment with language and do not worry about making mistakes, or
that their main cognitive focus is on communication (Hartshorne,
Tenenbaum, & Pinker, 2018). The primary limitation for L2 speakers is
in pronunciation (i.e., not sounding like a native speaker), but morphology
and syntax are also affected (Dekeyser, 2018). Thus, the argument holds,
nativelike performance among individuals who begin to learn an L2 after
the critical period is impossible, although some may acquire near-native
skills (Abrahamsson & Hyltenstam, 2009; Hyltenstam, 2018).
Nonnativeness ranges from the readily noticeable to the barely perceptible,
although psycholinguistic disparities are measurable with imaging even at
the most advanced levels of bi-/multilingual competence (Gass et al., 2013).
Other scholars, however, contend that successful L2 learning and
attaining near-native and native levels of competence is possible even
if the age-of-onset (when someone begins to learn an L2/Lx) is after
the critical age period (Birdsong, 2006, 2014, 2017; Birdsong &
Gertken, 2013; Hernandez, Li, & MacWhinney, 2005). This argument
is supported by a large-scale study of almost 700,000 respondents,
which led Hartshorne, Tenenbaum, and Pinker (2018) to extend the
critical age period to 17.4 years, significantly later than puberty,
which, the authors argue, might signal the beginning of decline rather
than its upper boundary. The authors also claimed that age is not
inherently limiting, and instead, L2 attainment is influenced by
a combination of other variables, such as motivation (cf. Dörnyei &
Skehan, 2003; Flege, 2018), language aptitude, working memory, the
learner’s L1 (typological similarity to the L21), the amount of time
spent in an L2-speaking environment using the L2 (Birdsong, 2006;
Flege, 2018), or to what extent an L2 user identifies with other L2
speakers (Moyer, 2014). Finally, the authors argued that age is not
simply a cognitive factor, but also a social one; around age 18 young
adults in most cultures finish school and begin working, and, unlike
1
Languages that are closely related to adult speakers’ L1 will be easier to learn (e.g., Amharic and
Hebrew) than languages that are typologically different (e.g., Malayalam and Maltese).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 109
young children, they are no longer able to focus primarily or almost
exclusively on language learning.
In a related vein, McLaughlin (1987) observed that, while children learn
more effortlessly or reach native attainment easily, they do not learn more
efficiently than adult L2 learners, who:
• have better cognitive skills because they are already aware of concepts
onto which the new lexicon needs to be mapped instead of having to
learn both simultaneously;
• have more world knowledge to inform their L2 learning and possess
significant declarative knowledge of their L1 that can facilitate SLA;
• are aware of sociolinguistic constraints on language use, such as know-
ing that different grammatical forms convey meaning (e.g., casual/
formal style, polite/impolite speech).
Just as importantly, imperfect knowledge of L2 grammar does not prevent
an individual from being able to communicate effectively. Instead, mis-
takes and errors are common even in L1 performance and in an L2 may
simply reflect stages of learners’ interlanguage development or incomplete
integration of linguistic data that learners are continually trying to make
sense of (Ellis & Wulff, 2015; Gass et al., 2013; Selinker, 1972). Yet,
cognitive SLA research typically compares notions of accuracy in L2
learners to idealized native speaker norms (Birdsong, 2006), although
this perspective has been changing slowly, as the profession reconsiders
what we expect learners to know, and how we can help them navigate
intercultural communication successfully in spite of incomplete grammar
systems.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
110 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Declarative versus Procedural Knowledge
One way to understand knowledge is to consider its accessibility.
Declarative and procedural knowledge, which have been mentioned briefly
before, merit further explication here, due to their relevance for grammar
instruction. Declarative knowledge is knowledge about language, for exam-
ple knowing the fact that Catalan distinguishes between formal and
informal pronouns for ‘you’: singular informal tu and formal vostè, and
plural informal vosaltres and formal vostès. In contrast, procedural knowledge
is knowing how to do something. Motor skills (e.g., pronunciation) and
cognitive skills that require information sequencing (e.g., encoding and
decoding sentences) may be difficult to access explicitly, we simply know
how to interpret what we hear and produce our own utterances.
Declarative knowledge is explicit, including facts that we are aware of
and can explain. Conversely, procedural knowledge is mostly implicit,
something we know intuitively, tacitly, without necessarily being able to
articulate the rules underlying language use. It is “the unconscious knowl-
edge of a much larger body of information that is the basis of automatic,
spontaneous use of language” (Little, 1994, p. 103). Scholarship currently
holds that procedural knowledge is mainly utilized by children when
acquiring their L1, whereas older learners rely more on declarative knowl-
edge to learn their L2/Lx (Gass et al., 2013; O’Grady, 2006).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 111
While there is some debate regarding the efficacy of implicit versus
explicit instruction, a meta-analysis of empirical studies by Norris and
Ortega (2000) found overwhelming evidence that explicit instruction does
not harm adult L2 learners and actually makes grammar development more
successful. Explicit instruction seems to be particularly helpful for teaching
more complex grammatical structures (Spada & Tomita, 2010). So, the
question we should entertain in L2/Lx pedagogy is not whether to teach
grammar explicitly (except for young children or illiterate adults), but
rather how to teach it while keeping the focus on meaningful real-world
language use.
2
http://hindiurduflagship.org/assets/pdf/Skeleton_Grammar.pdf
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
112 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Inductive learning, in contrast, exposes learners to contextualized
input, which learners first process for meaning, then use as a data
source for recognizing patterns, identifying the rules that guide
those patterns. Inductive learning may lead to either explicit or
implicit instruction, depending on whether the rules are explicitly
verbalized or if learners simply incorporate them implicitly into
their developing interlanguage. In an inductive lesson, the gramma-
tical information from Figure 5.1 would be presented in a reading text
or dialog, based on which the learners would try to determine the
rules for conjugating and using the verb to be in Hindi or for
interpreting the meaning of ‘to.’
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 113
should include input, drawing learners’ attention to important forms,
interaction for practicing structures, followed by opportunities for lan-
guage use completing real-world tasks.
Exercise Types
A key objective of L2 pedagogy is to prepare learners for effective language
use in the L2/Lx. This objective has been met with mixed success.
Textbooks were supposed to move away from mechanical grammar prac-
tice that had been common for teaching Latin or Classical Greek, but
progress has been slow and uneven. A cursory review of ESL textbooks
suggests more adherence to communicative goals, whereas foreign-
language (e.g., Arabic, French, German, Spanish) textbooks in the
United States continue to focus on grammar, offering numerous mechan-
ical and meaningful activities, with fewer opportunities for genuine com-
municative practice. The distinction between these activity types is
important to understand for fostering real-world L2/Lx use.
In mechanical exercises, rule practice is the objective, as in the following
example from German:
Conjugate the underlined verb according to each pronoun provided in
parentheses.
1. Ich habe zwei Schwester. (er, wir, du, ‘I have two sisters’ (‘he,’ ‘we,’ ‘you’ sg.,
sie pl.) ‘they’)
To complete this exercise, learners do not have to understand the meaning
either of the sentence or the pronouns, just manipulate the verb form based
on a chart in the textbook.
Meaningful practice, in contrast, requires comprehension, albeit with
limited responses, such as yes or no, or pre-provided choices, here in Italian:
1. Sai andare a cavallo? ‘Do you know how to ride a horse?’
2. Preferisci andare a passeggio o in ‘Do you prefer to walk or ride a bike?’
bicicletta?
Finally, communicative activities prompt learners to provide original
and creative information. Responses in these types of activities are open-
ended and unpredictable, reflecting learners’ genuine and unlimited
experiences, as the following Welsh examples illustrate:
1. Ble hoffech chi deithio? ‘Where would you like to travel?’
2. Pwy fyddai’n gwmni teithio delfrydol? ‘Who would be an ideal travel companion?’
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
114 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
In natural communication, meaningful and communicative exchanges
co-mingle, including yes-no (numbers 1 and 2 below) and multiple-choice
(number 3) questions. These examples are from a job interview with
a U.S. company that requires traveling or relocating to another country:
1. Have you ever lived, studied or worked outside of the United States?
2. Can you imagine living and working overseas for the next five years (as
our company expands its international presence)?
3. If you were placed at one of our overseas offices, would you have
a preference for Africa, Asia, or Europe?
While these questions may start out with a yes-no or multiple-choice
format, they may evolve into an open-ended exchange, depending on the
intentions of the questioner and respondent. These activities also reflect
real-world tasks, an ideal model for L2/Lx pedagogy.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 115
to study engineering in France should complete tasks that are similar
to the ones they are expected to perform at their host institution).
4. Holistic language learning: tasks should draw on multiple real-world
contexts to integrate form, function, and sociocultural meaning (e.g.,
knowing how to wish someone a happy birthday entails learning the
appropriate linguistic structures, selecting cards that are appropriate
for the recipient, and knowing how – if at all – birthdays are celebrated
in that culture).
5. Clearly defined communicative outcome: the purpose of tasks and the
language necessary for completing them should be shared with the learners,
along with an explanation of how they are relevant for real-world L2 use.
As these criteria indicate, tasks resemble or are communicative acts that
we perform in everyday life, depending on our age, personal or academic
interests, among other factors. Tasks vary in cognitive demand, requiring
simple or complex vocabulary, grammar, discourse structure, and socio-
linguistic or pragmatic knowledge. Learning to write a shopping list entails
finding the relevant vocabulary, with little grammatical knowledge beyond
plural forms. Writing an email to set up a meeting requires more formal
vocabulary and more precise grammar, while working with Ebola victims3
to try to stem the spread of the disease demands complex linguistic and
cultural knowledge: vocabulary, grammar to express politeness or identify-
ing narrative frames (yesterday I had a minor headache, and today it is much
worse), as well as pragmatic and discourse skills (see Chapter 6), such as
terms of address, rules for turn-taking, and cultural knowledge (e.g., who
decides when to seek medical help and from whom).
Ideally, task sets comprise multiple steps. Pre-tasks help build or activate
relevant vocabulary, discourse structure, or background knowledge and create
a plan of action for completing the task (Ellis & Yuan, 2004). The main task
may also comprise multiple subtasks, each of which allows learners to con-
centrate on different aspects of language, instead of trying to fulfill competing
demands all at once (Abrams, 2017, 2019). Afterwards, post-tasks should help
learners practice and reinforce the knowledge and skills they have gained,
including repetition or expansion, reflections or reiteration in a different
modality (e.g., write a term paper based on an in-class oral debate). This
sequence should help learners solidify and refine their knowledge through
repeated practice of the same or similar tasks (Bygate, 2006; Samuda &
Bygate, 2008). At every stage, meaningful language use – both in terms of
3
https://edition.cnn.com/2018/06/05/health/ebola-contact-tracing-culture-explainer/index.html
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
116 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
content and interest to the learner – and experiential learning are of primary
importance. Learners can complete (sub)tasks individually, in pairs, or in small
groups, combining various linguistic and sociolinguistic components to be
able to perform similar tasks in real-world intercultural communication.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 117
native speaker a failure? Such a narrow conceptualization of native-
speakerness discriminates against users of ‘nonstandard’ varieties of lan-
guages as well, whether we think of variation in terms of dialects and
pluricentricity or style and register, morphosyntactic differences are key
features of language variation.
Hyperfocusing on the monolingual native speaker model is problematic,
given the fact that “[m]onolingualism is neither the typical condition nor
the gold standard” in today’s world (Mauranen, 2014). Additionally, L1s
and L2s/Lxs reflect enormous variation, including complementary gram-
mar systems, whose use depends on the context of interaction. In German
there are both national and regional differences in the choice of auxiliary
verbs for the present perfect tense (e.g., ich habe gestanden ‘I stood’ in
northern Germany, but ich bin gestanden in southern Germany, Austria,
and Switzerland – see Duden4) or diminutive endings (e.g., -chen in
German Standard German, but -li in Swiss Standard German, -erl in the
Bavarian or -le in the Swabian dialect), while the use of the genitive case
reflects both dialectic and register-based variation (Fagan, 2009; Scott,
2011; Sick, 2004).
Even in geographically coherent communities, morphosyntactic fea-
tures can vary considerably, reflecting social and cultural patterns in
specific contexts of language use. Based on over 100 hours of recordings
of Bequian natives in three communities, Walker and Meyerhoff (2006)
identified several morphosyntactic features of Caribbean Creole English
that differ not only from ‘standard’ varieties of English, but also from the
local acrolect, the most prestigious dialect on the island of Bequia. The
acrolect and the local dialects are deployed in different interactional situa-
tions, in formal and informal exchanges, for example, even within the same
speech community. Differences include variable markings of subject-verb
agreement (e.g., If he find that people, you know, is a yes-man), the presence
of negative concord in negation (e.g., Well I ain’t into no jazz) or zero
copula (e.g., She ∅ gon run to see me), among other morphosyntactic
elements (Walker & Meyerhoff, 2006, p. 152).
While it takes experience to develop the necessary skills, competent
speakers can adjust the variety of language they speak in different situa-
tions, deploying more or less formal registers, or knowing when and with
whom to use which sociolects. Additionally, individuals can choose to flout
norms to separate themselves from social expectations. Counter-cultural
poetry, jazz, rap, and other genres of music and writing intentionally
4
www.duden.de/sprachwissen/sprachratgeber/Perfektbildung-mit-haben-oder-sein
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
118 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
separate the author from accepted societal norms, including linguistic
choices, such as double negatives (not no-how) or replacing he/she with
they as the preferred pronoun used by members of the Lesbian, Gay,
Bisexual, Transgender, and Queer community (Gelder, 2007; Lafrance,
2016).
Highly proficient L2/Lx users further complicate the concept of
native speakerness. Some L2/Lx users have extraordinary control of
vocabulary and grammar, rhetoric and pragmatics, even though they
have a noticeable accent in the L2/Lx (Davies, 2003; Dewaele, 2017).
Yet, their overall language skills might be better than that of less-
educated native speakers. The acknowledgment that multilinguals are
not deficient monolinguals is a welcome shift in L2/Lx research. We
have come a long way since adult L2 learning was viewed as an
“ineluctable failure” to reach native-like levels “to different degrees”
(Bley-Vroman, 1989, p. 7). Building on work by Cook (1999) and
Cook and Singleton (2014), Dewaele describes L2 users not as “defec-
tive monolingual native speakers or – even worse – failed native speak-
ers” (2017, p. 236), but rather as individuals who have a repertoire of
knowledge in multiple languages (Kamhi-Stein, 2016). In fact, some
scholars argue that multilingual language users have access to a broader
range of linguistic and cognitive skills to communicate than mono-
lingual L1 speakers, and may use any and all languages that they know
(Murahata, Muruhata, & Cook, 2016).
Given the breadth of L1 and L2/Lx language experience, models for
teaching grammar must include various sources of information from
both monolingual and multilingual speakers, because such variety
reflects linguistic reality. Moreover, strategy training for comprehend-
ing and interacting with other speakers – of varying L1s and L2s/Lxs
at different degrees of proficiency – should form part of any gramma-
tical curriculum, since understanding what someone says sets the stage
for being able to interact with members of other cultures successfully.
Diverse sources will also help illustrate the vast array of communica-
tive contexts in which multilingual intercultural individuals deploy
grammatical knowledge.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 119
friends in casual conversations than my friends and I, which sounds stilted
to most young people in the United States. Incomplete sentences similarly
abound, such as reporters on TV concluding their comments with Back to
you [name of anchor], a waiter asking customers if they want Rice or French
fries? or when people use they even with single subjects to avoid gender
specification (it is not accurate according to prescriptive grammar, but
widely accepted in casual spoken and written language).
Coherent discourse also frequently contains grammatically ‘incorrect’
language that makes sense globally, as the following excerpt from
a detective novel by Scottish author Stuart McBride5 illustrates:
5
Stuart McBride (2016) In the Cold Dark Ground. New York, NY: Harper Collins. Excerpt from
p. 175.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
120 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
grammatical accuracy is often limited, while communication remains feasible,
as we see in this brief exchange that took place between a museum guard in
Florence, Italy and an American visitor, whose children ran ahead:
Museum guard: It is good, children be with parents.
American tourist: Oh, ok, I’ll let the kids know to stay with us.
Museum guard: [Now not so many tourists, but later
many tourists. Then not good.
American tourist: Of course. Let me call them back.
Even though the guard’s utterances fell short of prescriptive norms, the
tourist understood her message. The willingness of participants to under-
stand one another, and their ability to think flexibly, goes a long way to
making communication successful; they will work to make sense of each
other’s comments, using contextual cues and world knowledge, even if
utterances are not entirely grammatically accurate. This openness requires
letting go of prescriptive expectations and committing to working with the
speaker’s available language.
Descriptive-grammar based approaches to grammar teaching are
increasingly common (Cook, 2016; Shrum & Glisan, 2015), yet many
educators still expect accuracy from learners, even if it is not feasible (i.e.,
learnability issues) or necessary (i.e., common usage). In order to help
learners interact in real-world situations, L2/Lx pedagogy should acknowl-
edge the variability of grammar across dialects and styles, and its locally
situated, collaboratively constructed nature.
Summary
This chapter offered an overview of research regarding the development of
L2/Lx grammar. Due to its importance for conveying precise meaning and
for expressing social identity, grammar plays an essential role in intercul-
tural communication. Current pedagogical practices, such as task-based
language teaching (Ellis, 2003, 2005; Samuda & Bygate, 2008), already
place great emphasis on the relationship between grammatical form and
real-world meaning, and this section lays out potential further connections
between grammatical knowledge and intercultural communication.
Following the concepts discussed in Chapter 1 and the guidelines presented
in Chapter 2, the synthesis below models an intercultural communication
oriented pedagogy.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 121
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
122 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Sample Teaching Activities
ACTIVITY SET 1
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, learners will be able to do the following:
1. Recognize the formation of the superlative form of adjectives (e.g.,
coldest, oldest, highest, lowest, most) – in both the video and the article as
complementary auditory and textual input.
2. Learn about the difference between Fahrenheit and Celsius.
3. Identify alternative expressions for describing extreme weather (e.g., all-
time record).
4. Identify words and phrases pertaining to the superlative form
of cold (e.g., temperature, degrees below zero, plummet, plunge,
subzero)
5. Recognize regions of the United States and identify expected and
unexpected weather patterns.
Analytic Approach
Multimodal analysis.
6
https://weather.com/news/climate/news/coldest-temperature-recorded-50-states
7
National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration | U.S. Department of Commerce; www
.noaa.gov/
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 123
cold
wet
winter
Alaska
3. Introduce the conversion between Fahrenheit and Celsius, identifying
what temperatures learners are used to during the winter where they
come from. Learners can also discuss who has experienced the coldest
temperatures.
Task
1. Watch the video, supported by true-false questions to ensure content
comprehension
2. Watch the video again and make a list of the state names that are
mentioned [Wisconsin, North Dakota, Alaska, Montana, Florida,
Hawaii].
3. Read the first two sections “At a Glance” and the short paragraphs below
that (“When winter approaches . . . When taking a close look at the data
there are some surprises.”); as a class, identify the theme of the article
[coldest temperatures reported in the 50 states].
4. Scan the article and underline or highlight all instances of the superlative
form of adjectives (e.g., most, coldest, oldest, warmest).
5. Have learners examine the weather map in the article. What do the
colors mean? (e.g., warmest to coldest temperatures). The class should
identify the states they recognize or consult a map that has the names of
the states, to expand their knowledge of U.S. geography.
6. Scan the rest of the article to identify the subsections (I. Continental
states with below-zero temperatures, II. Overall records, III. Regional
differences). Working in three groups, learners fill out the matrix
(Group 1 – section I, etc.) and prepare mini-reports on the facts (i.e.,
dates, locations, temperatures), words and phrases that describe winter
weather (e.g., degrees below zero, flurries, subzero, Arctic outbreak).
Learners take notes during the other presentations.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
124 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Record Interesting/
temperatures Other useful useful words
(date, degrees details about about winter
Section Region(s) State(s) in Fahrenheit) the weather weather
I
II
III
Analysis
This task fosters intercultural orientation in several ways. First, the target
grammar is presented in a meaningful context, including cultural
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 125
ACTIVITY SET 2
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, learners will be able to do the following:
1. Identify different ways to formulate questions in German (yes-no questions
with inverted word order, questions with question words, tag questions
[oder? na? ja? ~ the pragmatic equivalents of ‘well’ or ‘what do you think’],
rhetorical questions).
2. Identify how responses are formulated for each type of question.
3. Recognize the difference between formal and informal questions, as reflected
in verb conjugation, pronoun use, and grammatical completeness (e.g.,
formal questions typically receive grammatically more complete responses,
while less formal questions yield more fluid responses).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
126 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
4. Recognize the cultural meaning of formal and informal questions, how they
signal relationships between strangers or coworkers on one hand, and
friendship or youth on the other (keeping in mind that interpretations are
specific to particular interactions and reflect complex relationships).
5. Recognize that questions and responses are often resolved over multiple
turns
Analytic Approach
Ethnography of communication and interactional sociolinguistics
Steps in the Task
Pre-Task
1. As a class, watch a short segment at the beginning of the episode and ask
learners to identify the participants and what they are doing.
2. Watch the segment again, asking the learners to pay attention to the
relationships between the speakers and support their claim (guide them
to observe language form, but they should be encouraged to use any and
all available clues).
Task
In small groups, review a copy of the transcript and identify different question
types, organizing them into a matrix, based on the following features:
Response type
Question/
Formal/ Complete/ sentence pair
Question informal incomplete or multi-turn Other
type address sentence? response? observations
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 127
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
128 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Analysis
While authentic films are layered through multiple filters, from the
screenwriter to the director, in most cases they draw on realistic language
use. Thus, the dialogs can be used for analyzing question and answer
patterns in German. Importantly for teaching grammar with an
intercultural communication orientation, the dialogs depict real-world
language, since not all responses are grammatically accurate or complete,
and utterances also include fragments and false starts. These ‘inaccuracies’
offer excellent opportunities for exploring register, style, and social
variation. Another realistic feature of the exchanges is that many questions
are resolved across multiple turns (instead of quick question-answer pairs, as
typically modeled by language textbooks). Successful L2/Lx use requires
skills for interpreting and producing such discourse, and authentic resources
can offer accessible modeling.
Throughout the analyses, learners should support their claims with
linguistic evidence from the video regarding question types and the social
meaning they imply, in order to get into the habit of recursive thinking,
where they use linguistic data to generate hypotheses and then use evidence
to confirm, revise, refine, or reject those hypotheses. Analyzing interactional
data and comparing patterns across languages and social contexts helps
learners move beyond viewing grammar as abstract rules to recognizing it as
a real-world communicative tool.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
5 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Grammar 129
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:44:27, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.006
chapter 6
Chapter Overview
Pragmatics refers to the way we understand and use language in social
situations. It “examines the choices made by an individual speaker on
a particular occasion to signal meanings such as relative differences in role,
status, age, social distance, and so on” (LoCastro, 2003, p. 21). Calling it her
“cocktail party’ definition,” Bardovi-Harlig (2013) describes pragmatics as the
study of “how-to-say-what-to-whom-when” (pp. 68–69), referring to the
linguistic choices language users make in social interaction and the effect
they have on their interlocutors as they communicate. Whether in an L1 or
another language, there is ample opportunity for pragmatic mishaps, which
are actually responsible for the majority of L2/Lx miscommunication
(Bardovi-Harlig & Dörnyei, 1998). House (2003) echoes this sentiment
regarding the role of pragmatics in intercultural communication:
[T]he most interesting and puzzling misunderstandings in intercultural inter-
actions will not stem from mishearing, mispronouncing or misusing lexico-
grammatical rules, rather they are likely to arise as a result of a failure to
interpret . . . hidden meanings underlying discourse structures. As work on
communicative styles, politeness phenomena, indirect speech acts, Gricean
implicatures and neo-Gricean relevance theory have revealed, much of ‘nor-
mal talk’ is indirect, which means, of course, that if we want to understand
our interlocutor’s contributions . . . we must ‘infer’ their intentions in order to
find out how the words they use are ‘really meant’. (p. 23)
In order to set the stage for an interculturally oriented pedagogy that
emphasizes pragmatics, it is useful to understand pragmatics in more detail.
What Is Pragmatics?
Pragmatics reflects the interaction “among language, language users, and
contexts of use” (Taguchi & Kim, 2018, p. 14). That is, the linguistic
130
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
6 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Pragmatics 131
features we use index different attitudes and social relationships between
participants, and learning a second language entails figuring out new and
different ways of expressing ideas (i.e., linguistic form), and learning what
social meaning our linguistic choices imply (cf. Culpeper, Marti, Mei,
Nevala, & Schauer, 2010; Kecskés, 2012; Mills, 2009). This process can be
challenging, since what is appropriate in a new linguistic and social context
“is particularly sensitive to linguistic and nonlinguistic context including
users and places,” and thus may be different in each unique conversation
(Bardovi-Harlig, 2013, p. 80). Moreover, interaction is fluid, “achieved
jointly through the dynamic emergence of meaning in conversation”
(p. 600). That is, interactions are collaboratively produced in a local
context, shaped by the participants and their goals for communicating.
The dynamic nature of interactions makes it even more difficult to figure
out expected behaviors beyond individual interactions (Arnold, 1999;
Bella, 2011, 2012; Blattner & Fiori, 2011; Culpeper et al., 2010).
Therefore, instead of teaching pragmatic patterns as universals within
a broader culture (e.g., Germans always greet each other with ‘Guten
Tag ’), it is more fruitful to understand how to analyze the way pragmatic
practices are enacted.
Participating effectively in collaborative exchanges requires pragmalin-
guistic and sociopragmatic skills (Abrams, 2016b; Alcón Soler, 2018;
Bardovi-Harlig, 2013; Bella, 2011, 2012; Cenoz, 2007; Martinez-Flor &
Fukuya, 2005; Taguchi & Kim, 2018). Pragmalinguistic knowledge entails
linguistic features that we use as expressive resources, such as the use of
pronouns (e.g., informal and formal ‘you’ forms), intensifiers (e.g., super
nul [‘it really sucks,’ adjective, French]) or mitigators (e.g., somewhat),
terms of address (e.g., sensei [‘teacher,’ Japanese]), conventional expressions
(e.g., Cik tas maksā? [‘How much is this?’ Latvian]), and speech acts (e.g.,
Would you like to join us?). Sociopragmatic knowledge, in contrast, refers to
a speaker’s ability to know how to use language in social contexts: how to
partake in culturally appropriate practices, when to use specific pragma-
linguistic features, with whom and how. It also allows us to flout social
norms and understand the potential consequences for not adhering to
expectations (e.g., creating humor or being shunned by a community). We
continuously ‘read’ (interpret) complex and subtle cues in order to com-
municate with our interlocutors. For example, the general rule of thumb
for determining which form of the Hungarian equivalent of ‘you’ is to use
te when speaking with friends, family, children, or pets, whereas Ön is used
with adults or strangers. However, a sales person at an H&M clothing store
in Budapest is likely to greet shoppers using the informal pronoun to
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
132 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
humor
pragmatics
(im)politeness modifiers
conventional
expressions
highlight the youthful context, and social media also tends to promote
informality, regardless of the participants’ level of acquaintance. Such
paralinguistic and sociopragmatic knowledge guides our interpretation
and use of various aspects of pragmatics (see Figure 6.1), the focus of the
following sections.
Speech Acts
Speech acts are routinized exchanges such as greetings and leave-taking
sequences, making suggestions or requests, giving compliments or advice,
or expressing agreement or disagreement (Austin, 1975). Basic speech acts
consist of adjacency sequences: a question and an answer, a prompt and
a response (thank you → you’re welcome). However, sequences may also
span multiple turns. Youssouf, Grimshaw, and Bird (2007), for example,
describe an encounter between Tuaregs in the Sahara, where greetings are
“critically important, and lapses can have grave consequences for the
offender or errant traveler” (p. 51); withholding information about the
location of water sources in the desert would have terrible consequences for
participants. The authors describe these greeting rituals as “strongly rule
governed” multi-stage processes, with clear rules about who initiates the
greeting (e.g., depending on number of travelers, age), what information
might be asked for at each stage, as well as what utterances and responses
are required (p. 54). The greeting ritual starts with a summons, the salaam
(‘peace,’ Arabic), a single-word greeting that carries sociocultural
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
6 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Pragmatics 133
information about the participants, such as their geographic origins, reli-
gious affiliation or probable knowledge of the region. Following the inter-
action, during which participants try to avoid any appearance of arrogance
or curiosity, the leave-taking speech act is brief, with “minimum social
amenities” (p. 58).
Many speech acts are straightforward and can be easily interpreted and
encoded, as is the case with I would like a cup of tea, please. At other times,
such as the greeting ritual in the Tuareg example, speech acts need to be
teased apart, since the underlying intent may not be evident in the surface-
level utterance. According to Austin (1975), speech acts are comprised of
the locutionary act (what we actually say), the illocutionary act (the under-
lying message that may or may not be the same as the locutionary act) and
the perlocutionary act (the effect on the interlocutor, either in thoughts or
actions):
Locutionary act: Salaam! (surface-level statement)
Illocutionary act: ‘I greet you’ + ‘I want to know if you are safe’ (underlying
message)
Perlocutionary act: Aleikum salaam (the response reassures the original speaker
that no harm is intended)
In order to navigate social demands, speech acts can be modified, with
the help of lexical or grammatical choices the user makes. For example, in
English, apologies may be expressed as Please accept my apology or I’m sorry.
These comprise the head act or the core of the speech act (Alcón Soler,
Safont Jordá, & Martínez-Flor, 2005). Frequently the speech act is mod-
ified either to soften or intensify its impact. These modifications can be
external or internal. I hadn’t realized that I was running so late. Please
accept my apology is an example of an external modification, since the
modification occurs outside of the head act. I am really sorry, in contrast,
is an internal modification, because the head act itself is changed. Table 6.1
presents the modifier types outlined by Alcón Soler et al. (2005):
Speech acts are expressed, structured, and enacted in diverse ways across
languages, cultures, and individuals, and the pragmalinguistic resources
needed to modify them are language-specific (LoCastro, 2003). Modifiers
in English, for example, include lexical items (e.g., perhaps or maybe) and
grammatical structures, such as using the subjunctive to soften a request;
consider the difference between could you open the door, please versus open
the door. Knowing which modifiers are appropriate to use with which
speech acts is an important aspect of L2/Lx pragmatic knowledge (Bardovi-
Harlig, 2012).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
134 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Table 6.1 Sample internal and external modifiers
Although speech acts are greatly variable across situations, the meaning-
making process is facilitated by routinized, conventional expressions.
Conventional Expressions
Conventional expressions are frequently used, ‘frozen’ multiword units
that make up most of our language use and are generally unanalyzed by
native speakers (Bardovi-Harlig, 2009; Edmonds, 2014). They may be
short, such as that’d be great or extended, such as thank you for inviting
me over (Bardovi-Harlig & Vellenga, 2012). Conventional expressions are
underutilized by L2/Lx learners, who often replace them with grammati-
cally and lexically accurate sequences that are not quite native-like, even at
advanced levels of proficiency (Boers & Lindstromberg, 2009). Because
textbooks still treat most vocabulary as isolated lexical items, learners often
do not learn multiword chunks until they have intense exposure to them in
study-abroad contexts (Bardovi-Harlig & Bastos, 2011; Edmonds, 2014).
This approach requires reconsideration, with early and frequent expo-
sure to conventional phrases, including auditory input, to “give students
a model for production that includes tone of voice, stress, intonation,
rhythm, and pronunciation of words,” as those paralinguistic features play
a key role in the underlying message an utterance conveys (Bardovi-Harlig
& Vellenga, 2012, p. 87). Multimodal input is necessary, for example, for
the context-appropriate interpretation of such a simple phrase as come on,
which can convey a range of meanings: encouragement, when we cajole
a friend to go on a roller-coaster, with the implication that ‘It’ll be fun!’;
impatience, when someone is taking too long to get ready; or disbelief in an
interlocutor’s utterance, implying ‘You’re not serious!’ The lexical and
grammatical content is constant, and we require additional contextual
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
6 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Pragmatics 135
cues – here, the speaker’s intonation – to interpret the meaning of the
phrase, and determine whether it expresses politeness or impoliteness.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
136 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
from the surface-level message. If the violation is recognized, negative
consequences may range from mild to severe: a so-called white lie (e.g.,
Yes, your dress is lovely!) might not harm anyone in the long run, whereas
lying under sworn testimony could have dire consequences. Flouting
behavior is not always negative, of course; it also drives humor and friendly
banter, as long as participants can presume that they share an interest in
mutual, cooperative interaction, whether that interaction is polite or
impolite (Culpeper, 2015; Dynel, 2013).
One of the most influential early models of politeness by Brown and
Levinson (1987) suggested that flouting the maxims was necessary for
politeness. The resultant implicitness would help protect an individual’s
‘face’ – the needs and desires vis-à-vis others (LoCastro, 2003). Their model
has two complementary components: negative face and positive face.
Negative face refers to being able to act without imposition from others.
Threats to negative face are coercive, as they wittingly or unwittingly try to
limit our autonomy. Orders, requests, threats, warnings, dares, some
compliments, or expressions of strong negative emotions are examples of
potential face-threatening acts (Brown & Levinson, 1987). Such acts force
people into a position where they either have to comply or be seen as
uncooperative. For example, asking a friend for money threatens her
negative face, because she is forced into a position of either having to
give me money (an imposition) or tell me No. Positive face, on the other
hand, is the image we wish to project to others. Threats to positive face are
potentially humiliating. Expressions of disapproval, contempt or ridicule,
complaints, insults, disagreements and contradictions, mention of taboo
topics, raising emotionally charged or divisive topics, blatant non-
cooperation, and wrong terms of address are examples of potential positive
face threatening acts (Brown & Levinson, 1987).
Brown and Levinson’s (1987) work has been criticized for being focused
only on the individual, at the exclusion of their identity as “members of
a family, social class, company, or age cohort” (LoCastro, 2003), who co-
construct politeness through interaction. Such a hyper-individualistic view
of politeness, critics argue (Gu, 1990; Matsumoto, 1988), does not reflect
the reality in collectivistic societies, where the emphasis is on one’s position
in relation to other members of society, and face-threatening acts are an
affront to an entire cultural group (e.g., family, organization, nation)
instead of to the individual. Furthermore, Brown and Levinson’s model
did not adequately recognize that what seems to be impolite in one
situation may actually express solidarity or teasing among friends
(LoCastro, 2003).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
6 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Pragmatics 137
Consequently, recent research has sought to reimagine politeness by
developing “a framework of analysis which concentrates less on the notion
that politeness is contained in words or phrases, and more on politeness as
a question of judgements made by participants and negotiated with in talk”
(Linguistic Politeness Research Group, 2011, p. 2). There are several
important distinctions between this group’s work on politeness and
Brown and Levinson’s research. Unlike Brown and Levinson, who relied
on the researchers’ intuition of what educated native speakers would say in
given situations, recent research uses natural, discourse-length language to
study both politeness and impoliteness, using multimodal analyses (e.g.,
written or spoken language, intonation, body language). Importantly,
analyses include reflections by the participants (emic perspective), instead
of relying solely on the analyst’s etic (outsider) interpretation of an inter-
action (Culpeper, Mackey, & Taguchi, 2018). There might be valid reasons
for flouting social norms, and sometimes impoliteness is an appropriate
response, such as standing up to a bully. Thus, determining what is polite
or impolite must be done in specific contexts, among specific interactants
performing particular social roles (Culpeper, 2011, 2015; LoCastro, 2011).
This interpretive process is challenging in an L2/Lx, since expectations
for cooperation vary across cultures, and interpreting implications may be
difficult, if we are still struggling to understand the surface-level linguistic
content and have no frames for teasing apart social conventions in new
linguistic and cultural contexts. When mismatches in expected (im)polite
communication occur, humor can be an effective tool to mitigate negative
consequences for either the speaker or his or her interlocutor.
Humor
Humor, which can take a variety of forms (see Table 6.2), is a universal
human phenomenon, expressed verbally and nonverbally. It improves
cognitive flexibility, memory, attention, and creative problem solving
(Lyubomirski, King, & Diener, 2005; Martin, 2007; Martin & Ford,
2018). It also builds and nurtures relationships, helps identify potential
friends, fosters positive group affiliation and social responsibility, and
eases conflict (Breckler, Olson, & Wiggins, 2006; Gervais & Wilson,
2005; Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004; Shiota, Campos, Keltner, &
Hertenstein, 2004). Importantly, for L2/Lx learners, humor also helps
reduce stress and anxiety that people in new cultural contexts often
encounter (Isen, 2003; Lefcourt, 2001; Martin, 2007; Martin & Ford,
2018).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
Table 6.2 Types of humor (based on Long & Graesser, 1988 and Nilsen & Nilsen, 2000)
Irony The literal and implied meaning of a word or expression Nice weather, eh? while it is raining
differ
Satire Aggressive humor that makes fun of social institutions, A political skit from Saturday Night Live that makes fun of
politics, and of people in power the fact that election polls can be misleading
Sarcasm Aggressive humor directed at individuals Well spotted, Sherlock in response to someone making an
obvious observation
Overstatements/ Humor that relies on intensifiers or modifiers to enhance I waited forever for that phone call
understatements or reduce the intensity of a statement
Self-deprecation Helps put listener(s) at ease or indirectly asks for support No worries, I’m not really good with numbers either.
Teasing Playful humor directed at others, both intimate and Oh, come on. Admit that the Scorpions are way better!
distancing, friendly and hostile (its negative extreme is
bullying)
Replies to rhetorical Responses that flout the expectation that no answer is Is the sky blue? (to indicate that something is obvious) –
questions required No, it’s only the tiny molecules of air in Earth’s atmosphere
scattering light from the sun.
Humorous response Intentional misunderstanding of the questioner’s When should we have our next meeting? – When hell freezes
to a serious intention or the seriousness of the question over?
question
Double entendres Deliberate misunderstanding of a speaker’s utterance, A cartoon on the Sardonic Salad blog depicts two strips of
typically hinting at a sexual connotation film sitting at a bar. One asks: “How about you and
I find a dark room and see what develops?”
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
6 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Pragmatics 139
Humor is primarily a social phenomenon, and it is the social-cultural
context that determines what type of humor is appropriate, with whom,
under what circumstances, and within what social parameters (Martin &
Ford, 2018). What topics are considered funny or taboo, who are the likely
targets and objects of jokes, where, when, and who can employ humor, and
even the preferred responses to humor (ranging from a smile or laughter to
frowning or booing) reflect cultural preferences (Davies, 1990; Gervais &
Wilson, 2005; Oring, 1994; Wyer, 2004; Wyer & Collins, 1992). Cultures
also vary in the types of humor they prefer: affiliative humor that empha-
sizes social cohesion and interpersonal responsibility (i.e., the interdepen-
dence of members of social groups) or aggressive humor that emphasizes
individualism. Thus, humor can illustrate high- or low-power distance,
gender roles, or sociopolitical concerns. Humor – especially negative
humor – can also highlight (or reinforce) existing social structures, ridicule,
bully or coerce an opponent, or stigmatize members of an out-group
(Martin, 2007; Martin & Ford, 2018). Ultimately, however, what is and
is not acceptable humor is determined by participants in specific interac-
tions “depending on the speaker’s intention . . . the hearer’s recognition of
the speaker’s intention, as well as his/her ultimate amusement or taking
offense” (Dynel, 2016, p. 142).
Since, humor is a part of our core personality, when we are unable to
comprehend or create humor in an L2/Lx, our sense of self is greatly
limited. Additionally, humor can help us establish friendships and set
boundaries, for example, by letting someone know gently when someone
does not wish to be hugged, even if it is common practice in a culture,
because the indirect and ambiguous nature of humor allows us to say
things that we would not be able to or wish to say directly (Arriaga,
2002; Martin & Ford, 2018; Pexman & Zvaigzne, 2004). Furthermore,
individuals with a sense of humor are seen as friendly, cooperative, inter-
esting, creative, emotionally more stable, open to new experiences, intel-
ligent, and perceptive (Cann & Calhoun, 2001; Martin, 2007; Martin &
Ford, 2018). Having these traits helps us engage with people who are
different from us, for building positive attitudes towards others and trying
new things, but being perceived to have these characteristics is important as
well, since it makes members of another culture more willing to interact
with us.
Therefore, it is incumbent upon instructors to help L2/Lx learners
express their sense of humor in a way that reflects their personality, in
a wide variety of communicative contexts (Ife, 2007). With sufficient
variety of authentic humorous and playful language, learners can develop
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
140 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
a sense of the kind of humor that best represents this aspect of their
identity.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
6 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Pragmatics 141
competence (Bardovi-Hardig & Hartford, 2005; Cohen, 2008; de Pablos-
Ortega, 2011; Ishihara & Cohen, 2010; Kallia, 2005; Kasper & Rose, 2002).
Foreign language learners, whose exposure to the language and culture is
limited, face even greater challenges and are likely to retain more L1
pragmatic features in the L2 (Kasper, 1997; Kasper & Rose, 2002). In
spite of these challenges, given the importance of pragmatic skills in real-
world communication, this area of L2/Lx learning deserves prominent
attention in order to help learners develop “the ability to manage
a complex interplay of language, language users, and context of interac-
tion” (Taguchi, 2011, p. 291). Some variables – such as L2 proficiency,
length of instruction, or time on task may be beyond instructors’ control,
but the pedagogical approach we adopt can help encourage pragmatic
development.
L2 pragmatics research has shown that pragmatics is teachable (Bardovi-
Harlig & Griffin, 2005; Gass et al., 2013; Schmidt, 1993). While some
pragmatic knowledge can be taught implicitly, especially if the L1 and the
L2 overlap in the language-function mapping, more complex pragmatic
concepts (or ones different from a user’s L1) benefit from explicit instruc-
tion, where rules are stated, whether before or after learners examine
relevant examples (Alcón Soler, 2008; Cohen, 2005; Halenko & Jones,
2011; Jeon & Kaya, 2006; Takahashi, 2010). For example, at more
advanced levels, learners would receive explicit training in expressing
agreement, disagreement, and providing constructive criticism (Bardovi-
Harlig, 2015; Bardovi-Harlig, Mossman, & Vellenga, 2015; Nguyen, 2013;
Nguyen & Basturkmen, 2010). Their training would include listening to
authentic data between native speakers, such as a peer review session in
academic contexts and focusing on the linguistic forms used by the speak-
ers, the way they express their views and the organizational structure of
their feedback on their peer’s work. Additionally, providing metaprag-
matic information (i.e., talking about pragmatics), mapping form onto
function, enhancing input (e.g., by bolding the focal point or flooding
a source text with examples), and completing repeated tasks with the same
pragmatic information have also been found effective. Notably, Koike and
Pearson (2005) found that the pedagogical focus impacts learning out-
comes: explicit instruction seems to improve explicit knowledge (i.e.,
judgment tasks), while implicit instruction improved spontaneous prag-
matic production. However, Alcón Soler (2008) found that, while both
implicit and explicit instruction was beneficial, explicit instruction led to
long-term gains, possibly due to greater cognitive engagement by the
learners.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
142 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Building on VanPatten and Williams’ (2007) work, input processing
can make pragmatics accessible for learners. Instruction might entail asking
learners to (1) listen to a dialog with several examples of the target prag-
matic structure, (2) work in small groups or pairs and examine the appro-
priateness of the target structure, evaluating both the linguistic forms and
social context in which they occur, (3) analyze additional texts receptively,
and (4) complete productive tasks, such as discourse completion, role-
playing, or judgment tasks to determine the appropriateness of examples
provided (Takimoto, 2009). Follow-up tasks can be either the same as the
input task or similar to it; the available evidence suggests that repetition
yields slightly better outcomes and may build fluency (Takimoto, 2012).
A recent approach to teaching pragmatics focuses on skills development,
moving learners from declarative (being able to talk about pragmatics) to
procedural knowledge (being able to produce pragmatic constructs).
According to this model, applying pragmatic knowledge during actual
interaction requires repeated practice of rules via tasks that foster receptive
as well as productive skills (Li, 2012). Sample dialogs, discourse completion
tasks (where learners provide correct responses to prompts), and multiple-
choice activities are used to model the use of speech acts in particular
speech events and social contexts (Ishida, 2009).
Martínez-Flor and Usó-Juan (2006) designed a six-stage framework for
fostering pragmatic and intercultural competence through skills-
development. During the first two stages – researching and reflecting –
L2 learners explore pragmatic concepts in their L1 (e.g., speech acts or
politeness formulae), analyzing the data according to gender or age, as well
as other social variables. These stages help learners become aware of
pragmatics and access concepts through familiar interaction (i.e., in their
first language). Stages 3 and 4 – receiving and reasoning – entail explicit
instruction about how the speech acts or politeness formulae are expressed
in the L2, including examining the impact of social variables on the use of
these pragmatic concepts. The final stages – rehearsing and revising – allow
learners to practice what they had learned, both receptively and produc-
tively, focusing on real-world communication and receiving feedback. In
order to ensure accurate pragmatic development, Martínez-Flor and Usó-
Juan’s (2006) recommend comparative analyses between cultures, explicit
instruction and awareness raising, analyzing situated language use, focused
practice, repetition, and feedback.
Skills training may be particularly effective in study-abroad situations.
Shively (2010) suggests that prior to departure, learners receive explicit
instruction in pragmatics and training in analyzing language use. Once
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
6 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Pragmatics 143
they are in the L2 community, they practice different speech acts each week
in real-world communication with other L2/Lx users and analyze how
social variables impact language choice. After returning home, learners stay
connected to the host community via social media and continue to explore
pragmatic concepts, including practice, analysis, and interpretation each
time. Skills-training programs like this can give L2/Lx users great auton-
omy and flexibility for learning pragmatics, as they can progress at their
own pace, following their own learning-style preferences.
Like skills-training, task-based approaches to pragmatics instruction also
prioritize socially situated meaningful language use. Studies in a recent
edited volume by Taguchi and Kim (2018) reiterate the effectiveness of
explicit instruction of pragmatic features, collaborative problem-solving
and creative language use (e.g., writing a drama) to foster pragmatic
development. Studies showed, for example, that carefully designed tasks
improved learners’ understanding of pragmalinguistic and sociopragmatic
features of Korean honorifics (Kim, Lee, & Kim, 2018), request modifica-
tions used in email (Alcón-Soler, 2018), and persuasive writing (Gomez-
Laich, & Taguchi, 2018).
Reflecting a growing field of related research, González-Lloret and
Ortega (2018) explored how task-based pragmatics learning can be
fostered by technology-mediated communication. In agreement with
Canto, de Graff, and Juaregi (2014), the authors argue that telecolla-
borative tasks, where communicative partners are geographically apart
and interact via social media, promote pragmatic development, which
aligns closely with intercultural competence, if tasks guide learners to
compare their own language use and those of their interlocutors.
González-Lloret and Ortega (2018) identified three guiding principles
for designing tasks that connect technology, task-based language learn-
ing, and L2 pragmatics: actions, language, and technologies. Actions
refer to the objectives learners want to achieve with the language (e.g.,
purchasing a mobile phone) and sociopragmatic features, the cultural,
contextual, and relational norms needed for completing actions. The
language focus includes lexical, structural, and pragmalinguistic knowl-
edge that learners need to access for completing the task (e.g., asking and
answering questions, agreeing and disagreeing, taking turns). The final
component, technologies, includes ensuring students’ access to and
familiarity with technology, as well as their ability to navigate
a website and participate in online communities.
Some computer-mediated task-based language teaching (TBLT)
research explores how online games enhance learners’ pragmatic skills in
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
144 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
locally situated interactions, including regional dialects, power, social
distance, and varying levels of imposition (Holden & Sykes, 2012; Sykes,
2017). Furthermore, as Sydorenko (2015) and Taguchi, Li, and Tang (2017)
found, even a few sessions of game-based native-speaker modeling led to
significant and long-term gains in learners’ pragmatic knowledge.
Sydorenko noted, however, that peer practice via role-plays yielded more
creative, humorous language that was pragmatically “inappropriate” but
offered more opportunities for “metapragmatic discussion, and extended
turns similar to those in naturalistic interactions” (2015, p. 333). Thus,
technology-based research repeatedly demonstrated the teachability of L2
pragmatics.
The final approach to teaching L2/Lx pragmatics discussed here focuses
on the local situatedness of interaction. This approach helps learners become
more aware of pragmatic issues through a process of active analysis of
authentic data and incorporates individuals’ prior experiences, both in the
personal and the cultural domains, which help them determine what is
appropriate or how their interlocutors might perceive what the speakers
say in particular situations (Kecskés, 2006, 2012; LoCastro, 2011; Mills,
2009). Our unique experiences as individuals impact our choice of words,
expressions, speech styles, and language use, and our language choices shape
our interactions, which are dynamic, situation-specific, locally contextua-
lized processes (Kecskés, 2010, 2019).
A locally situated pragmatics pedagogy recognizes that there is no single
mapping of linguistic form onto pragmatic meaning, and that language use
varies both across speakers and even within the speech of an individual
speaker across different interactional situations. That is, different indivi-
duals may use divergent speech acts to express the same pragmatic func-
tion, and even the same person might use different expressions to describe
the same idea at different moments in time (Cohen, 2005). Similarly,
politeness and impoliteness are expressed in diverse ways, depending on
the unique, local, dynamic social context (e.g., the same behavior may be
acceptable or disruptive, depending on the interlocutors and interactional
goals), in which the interaction takes place (Culpeper et al., 2010; Mills,
2009; Spencer-Oatey, 2002). Importantly, this approach also recognizes
that there is no single norm or standard form to which L2/Lx speakers must
aspire (Mills, 2011). Instead, a broad spectrum of language varieties, speak-
ers, social roles, and communicative purposes should be modeled so that
learners can make choices regarding their interactions. This is important for
college-aged learners in particular, who may wish to adopt some but not all
L2/Lx pragmatic practices (Barron, 2005; Kallia, 2005; Washburn, 2001).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
6 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Pragmatics 145
Discourse-rich material (movies, documentaries, newspaper articles, etc.)
can provide sufficiently contextualized pragmatic modeling to help lear-
ners understand interaction as a dynamic, “moment-to-moment . . . emer-
gent process” (Davies, 2004, pp. 210–211).
In addition to describing the different approaches, there are a few
general points regarding pragmatics instruction that merit attention.
First, while research tends to focus on advanced L2/Lx learners’ pragmatic
development, earlier intervention is possible and useful; beginning lan-
guage learners are quite capable of developing sociopragmatic skills and
should be exposed to pragmalinguistic features of the L2/Lx as well
(Abrams, 2014; Dewaele, 2008). Second, longer authentic materials (e.g.,
a television series, longer feature film, documentary, novel) can serve as
crucial input for learners by modeling dyadic and multi-participant inter-
actions, with messy turn-taking patterns, false starts, interruptions, and
discontinuities, along with numerous cultural references (Abrams, 2016b;
Gilmore, 2015). Third, authentic materials represent many different types
of spoken, written, and signed language – such as formal talks, interviews,
informal chats, storytelling, opinion pieces, gossip, and jokes – genres that
are usually inadequately represented in textbooks. Such socially situated
materials can “sensitise learners to the ways in which the discourse reflects
its context” (Gilmore, 2015, p. 103). Fourth, tasks should prompt learners
to interpret the social meaning of interactions and allow them to identify
and adopt appropriate participant roles, reflecting learners’ needs for
genuine interpersonal exchanges in which they may have to participate in
real, authentic communication (DeBot, Verspoor, & Verspoor, 2005;
Eisenchlas, 2011; Seedhouse, 1996). Fifth, the profession needs to move
away from comparing learners’ pragmatic performance to that of native
speaker models in both research and pedagogy (Martínez-Flor & Fukuya,
2005). Aiming for native-speaker standards is unrealistic in the typical two
to four years of L2 instruction most students complete in the United States,
given how difficult it is to learn pragmatics even when one spends a long
time in the host culture. Native speaker ‘norms’ are also unrealistic,
because there is great diversity among individuals’ pragmatic preferences,
influenced by gender, age, education, or the participants’ interest in
succeeding at the interaction (Culpeper et al., 2010; Mills, 2009). Even if
norms could be identified, L2 practices might not be desirable for some
learners because they do not match the interactional preferences of the
particular social group, whose language is modeled (Kallia, 2005;
Washburn, 2001). However, although individual conversations may differ
from normative practices, analyzing cultural trends can be useful. For
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
146 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
a brief introduction to diplomats moving to a new country, it might be
useful to describe general pragmatic practices that they are likely to
encounter. However, long term, it is more productive to teach learners
how to analyze interactions, how to figure out the relationship between
language form and the specific social contexts in which it creates
meaning.
Summary
Pragmatics is an obvious language feature to connect to intercultural
communication, since it focuses on the creation and interpretation of
meaning in a social context. This section lays out existing and potential
further connections between pragmatics and intercultural communica-
tion, in terms of the theoretical concepts presented in Chapters 1 and 2.
The resulting synthesis suggests how intercultural communication-
oriented pedagogy might foster the development of pragmatic
competence:
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
6 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Pragmatics 147
(cont.)
Linguistic repertoire
1. Introduce conventional expressions to facilitate more natural and fluid interactions
(e.g., Don’t mind me! ).
2. Ensure that learners understand terms of address, both their linguistic form and
appropriate contexts of use.
3. Teach speech acts, raising learners’ awareness that the expressions we use differ across
languages, both how often and what we need to say in response to a request or an
invitation, for example.
4. Examine phrases, terms of address and other linguistic forms used to express politeness
and impoliteness in various social contexts. Help learners identify when something they
say is perceived as acceptable and when it is not.
5. Explore how pragmatic miscalculations can be repaired (e.g., apology, humor).
6. Teach typical frames and topics of humor, including references to current events.
Curricular considerations
1. Incorporate pragmatic skills into the curriculum from the beginning – let students tap
into their L1 sociopragmatic knowledge at early stages of L2/Lx development,
expanding that knowledge to L2/Lx-speaking contexts and pragmalinguistic
knowledge gradually.
2. Provide activities that foster awareness raising, skills training, explicit instruction, and
extensive practice focusing on pragmatics.
3. Treat pragmatics explicitly, including analyzing multiple ways of expressing the same
sentiment (e.g., making a request); activities and tasks should focus on the social
meaning conveyed by users’ choices, in addition to the surface-level content.
4. Ensure that tasks across the curriculum explore how pragmatics reflects societal,
institutional and smaller social group trends, as well as personal preferences, enacted in
unique interactions moment-by-moment.
5. Include humor as an essential component of pragmatic knowledge and self-expression
as learners’ intercultural selves emerge (e.g., cross-linguistic puns, mixed frames for
jokes).
6. Utilize longer authentic materials – particularly movies, documentaries, or TV shows –
that can fill in the contextual gap often characteristic of textbooks.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
148 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Learning Objectives
By the end of this telecollaborative project, learners will be able to do the
following:
1. Identify preferred genres of humor about social and political issues (see
Table 6.2).
2. Recognize how verbal and nonverbal communication is used to express
humor.
3. Comprehend how visual cues and language reciprocally support each
other to create humor in particular contexts (e.g., an artist’s message,
political affiliation, target audience, etc.).
4. Identify topics that are current in political humor and what each venue
might concentrate on (e.g., The Economist, a local paper, late night
comedy), including jokes on current events and politics, historical
events and references, gender roles, social issues.
5. Create a joke, including visual and linguistic content to convey
a humorous message.
Analytic Approach
Multimodal analysis. Students learn to interpret the complementary
semiotic systems of images, verbal, and nonverbal communication, as well
as how information is presented.
Task 1
During week 2, student-groups brainstorm, where they see humor in
the media, what kinds of humor they like and read/watch, what topics
are current, and what kind of language is allowed by different artists
and performers, etc. The instructor provides students with information
regarding different genres of humor.
In each location, students should share their findings with their class for
a broader coverage of ideas.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
6 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Pragmatics 149
Task 2
Weeks 3–4: Student-groups share two or three social/political cartoons that
they saw in their home culture and explain them to their telecollaborative
peers, including pragmalinguistic features, the sociopragmatic context, any
required background information required for understanding the joke, and
why it is currently relevant.
Task 3
1. Weeks 5–6: Student-groups watch one comedy show from each cultural
context, taking turns (e.g., week 4: an episode of Trevor Noah’s Daily Show,
and week 5: an episode of the Joe Show from Egypt with Youssef Houssein).
Learners analyze the format of the show, the participants, permissible
topics, relevant cultural and linguistic background knowledge, verbal and
nonverbal communication used to express humor, etc. Each side helps the
other make sense of the show; students will have to conduct some research
to find the necessary information.
2. Students present their findings to their classmates, comparing different
types of humor, current topics, and how they are treated humorously
(and what purpose humor serves), stating explicitly how multimodal
information is used to create humor; as each group presents, the rest of
class should take notes, perhaps creating a matrix as shown in the pre-
task.
Post-Task
1. Student-groups (either telecollaborative groups or newly formed groups
from each class) collaboratively create a cartoon that depicts a topic
related to a social or political issue they are interested in, showing that
issue from the perspectives of both cultural groups involved in the
telecollaborative project, using multimodal humor, effectively incor-
porating pragmalinguistic and sociopragmatic features based on their
preceding analyses.
2. Alternately, groups could create a short skit from their favorite comedy
show, addressing the same points as with the cartoon project in (1).
Analysis
These activities build awareness of the pragmatics of humor with the help of
in-group informants. The repeated analyses, explicit discussions of
pragmatics, genre-specific terminology (metapragmatic knowledge), and
examinations of how culturally contextualized language and visual
information mutually reinforce each other help learners expand and deepen
their knowledge of socially contextualized pragmatics as it relates to humor.
Noticing cultural comparisons, including variation among cultural groups,
helps enhance learners’ intercultural communicative competence. This
project also raises learners’ awareness of political, historical, and other
cultural issues relevant for intercultural communication (see Chapter 9).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
150 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
ACTIVITY SET 2
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, learners will be able to do the following:
1. Identify different ways of greeting people, terms of address, pronouns,
verb forms to express formal and informal language; note how partici-
pants use grammatical forms to express solidarity or distance with their
interlocutors.
2. Express politeness and impoliteness, using a few targeted downgraders
and intensifiers.
3. Identify a few pragmatic particles (e.g., ja, also, and doch), understand-
ing how they reflect participants’ understanding of the interaction.
Analytic Approach
Ethnography of communication and interactional sociolinguistics.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
6 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Pragmatics 151
Post-Viewing
Working in small groups of four or five (preferably not in their original
group), learners identify their favorite scene – or important scenes for the
plot – and re-create it in their own words, using the pragmatic features the
class had discussed.
Analysis
These tasks encourage noticing and raise learners’ awareness of pragmatics,
helping them recognize different terms of address, greetings, and leave-
taking formulae, as well as the social meaning (e.g., politeness and
impoliteness) expressed by the choice of pronouns and verb forms. Learners
also become aware of the dynamic nature of interaction, how interactants’
comments prompt another’s responses, as well as its local situatedness (some
interactions use seemingly impolite language to bond, for example). The
analyses described in this task also ensure that learners recognize
pragmalinguistic and sociopragmatic aspects of contextualized language.
Additionally, such analyses help learners recognize unanalyzed formulaic
units (e.g., thank you/can you, please . . .) at early stages of L2/Lx
development, to prepare them for using increasingly complex forms of
nuanced, multifunctional language later on.
If desired, this task could be expanded by having learners first analyze
a pragmatic feature in their own L1 and use their observations as
a springboard for understanding these features in the L2/Lx, based on
variations of the following questions:
How do greetings work in your own speech community? . . . Can you
develop an inventory of different types of greetings? Does a greeting
tend to take a certain form? . . . What can you tell about people’s
relationship from the way they greet each other? What do you think
would happen if someone used a greeting form that was not appro-
priate to their relationship? (Youssouf et al., 2007, p. 50).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
152 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:53:21, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.007
chapter 7
Chapter Overview
In the Concise Oxford Dictionary of Linguistics, P. H. Matthews (2014)
defines paralanguage as:
Any aspect of vocal behaviour which can be seen as meaningful but is not
described as part of the language system. Thus, in particular, aspects of voice
quality; of the speed, loudness, and overall pitch of speech; of the use of
hesitation; of intonation to the extent that it is not covered by an account of
phonology.
Thus, the term paralinguistics (from ‘alongside language’ in ancient
Greek) describes sounds that do not serve symbolic referential pur-
poses themselves (Frawley, 2003), but rather subtly shade “the way we
say what is on our mind” (Townsend, 1985, p. 27). The affective
information people try to convey to each other is not always clear,
perhaps leaving us with a sense of ‘It wasn’t what they said, but how
they said it.’ In order to interpret the speaker’s tone, then, partici-
pants often rely on the context of the interaction, as the following
example from the British Parliament illustrates. In this excerpt from
July 24, 20181, the Secretary of State, Dominic Raab, attempts to
reassure Members of Parliament that the withdrawal of the United
Kingdom from the European Union (Brexit) can be done smoothly,
in spite of a rapidly approaching deadline.
mr. raab: With permission Mr Speaker, I would like to make a statement on the
White Paper published today setting out the Government’s plans for
legislating for the withdrawal agreement and implementation period. On
Friday 29 March 2019, the UK will leave the European Union, giving effect
1
https://hansard.parliament.uk/commons/2018–07-24/debates/D49F4F4C-FC7C-403A-8BFE-C
D9999DF1F2B/EUWithdrawalAgreementLegislation
153
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
154 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
to the historic decision taken by the British people in the 2016
referendum. The Government are committed to delivering a
smooth and orderly Brexit. That is why we have already passed
the European Union (Withdrawal) Act 2018.
several mps: [loud laughter by numerous MPs immediately following “smooth
and orderly Brexit,” overlapping with the final sentence]
mr. raab: The Shadow Foreign Secretary2 is laughing. She and her party
voted against the Bill, thereby undermining her commitment to
give effect to the referendum . . .
mr. starmer: I thank the Secretary of State for providing advance copies of his
statement and the White Paper. I am glad to say that that was two
HOURS ago, and it is MUCH appreciated.
The debate took place in the House of Commons, as members of the
opposition expressed their cynicism regarding comments made by a
member of the Executive Branch through laughter. The laughter was
sufficiently loud to make the opposition’s disbelief in Mr. Raab’s
claims evident, and its meaning was reiterated when the Shadow
Secretary of State highlighted the brief time his party had to read
the document provided by Mr. Raab. In combination with the
emphasis on two hours, in contrast to the falling intonation of I am
glad to say, and a pause (along with background knowledge about the
topic) the audience was able to interpret the meaning of the laughter
as ridicule.
As this example illustrates, paralinguistic features are integrated with
other facets of language or “modulated onto or embedded into the verbal
message” (Schuller & Batliner, 2014, p. 9; emphasis original). They
contribute details about a speaker’s affect, forming an important layer
of communication (Blommaert, 2008; Austin, Hampel & Kukulska-
Hulme, 2017). To explore this topic in depth, this chapter looks at
paralinguistic features in the four categories set forth by Matthews
(2014) – voice quality, prosody, conversational management, and
accent,3 – followed by a discussion of affect in electronic communica-
tion. Afterwards, these concepts are connected to an intercultural
communication-oriented pedagogy.
2
www.parliament.uk/mps-lords-and-offices/government-and-opposition1/opposition-holding/
3
Sometimes kinesics (body language), proxemics (preferences for personal space), and haptics (touch-
ing) are included in paralinguistics (Celce-Murcia, 2007, p. 49). Research in the social sciences,
however, typically separates paralinguistic features from nonverbal communication, a distinction
maintained in this book. All emojis are from Twitter and the graphics are licensed under the CC-
BY 4.0.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
7 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Paralinguistic Features 155
Voice Quality
Speech Rate
The rate or tempo of speech varies with our intended meaning, emotion(s),
attitude, and personality. Sometimes we can control the rate of our speech,
at other times its tempo is involuntary. A slow tempo may suggest hesita-
tion, uncertainty, a warning, self-assurance, or higher status, whereas a fast
tempo conveys “[a] care-free attitude, gaiety . . . anger, annoyance, impa-
tience, haste, urgency” (Poyatos, 2002, p. 8). Fast and slow speech can also
reflect personality, such as extroversion or introversion, respectively,
although stereotypes exaggerate the actual differences between these traits
(Feldstein & Sloan, 1984; Schuller & Batliner, 2014). Since some of these
characterizations are contradictory, the local context is particularly impor-
tant for accurate interpretation.
In addition to personal and situational factors, some languages are
believed to be spoken more rapidly than others. For example, cowboys in
Western movies seem to drawl, whereas films set in New York tend to reflect
faster speech (Roach, 1998). In reality, numerous factors impact how quick
speech sounds. Some languages (e.g., agglutinative languages like Quechua,
in which syntactic relationships are expressed by extensive morphemes) have
long words with comparatively infrequent stops, which might make them
sound faster than languages with shorter words. Also, early investigations
into the idea that speakers of stress-timed languages (e.g., Arabic) may
perceive syllable-timed languages (e.g., Spanish) as being fast (and vice
versa) seemed to show that perception may reflect familiarity with a language
instead of its phonetic structure. In spite of a lack of measurable differences
between languages in terms of tempo, stereotypes exist about fast or slow
languages (Poyatos, 2002), which may lead people to associate certain
personal characteristics with their speakers, considering faster speakers to
be more competent or slower speakers to be more respectful and sincere,
depending on our own cultural practices (Roach, 1998). Specific types of
speech events, such as religious rituals, as well as situational conditions (e.g.,
being in a hurry versus chatting with friends) similarly prompt varied rates of
speech, as does a speaker’s personal style and emotional state.
The rate at which we speak may also be a function of interactional
dynamics: interlocutors may mirror each other’s speech rate to influence
their interactant(s) or express affiliation, whereas a mismatch may indicate
an unwillingness to accommodate them (Poyatos, 2002). Alternately, by
speaking unnecessarily slowly, interactants may insult their conversation
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
156 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
partners (see Chapter 13). The level of L2/Lx proficiency also impacts rate
of speech (Lennon, 2000), although proficiency level might be a coexisting
variable alongside cultural, personal, situational factors (Jackson &
Suethanapornkul, 2013). Yet, since fluency (speech rate) is a predominant
factor in proficiency testing (Préfontaine, Kormos, & Johnson, 2016), it is
particularly advisable to consider how a language is perceived in terms of
tempo, and how this perception may impact raters’ assessment of an L2/Lx
speaker’s proficiency (Duijm, Schoonen, & Hulstijn, 2018). A mismatch
between L2/Lx users’ perceived and actual fluency may affect ratings on
high-stakes exams that determine whether they meet graduation or
employment requirements.
Volume
Along with tempo, the volume of speech also varies across cultures and
languages. In the United States, for example, loudness is typically perceived
as a sign of a high-achieving, charismatic, and confident person (Schuller
& Batliner, 2014). Using our own cultural practices as a baseline, we
perceive speakers of other languages as being particularly loud and judge
entire nationalities based on our perceptions of a few members of another
culture. Cultural preferences also account for locations where we may
speak loudly and where we are expected to be quieter. In Latin America
and many Mediterranean cultures, for example, public spaces encourage
louder speech than what we are likely to find in North America in similar
types of venues or in the same countries in other locations. It is useful to
understand differences in loudness – perceived or real – across languages
and cultures and to develop a sense of what Poyatos (2002) calls the
“cultural standard level ” (p. 5), so that we can interpret the meaning of
volume more accurately in its local linguistic and cultural contexts and not
mistake an enthusiastic loudness for an argument or quietness for disin-
terest. Mistaking the meaning of loud or quiet speech can have detrimental
effects for interactants. In the 1960s, children in a school for Native
Americans thought that their teachers were angry or mean, because they
were speaking so loudly, while the teachers found the children disengaged
or even rude, due to their quietness (Key, 1975).
Preferences for loud, soft, quiet, or inconspicuous speech also depend on
the social roles participants enact. Disparate power distributions, such as
student–teacher or doctor–patient might demand louder or quieter speech
or silence (Poyatos, 2002). Furthermore, interactants of relatively compar-
able social status in low-stakes contexts will interpret differences in volume
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
7 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Paralinguistic Features 157
with less consequence than interactants with vastly disparate status in high-
stakes contexts. Imagine, for example, the potentially devastating outcome
of reacting loudly to an antagonistic immigration agent by an asylum
seeker with limited L2/Lx skills. Additionally, specific social situations
may require a particular volume: Should we speak quietly at work, or is a
loud, boisterous debate expected to show engagement? Is a funeral a quiet
and somber ritual, a joyous celebration of life, or an occasion to express
communal grief through shared crying? Does whispering indicate
repressed anger, intimacy, rudeness, or an expression of interest?
Pitch
Poyatos (2002) describes pitch as the “most versatile message-conveying
feature of voice, which accompanies all verbal and nonverbal communica-
tive sounds with the subtlest possible symbolic variations” (p. 10). The
pitch of our voice has some physiological determinants: the longer and
thicker our vocal chords, the slower their vibrations or frequency, therefore
the lower the pitch (most men’s voices); conversely, shorter and thinner
vocal chords produce higher pitch, typical of women and children
(Kreiman & Sidtis, 2011; Poyatos, 2002). Although the physiological
characteristics of our vocal chords post puberty are relatively stable
(Schötz, 2007), our pitch changes across conversational situations and
conveys social meaning. Occupational (e.g., somebody working with
small children will likely have a higher pitch) and situational factors
(e.g., excitement or humor), language fluency, and geographical location
can impact pitch (Schuller & Batliner, 2014).
Other elements of pitch, such as creakiness or laryngealization, are
language-specific: Danish has a suprasegmental glottal catch (stød
[‘shock’]) (Fischer-Jørgensen, 1989), as do Latvian and certain Lithuanian
dialects (Kiparsky, 2018). Elsewhere, creakiness carries attitudinal connota-
tions. According to Laver (1994) and Grivičić and Nilep (2004), the ‘creak’
in English Received Pronunciation signals boredom, commiseration in
Tzeltal (an indigenous language in Southern Mexico), an apology in
Oto-Manguean languages (an indigenous language family in Central
America), or rejection of a speaker.
Variations in pitch also reflect intra- and intercultural variation, encod-
ing differences among social groups. Van Bezooijen (1995) found, for
example, that Japanese women have a higher pitch range than Dutch
women, possibly because a higher pitch in Japan seemed to “project a
vocal image associated with feminine attributes of powerlessness” (p. 253).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
158 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
In a more recent study, Yuasa (2010) observed that many English-speaking
women in California use a creaky voice to project an image of someone
who is “hesitant, nonaggressive, and informal but also educated, urban-
oriented, and upwardly mobile” (p. 315). Young U.S. American women
have also been found to use a phrase-final vocal fry (a low-pitch, vibrato
sound or glottalization) to signal hesitation and positive affiliation with
their interlocutors (Wolk, Abdelli-Beruh, and Slavin, 2012). Schuller and
Batliner (2014) posit that laryngealization might also be a way to counter a
higher feminine pitch in order to signal competence, since a “low voice
means big and powerful. This feature can be adapted by groups that are not
big and powerful, such as female teenagers” (p. 90). The authors also
observed that young women in Germany use nasalization and uptalk
(a raised pitch at the end of phrases or sentences) to signal group member-
ship, a phenomenon possibly influenced by soap operas that have effected a
cultural shift in pitch.
Variation in men’s voices also carries social meaning. Puts, Gaulin, and
Verdolini (2006), Puts, Hodges, Cárdenas, and Gaulin (2007), and
Weninger, Krajewski, Batliner, and Schuller (2012) found, for example,
that a deeper voice among men was perceived as more dominant, especially
in the contexts of romance or friendship, whereas in politics, a higher pitch
more positively correlated with charisma and leadership. Thus, pitch has
implications for being perceived as strong, feminine or masculine, ener-
getic, mature/immature, wise, emotional or unemotional, among other
characteristics (Poyatos, 2002).
Interpretations of pitch, however, are variable. Individuals may find the
same pitch range alluring in one person but irritating in another. The
perceived attractiveness of a particular pitch may also shift over time:
“female pitch has been lowered in the US and other Western countries
since the 1950s due to the changing role of women in society” (Schuller &
Batliner, 2014, p. 83). The appropriateness or attractiveness is also situa-
tionally variable. A breathy voice might be considered soft and sexy on a
date, but puzzling while purchasing a movie ticket, and perhaps detri-
mental in a job interview. Similarly, a high pitch might indicate anxiety
before an exam or excitement at a party.
Prosody/Intonation
Languages reflect diverse intonation patterns that differentiate between
statements and questions, surprise or consternation, innuendo or sarcasm,
nervousness or self-assurance, among other affective connotations. Thus,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
7 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Paralinguistic Features 159
Table 7.1 Intonation variation for Thanks a lot in U.S. mainstream English
Conversation Management
Interaction is jointly created by participants within social and cogni-
tive contexts (Bardovi-Harlig, 2015; LoCastro, 2003). It is like a dance
between two or more partners, during which speakers and listeners
actively contribute to communicate (Tannen, 2007). Their contribu-
tions include conversational devices – such as hedges, filler words,
pauses, silence, laughter – that, although often verbal (how interesting
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
160 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
or aha), add little or no substance to the interaction but nonetheless
shape its meaning.
Speakers can use hesitation markers, filler words, and hedges, for exam-
ple, to indicate their enthusiasm or disdain for a topic or their interlocu-
tors, to suggest that they wish to soften a claim or to let their interlocutors
know that they are finished speaking (Tannen, 2007). In order to engage
their listeners, speakers might use tag questions (e.g., ikke [‘no?’] in Danish
or oder [the pragmatic equivalent of ‘right’] in German) to prompt an
acknowledgment or reaction; they may or may not also yield the conversa-
tional floor to an interlocutor. In contrast, filler words and hedges, such as
ehm, like, or actually in English, asszem (azt hiszem [‘I think’]) in
Hungarian, or แบบ (bàep [‘like’]) in Thai help retain the speaker’s turn at
talk or soften and modify an actual utterance (G. Lakoff, 1973; Ranganath,
Jurafsky, & McFarland, 2013; Schuller & Batliner, 2014).
During interaction, listeners also play an active role. They signal their
interest or (dis)approval with various backchanneling devices, such as
expressive interjections (e.g., really? or wow), laughter, silence, yawning,
or coughing (Ephratt, 2011; Provine, 2012). These features provide direct or
indirect feedback to the speaker, such as aha to express agreement, hmmm
to indicate surprise in English, rising intonation to encourage the speaker
to continue, or laughter with a raised eyebrow to signal disbelief in the
speaker’s utterance.
Gender, social status, linguistic differences (e.g., language variety spo-
ken), as well as the speaker’s intention and personality impact the use of
hedges and filler words (Dixon & Foster, 1997; R. Lakoff, 1973).
Additionally, there are broad cultural patterns that guide how such para-
linguistic features are used. For example, Salager-Meyer (2011) reported
that research papers written by French, Finnish, and Bulgarian academics
used fewer hedges than their English-speaking colleagues, which their
readers perceived as more arrogant, self-confident, decisive, authoritarian,
and categorical, showing less deference towards readers than the language
used by English-speaking authors. However, empirical evidence suggest
that, in most interactions, contextual influences (the particular interlocu-
tors, purpose of the interaction, etc.) are more relevant for the use of hedges
and fillers than general cultural practices (Dixon & Foster, 1997).
Similarly, the use of silence as a conversational device reflects situational
as well as cultural preferences. Brief pauses can provide thinking time as we
plan our next comment, or they can help us emphasize a point; they may
also express disapproval or sadness. There may be expected pauses between
interactants in some cultures to indicate respect and deference to other
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
7 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Paralinguistic Features 161
interlocutors. Pauses may also be a function of language proficiency,
especially at earlier stages of L2/Lx development, where they may signal
communication breakdown and prompt interlocutors to step in and help
with vocabulary, for example (Grimshaw & Cardoso, 2018; Schuller &
Batliner, 2014). Disfluency, that can occur during both L1 and L2/Lx use,
also depends on the modality of communication. Spontaneous speech, for
example, tends to include more hesitations, false starts, repetitions, or slips
of the tongue than planned speech or written communication (Schuller &
Batliner, 2014). In order to maintain their turn at conversation, while also
reducing the appearance of hesitation, speakers can lengthen syllables to
buy more time and prevent their interlocutors from taking over the
conversational floor or use fillers and hedges (Böhm & Shattuck-
Hufnagel, 2007, cited in Schuller & Batliner, 2014).
How long participants may or should speak also pertains to conversa-
tional management, determined, for example, by cultural preferences, the
number of interlocutors, the purpose of the interaction, the level of
intimacy among interlocutors, their interest in each other and the topic,
as well as the power distribution among participants. More powerful
people, or people who hold a more powerful position in a particular
interaction, tend to talk more than less powerful individuals (Spender,
1980). Contrary to stereotypical views in many cultures, however, women
do not speak more than men (Mehl, Vazire, Ramírez-Esparza, Slatcher, &
Pennebaker, 2007).
Native or advanced L2/Lx speakers use conversational signals or devices
mostly subconsciously and only notice them if the conversation goes awry.
For example, an interactant may not realize that the speaker has finished
her turn and waits too long before making his own contribution, which the
speaker might misinterpret as disinterest. Once interlocutors realize that a
misunderstanding has occurred, they can apologize, complain, or adjust
their responses, depending on what they are trying to accomplish with the
interaction, and whether they are able to find a more effective interactional
pattern (which may depend on the speaker’s language skills).
Knowing when and what kinds of backchanneling devices can be used in
particular contexts is an important skill to develop in any language at other
stages of L2/Lx development as well, since a “participant in the active,
speaking role would be very irritated if the listener either did not produce
any backchanneling signals at all, or did so in the wrong places” (Schuller
& Batliner, 2014, p. 134). In an L2/Lx, participants have to acquire new
linguistic manifestations of conversational signals and devices, and
understand how and when they are used in communication.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
162 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Additionally, they also need to learn about the surrounding social structures,
power dynamics, and interactional expectations that influence the culturally
and locally appropriate use of conversational devices. Learning how and when
to use these devices in a new language is challenging, because the appropriate
indications for expressing affect are culturally varied. Tannen (2005, 2007,
2012) found, for example, that her Greek-American family had significant
overlap among speakers, which instead of being disruptive as previously
considered, helped express affiliation. In fact, in many Mediterranean cul-
tures, interlocutors repeat each other’s comments to express cooperative
conversational behavior, confirm comprehension, and manage interpersonal
relations, such as affiliation or agreement (Acosta & Ward, 2011).
Thus, the meaning of conversational devices requires contextual inter-
pretation. Mismatches in their use may be easily resolved if participants are
willing and able to negotiate what form cooperation should take. We can
ask questions to clarify an interlocutor’s meaning or assume miscommu-
nication rather than disrespect from our interlocutor.
4
https://medium.com/@hana.jafar09/why-i-faked-an-accent-for-years-dabe0c6a04ff
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
7 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Paralinguistic Features 163
Gudmestad (2011) likewise found that, instead of L2 proficiency or time
spent studying abroad, it was learners’ attitude towards the host commu-
nity that determined their adoption of the local language variety.
The cultural forces we are socialized by also impact how we perceive
accents that differ from ours (Lippi-Green, 2012). We find certain accents
more pleasant than others, and attribute traits or intentions to speakers
with those accents based on stories we read or hear during our childhood
and throughout our lives. We may judge people to be exotic, (un)attrac-
tive, (un)trustworthy, (un)intelligent, emotional, or cold, drawing on
stereotypes we develop as a shorthand for determining what a person is
like merely based on their pronunciation (Fuertes, Gottdiener, Martin,
Gilbert, & Giles, 2012; Lippi-Green, 2012; Popp, Donovan, Crawford,
Marsh, & Peele, 2003; Ng, 2007; Vincze & McIntyre, 2017). Typically,
‘standard’ dialects are rated more positively than ‘nonstandard’ or non-
native varieties in terms of competence, status, intelligence, and literacy
(Lev-Ari & Keysar, 2010; Wolfram & Schilling-Estes, 2015; Saito,
Trofimovich, Isaacs, & Webb, 2017; Vincze & McIntyre, 2017).
Consequently, speakers of varieties other than a prestigious standard may
be systematically disadvantaged in job interviews, healthcare and
education:
Accent might reduce the credibility of non-native job seekers, eyewitnesses,
reporters or news anchors . . . such insidious impact of accent is even
apparent when the non-native speaker is merely a messenger. Most likely,
neither the native nor the non-native speakers are aware of this, making the
difficulty of understanding accented speech an ever present reason for
perceiving non-native speakers as less credible. (Lev-Ari & Keysar, 2010,
p. 1095)
In contrast with ‘nonstandard’ varieties, having a nonnative accent
might help interlocutors accommodate the speaker, because they listen
more carefully and accept nonnative language use (e.g., lexical choice,
grammar, pragmatics). Some individuals with nonnative or nonprestigious
accents try to ‘correct’ their pronunciation (Schuller & Batliner, 2014);
their age, motivation, access to L2/Lx materials, and the context of lan-
guage learning and use can facilitate or limit their success (e.g., second
versus foreign language environment). Although as linguists we tend to
view all varieties of language as equal and equally valuable, learners should
be allowed to determine what their language needs are regarding accent. If
their lives are not impacted by having a particular accent (foreign or
regional), they might not want to change it. However, some individuals
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
164 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
have a strong desire or need for reducing their accents and should be
assisted in this endeavor (McCrocklin & Link, 2016; Thomas, 2014).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
7 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Paralinguistic Features 165
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
166 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
that the author means the opposite of the surface-level content (e.g., that
was *really fun*). Writing something all in capital letters also expresses
affect, either enthusiasm when rooting for a sports team (GO GIANTS!!!!!!!)
or extreme emotion (ICH BIN MÜDE!!! [‘I’m tired’]); the affective content
is reiterated in both cases with several exclamation points. Likewise,
repeated letters suggest strong sentiment, mostly humorously
coooooooooolllll (Brody & Diakopoulos, 2011). The surrounding context
helps interpret the meaning of punctuation, font, and, in this case, unusual
spelling.
While functionally complex tools – like emojis, gifs, memes, and typo-
graphic manipulation – help online communication express affect, inter-
preting them takes practice, especially since their use varies by gender
(Brunet & Schmidt, 2010), as well as geography and culture (Jack,
Caldara, & Schyns, 2012):
Easterners, for example employ a vertical style like ˆ ˆ, while westerners
employ a horizontal style like :-). This difference may be due to cultural
reasons since easterners are known to interpret facial expressions from the
eyes, while westerners favor the mouth. (Park, Fink, Barash, & Cha, 2013,
p. 466)
Emoticons also reflect orthographic and cultural practices. In
Arabic, for example, the smiley face goes right to left, resulting in
(: instead of :) as in Western cultures, while South American social
media users often experiment with various eyebrow depictions for
more expressive affective content. Park et al. (2013) note, however,
that language use (i.e., English versus Chinese) is more influential in
determining the use of emoticons than geographic or national-cultural
affiliation.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
7 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Paralinguistic Features 167
The nascent research on L2/Lx development regarding paralinguis-
tics has offered promising results. Specifically, findings suggest that
paralinguistic features are essential for comprehensibility in an L2/Lx
and are teachable, especially with video-supported pedagogy. Chiu
(2012) found, for example, that film dubbing helped ESL learners
produce diverse intonation patterns and stress to express emotions,
emphasize important points, and increase their fluency, possibly
because the intonation patterns in the authentic interactional situa-
tions presented in the movie were easier to distinguish than in
textbook dialogs. Since many students wish to reduce their L1 accents
(even though developing native-like pronunciation is not an objective
in intercultural pedagogy), such activities can improve learners’ self-
confidence, lower their L2 anxiety, and increase their willingness to
communicate (Vincze & McIntyre, 2017).
Similarly, several studies on telecollaboration (groups of students inter-
acting online for pedagogical purposes) utilized video conferencing and
found that these projects helped learners develop more fluent and seamless
conversations, improving their turn-taking skills, intonation, and emo-
tional expressiveness in the L2/Lx, as they formed new social arrangements
and interactions with their interlocutors (Austin et al., 2017; Guichon &
Cohen, 2014; Vurdien & Puranen, 2018). While not the same as face-to-
face interaction, Internet-mediated communication is also part of new,
twenty-first-century skills of using information and communication tech-
nology (Austin et al., 2017), that is ubiquitous in real-world communica-
tion (Satar, 2016).
Undoubtedly, paralinguistic features play an important role in lear-
ners’ ability to interpret and create meaning. Equally importantly, how-
ever, learners’ paralinguistic features impact how they are perceived by
other users of the L2/Lx as well. Saito, Trofimovich, and Isaacs (2017)
conducted a study with native French speakers learning English and
found that learners’ comprehensibility was closely linked with their
pronunciation (of individual phonemes and overall prosody). The
authors emphasize that, while it is important to set realistic instructional
goals regarding what L2/Lx learners can achieve pronunciation-wise,
even small improvements in word and sentence stress, speech rate,
pitch, stress, and pauses can make a large difference in comprehensibility.
These gains are possible if pronunciation practice encourages “learners to
notice, practice, and ultimately automatize various linguistic dimensions
of speech” (p. 460).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
168 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
In a related study by van Maastricht, Zee, Krahmer, and Swerts (2017),
native speakers of Dutch rated their perceptions of speech produced by L2/
Lx learners of Dutch (L1 Spanish speakers) in terms of speech rate, rhythm,
intonation, and (nonnative) accent. The results revealed that intonation,
combined with speech rate, impacted perceptions of accentedness, while
intonation, fluency, and rhythm affected ratings of comprehensibility. The
more native-like the sample language was, the more comprehensible and
less accented it was deemed to be, although the study did not explore what
constitutes easily or hardly comprehensible versus incomprehensible lan-
guage. The authors noted that, while fluency (i.e., speech rate) increased
with proficiency, “specific training to improve intonation” can make a
useful addition to the curriculum and to improve the way learners are
perceived by other speakers of the L2/Lx (p. 367).
Discussing accentedness in the curriculum is also important because,
just as learners’ speech may be viewed negatively if they have a stronger
nonnative accent, they themselves may judge others who speak their
language with a nonnative accent more negatively; helping learners become
aware of this phenomenon can make them more accepting and accom-
modating towards L2/Lx learners of their own L1, and more compassionate
towards themselves as well.
Summary
As the preceding discussion has demonstrated, voice quality, prosody,
conversational management, and accent play an important role in
communication and serve as markers of social identities both in
spoken and written communication. These paralinguistic features do
not serve symbolic referential purposes per se, but rather shape how
we say what we say, thus contributing important details about the
speaker’s affect, emotion, and personality. Fortunately, paralinguistic
features are teachable, especially using filmic materials. Instruction
should go beyond the nation-as-culture level analyses and examine
specific contexts of use; it should also deal explicitly with L1–L2/Lx
(perceived) differences in paralinguistic features, as this approach may
help learners preempt or mitigate miscommunication and stereotyp-
ing. With these points in mind, the following suggestions synthesize
the concepts and guidelines presented in Chapters 1 and 2 in relation
to teaching paralinguistic features in an intercultural communication-
oriented pedagogy.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
7 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Paralinguistic Features 169
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
170 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Sample Teaching Activities
ACTIVITY SET 1
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, learners will be able to do the following:
1. Develop an awareness of how intonation signals questions, commands,
suggestions in these commercials by specific participants.
2. Identify unexpected paralinguistic features and explore why these might
be humorous and what underlying social expectations they may flout.
3. Analyze how paralinguistic features support verbal messages in these
commercials.
Analytic Approach
Multimodal analysis.
Viewing
1. Watch all six commercials without sound and identify the product adver-
tised in each and the participants, filling in the first two rows of the matrix.
Ad 1 Ad 2 Ad 3 Ad 4 Ad 5 Ad 5
1. Product advertised
2. Participants
3. Notes on voice quality
4. Notes on intonation
5. Possible target audience
6. How does voice quality
and intonation help
convey the ad’s message
to the target audience?
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
7 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Paralinguistic Features 171
(cont.)
Ad 1 Ad 2 Ad 3 Ad 4 Ad 5 Ad 5
2. Working in six groups, one per ad, learners observe the voice quality and
intonation patterns of the speakers in each commercial, watch the video
once, then have groups discuss their findings, filling in Rows 3 and 4.
3. Watch the video for a second time, giving groups a few minutes to refine
their observations and polish their notes in Rows 3 and 4.
4. Have groups present their findings to the rest of the class.
5. Together, discuss the responses to Rows 5 and 6. When discussing Row
6, use guiding questions: What does a fast v. a slow tempo imply in this
particular context? When do the speakers speak loudly or quietly and
why? Is a higher or lower pitch – especially when unexpected (see the
ring tone ad) – associated with a particular social expectation for men or
for women? What social expectations regarding paralinguistic features
do these ads indicate?
Post-Viewing
Forming new groups of three to five, learners create a commercial for a product
of their choice; include the paralinguistic features analyzed in the main task.
Analysis
The analyses at this level might be relatively superficial, but the objective is
simply to raise learners’ awareness about and ability to analyze paralinguistic
features in natural/naturalistic discourse. By drawing their attention to
these features, learners can process future interactions more carefully and
identify ways in which they may wish to adopt paralinguistic features into
their own communicative practices. To facilitate this process, learners need
to be able to build hypotheses (Row 5 prompts this) and evaluate the
accuracy of their hypotheses based on language data (Row 6). While such
tasks are time-consuming at first, the process becomes faster with practice,
and since paralinguistic features contribute in significant ways to meaning-
making and the way learners are perceived by other speakers of the L2/Lx,
this is a worthwhile investment of time. This set of activities intentionally
focuses on only a few paralinguistic features (tempo, volume, pitch, and
intonation), because beginning-level learners cannot process longer
discourse segments that would allow for more nuanced analyses of
conversation management or the impact of language variation. Tasks
should encourage observation, not frustrate learners.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
172 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
ACTIVITY SET 2
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, learners will be able to identify the following:
1. Several conversation management devices (e.g., hedges, fillers, laughter)
appropriate for the specific genre of each show.
2. Whether overlapping turn-taking is allowed or even encouraged, and in
what way participants (e.g., the host, guest, studio audience) participate
in the interaction.
3. How conversation management devices help create meaning in combi-
nation with the verbal message.
4. The consequences of flouting expectations, for example, by attending to
the reactions of the studio audience to the speakers’ utterances (e.g.,
gasps, laughter, disapproval).
Analytic Approach
Multimodal analysis, ethnography of communication, and interactional
sociolinguistics.
Viewing
Students view and analyze their shows alone or (ideally) in groups as
homework, using guided questions. Sample questions may include the
following:
1. What is the name of the show?
2. What do you know about that channel and the show (owner/host,
reporting bias, etc.)?
3. What is its genre or purpose?
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
7 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Paralinguistic Features 173
Post-Viewing
1. Students report their findings to the rest of the class. If multiple students
watch the same show individually as homework, they should have the
opportunity to meet for 15–20 minutes in class to discuss their findings
and prepare a collective presentation.
2. Time permitting, and depending on learners’ interest, small groups
could perform a segment of a show to practice producing paralinguistic
features to complement their verbal messages.
Analysis
By analyzing a small section of one show together, learners’ attention can be
drawn to the local context. Who are the participants? What is the purpose of
this segment? Explore the emotions expressed by the participants. What
conversation management devices, such as hedges, fillers, backchanneling
devices, or other features can learners observe? How do the participants
cooperatively or competitively construct their conversation with the help of
these devices? These questions help raise learners’ awareness of
conversation-management devices, while simultaneously building their
analytic skills using authentic resources that reflect multiple layers of
cultural patterns (e.g., national, regional, local) and individual variation. If
learners get to re-create a segment of the show, the repeated practice can
foster fluency and improve learners’ use of paralinguistic features.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
174 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:47:55, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.008
chapter 8
Chapter Overview
Our appearance, gestures, body language, and other modes of nonverbal
communication convey large amounts of information about who we are,
what we want to accomplish in an interaction, or our attitude towards
others (Pentland, 2008). In addition, nonverbal communication estab-
lishes and maintains relationships, status, honesty, or deception (Martin
& Nakayama, 2018); it is perceived quickly and mostly subconsciously,
drawing on culturally patterned expectations. Some nonverbal commu-
nication is consistent across cultures (e.g., the palm-up gesture for ‘give me’
and raised eyebrows in recognition of someone; Martin & Nakayama,
2018), but otherwise, nonverbal communication reflects great cultural
variation. For example, when and how we touch others, how we relate to
time, or what clothes we wear is interpreted and enacted differently among
social communities.
The expressive capacity of nonverbal communication sometimes enables
us to rely on it completely in the absence of a shared language. In the Tate
Modern Museum in London, an exhibit by the Turkish artist Erkan
Özgen’s – Wonderland 2016 1 – presents a short video in which
Muhammed, a child refugee from Syria, describes his horrific experiences
in his devastated home country. He is deaf and does not know Turkish, yet
Muhammed vividly portrays the horrors he had seen in war to the inter-
viewer and his multilingual audiences: bombs exploding, houses collap-
sing, prisoners being executed, and feeling sad. The viewer cannot but
agree with the artist who explained that “perhaps only Muhammed could
truly communicate this unseen and unheard brutality . . . The power of his
body language made any other language form insufficient and insignif-
icant.” Indeed, nonverbal communication is an important conveyor of
1
Artist and Society, Summer 2018; www.youtube.com/watch?v=FuUz3giIbSg
175
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
176 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
information, whether our message is intentional or accidental, and regard-
less of the form it might take.
Byram (1997), based on Van Ek’s (1986) model, outlines several aspects
of nonverbal communication: facial expressions, gaze, gestures and bodily
movements, posture, contact, spatial behavior, clothes and appearance,
and nonverbal aspects of speech. These types of nonverbal communication
map onto seven broader categories used in social science research: signs,
personal presentation, kinesics, haptics, proxemics, chronemics, and voca-
lics. Except for vocalics (Byram included paralinguistic features in non-
verbal communication), which were covered in the previous chapter, these
categories guide the following discussion.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
8 Intercultural Communication 177
photograph of a swim-team. Similarly, onomatopoetic words, like hiss for
the sound of a snake or tick-tock for a clock, vary across languages (e.g.,
English dogs say bow-wow, while Hungarian dogs go vau-vau). Icons must
closely resemble what they signify in order to be recognized, although their
core representation may vary across different cultures.
An index, the second type of sign, indicates an observable phenomenon,
something we can see, hear, sense, smell, or taste. A tornado siren indexes
the arrival of a damaging storm, a referee’s whistle indicates that a player
did not follow the rules of the game, while an accent can reveal a speaker’s
affiliation with a particular social group. Although some facial expressions
(e.g., a frown) and other indexes (e.g., storm clouds) are universal, many
are culture specific. We are socialized into interpreting and using them in
particular ways in our interactions. An example of this would be the bindi
(the red dot on the forehead) in Hindu culture2, which can be used to ward
off bad luck, similar to the blue and white evil eye talismans, called mati in
Greek and nazar boncuğu in Turkish (see Figure 8.2). Several lexical items
are also indexes: here, there, this, that, and personal pronouns, because they
reference specific observable places or individuals.
Finally, symbols do not resemble the signified object, concept, or person.
Instead, the connection between the signifier and the signified is culturally
determined. Some supranational symbols represent the Olympic Games or
the International Red Cross and Red Crescent organizations, whose aim is
to help provide relief during large-scale emergencies. Similarly, fans of the
Harry Potter books have learned that the four animals on the Hogwarts
coat of arms represent courage, intelligence, power, and loyalty. Infamous
symbols include the swastika that Nazis appropriated from Hinduism3
(where it signals good fortune and prosperity) and the white pointed hat
2
https://nileswestnews.org/31336/west-word/bindis-are-not-a-fashion-trend/
3
www.britannica.com/story/how-the-symbolism-of-the-swastika-was-ruined
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
178 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Personal Presentation
How we present ourselves to others – our appearance and adornments,
such as jewelry, vehicles, tattoos, body piercings, clothing – conveys
4
www.history.com/topics/ku-klux-klan
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
8 Intercultural Communication 179
messages to our interlocutors, whether intentionally or unintentionally.
We may have short hair to present an image of being hip, sporty, or young.
We may wear a suit and tie to appear more professional at a business
meeting, or wear nurse’s scrubs with prints of fun animals to help kids relax
at the hospital. These are intentional messages. Other messages we convey
unintentionally. For example, we might wear a color that has special
meaning for a culture (e.g., red for luck in China) or one that a social
group finds threatening (e.g., a gang identifier in Los Angeles5). Yet other
messages that we convey are beyond our control, such as skin color or
height. The value placed on skin color is socially learned and can have life-
altering implications, such as longer and harsher prison sentences, as has
been found in Brazil and the United States6 (Wade & Bielitz, 2005).
Notably, the intended meaning of nonverbal communication comprises
only part of the message that is received by our interlocutors, whose
socialization, personal experiences, and preferences influence the meaning
they glean from nonverbal cues. One person might find a tattooed, long-
haired young man scary, while another individual happily greets a kindred
spirit in him. Similarly, some fans of the Cleveland Indians might consider
t-shirts with their team logo to reflect team spirit, whereas other fans and
many members of Native American tribes7 find the image offensive and
dismissive of the long history of the U.S. government’s oppression of
indigenous peoples. Such cultural appropriation – the adoption of imagery,
clothing, and behaviors that is part of the fabric of a particular social group
by powerful, dominant cultures – is often used to ridicule or repress
members of that social group (Riley & Carpenter, 2016; Matthes, 2016).
Whether clothing with Native American symbolism elicits support or
condemnation, and how that support or condemnation is expressed, is
codetermined by immediate and secondary, ratified and unratified parti-
cipants in the communicative event: how the fans, the team, its manage-
ment, affected Native American tribes, viewers, courts, and the
government interpret the situation.
As with other aspects of communication, situational factors are neces-
sary to consider when interpreting nonverbal communication. Showing up
in sweatpants and a t-shirt for a job interview at a multicultural company in
Taiwan will unlikely net the candidate a job, but this attire is perfectly
acceptable for a summer camp counselor. Similarly, situational factors
5
www.lapdonline.org/get_informed/content_basic_view/23468
6
https://courses2.cit.cornell.edu/sociallaw/student_projects/Definingbias.html
7
www.theatlantic.com/entertainment/archive/2018/02/looking-beyond-chief-wahoo/552386/
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
180 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
inform expected body language; a camp counselor can high-five children,
while this action might be frowned upon at a job interview.
Kinesic
behaviors Definition Example
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
8 Intercultural Communication 181
Eye-Contact
Eye-contact, which is an essential component of nonverbal communication
in most face-to-face interactions, represents culturally diverse practices.
While in many languages we have expressions similar to the English The
eyes are the window to the soul or The eye is the true outrider and faithful guide
of the soul 8 in Arabic, we must be careful when enacting or decoding eye-
contact. In much of the United States eye-contact lasts a second or so, then
interactants look away during conversations. Eye-contact is expanded only
when we wish to emphasize a point or signal that we are ready to relinquish
our turn at speaking, as a regulator (Martin & Nakayama, 2010). From the
listeners’ perspective, eye-contact indicates that they are paying attention,
but maintaining eye-contact beyond expected norms may seem threatening
to the speaker. In novel cultural encounters, we need to learn whether we
can look at others directly or are expected to be more indirect in our gaze,
how long we may maintain eye-contact before we appear aggressive, whether
looking away indicates respect or fearfulness, and how eye-gaze is influenced
by social status, gender, or age, just to name a few possible variables.
Facial Expressions
Like eye-contact, facial expressions contribute important information to our
interlocutors. Several facial expressions had been long held to be universal
across humans: anger, disgust, fear, happiness, and sadness (Andersen, 1999).
Recent psychophysical research using image analyses, however, suggests that
rather than being biologically hard-wired and innate, recognizing even basic
emotions is culturally informed. Jack, Caldara, and Schyns (2012), whose
study sorted participants into members of Western Caucasian and East Asian
cultures, found that cultural conditioning generated distinct internal repre-
sentations of facial expressions between the two groups: Western Caucasian
participants seemed to rely on the eyebrows and mouth in the images to
determine what emotion they conveyed, while East Asian participants seemed
to focus on the eyes and gaze direction depicted in the images (see Chapter 7
regarding the use of emojis in different cultural contexts as well). Other
research (Sauter, 2006; Sauter, Eisner, Ekman, & Scott, 2010, cited in
8
“The eye is the true outrider and faithful guide of the soul; it is the soul’s well-polished mirror, by
means of which it comprehends all truths, attains all qualities, and understands all sensible
phenomena.” Translation based on Arthur John Arberry’s translation of “The Ring of the
Dove” by Ibn Hazm, 994–1064. Accessed at https://blogs.transparent.com/arabic/the-arabic-art-
of-hinting-with-the-eyes/
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
182 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Schuller & Batliner, 2014; Susskind, Lee, Cusi, Feiman, Grabski, &
Anderson, 2008) found similar perceptions of negative facial expressions
(fear, anger) across two distinct cultural groups, but divergent perceptions
for positive emotions, suggesting that the latter are communicated in culture-
specific ways.
While the universality of negative emotions suggests an evolutionary
necessity for recognizing potential threats in order to stay alive, what causes
people to experience these emotions, and how they express them with facial
expressions, is determined by cultural and social expectations. We may retain
the full-throated belly-laugh of childhood if our cultural expectations allow, or
we learn to soften it and express joy in more ‘decorous’ ways. We may also
laugh differently at a wedding than when watching a child’s antics or when we
feel compelled to laugh at our supervisor’s joke. Not just the how, but the
where and when we can laugh is culturally determined: Is laughter appropriate
in church? At a funeral? During a job interview? With an elder of our
community?
Some facial expressions are involuntary – including some laughter, pain,
or surprise – but most of them are under our control. They can help
support our verbal communication, aligning two modes of expression to
convey meaning to our interlocutors. When we enter a new cultural
context, however, we have to learn new ways of expressing our thoughts,
often matching expectations different from our socialization. In order to
learn how facial expressions are used (alongside verbal messages) in another
culture, authentic documentaries or feature films, comedy shows, or news
reports can offer valuable data. What emotions do speakers convey when
they are smiling: openness and confidence or sarcasm? Do their raised
eyebrows signal excitement, curiosity, incredulity, or smugness?
Importantly, facial expressions should be interpreted as a component of
a larger communicative palette, which includes verbal, paralinguistic, and
other nonverbal channels of meaning-making.
Gestures
Gestures vary greatly across cultures, in terms of their meaning, how much
they are used, and how much space they are used in. They help individuals
process their thoughts as they speak, connect concrete ideas with their
inner thoughts, lessen the listeners’ cognitive load by reducing the amount
of time required to express an idea (gestures can act as short-cuts to larger
concepts), and mitigate potential social risk by expressing affiliation or
hesitation (Park, Carter, Wiebe, Reid-Griffin, & Butler, 2006).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
8 Intercultural Communication 183
Gestures, which support almost 90 percent of our verbal utterances
(Bonvillain, 2020), are most commonly adaptors, emblems, and illustra-
tors (Jones, 2017). Adaptors reflect internal emotional states, such as anxiety
or happiness, while emblems depict a word or expression, such as signaling
‘patience’ or ‘slow down’ by waving our hands slowly in front of our body.9
The third type of gesture, illustrators, is the most common. Illustrators
coindex our verbal messages, suggesting how big or small, round or square
an object is, or whether it is ‘here’ or ‘there.’
For the most part, gestures are socially bound. The same gesture can
carry a different meaning in different cultures, such as the sign for ‘ok’ in
the United States, which means something vulgar in Brazil. Similarly, the
same meaning can be expressed by different gestures within and across
cultures: a handshake, a fist-bump, a pat on the shoulder, or a wave can be
used to greet people. Hand gestures are almost always accompanied by
other bodily movements, which are discussed in the next section.
9
Emblems are different from sign languages in that they are a limited array of signs performed with
our hands or arms, whereas sign languages are formal linguistic systems with complex grammars,
lexicons, and pragmatics.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
184 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Different cultures incorporate body movements as part of rituals and
celebrations: kneeling during a Catholic mass or a Muslim prayer to show
devotion to God, dancing at a Greek wedding with your arms around your
neighbors, or performing a Maori haka. There are everyday differences as
well: who gets to walk with whom (e.g., are men and women allowed to
walk together), whether you stand in a line or push together when boarding
a train, or whether you bow as a greeting. Our preferred practices often
require adjustment in new cultural contexts, where we need to learn how to
interpret and encode new body language, but also become aware of our
affective reactions to it, so that we can adapt to new patterns of behavior or
recognize the limits of what we are comfortable with, including touching.
Haptics
Haptics refers to touching as a form of communication, from a caress of
affection to a high-five congratulating a fellow teammate. Whether a touch
threatens, welcomes, persuades, or soothes depends on who is doing the
touching, for what purpose, and under what circumstances. Touching can
be accidental (e.g., bumping into someone on a crowded subway),
functional-professional (e.g., nurses evaluating their patients), social-polite
(pat on the shoulder to ask someone to let us pass them in a busy hallway),
friendship-warmth (e.g., holding hands, hugging, kissing each other on the
cheek), love-intimacy (touching between lovers, best friends, close family
members), and sexual touching between lovers (Jones, 2017). Touching
reflects status and gender differences as well, but in general, what one
person may be comfortable with vis-à-vis one friend, she or he may not
appreciate from another person or even from the same person in other
contexts. Think, for example, of teenagers accepting a hug from their
parents at home, but perhaps not in front of their schoolmates (Floyd,
2006).
What touch we prefer or are used to varies across cultures and even
among members of the same culture, of course. In much of Latin America,
for example, female friends kiss each other on the cheek (or rather past each
other’s cheeks), while men tend to shake hands and clap each other on the
shoulder. Among Japanese women, less physical contact is preferred, even
among close friends. Men in many parts of the United States exhibit
relatively short handshakes, and even among close friends, a pat on the
shoulder is considered a sufficient exhibition of closeness. However, they
may offer each other longer, fuller body hugs at times of grief or celebration
or even extended contact, such as members of a sports team sharing
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
8 Intercultural Communication 185
a ‘group-hug’ upon winning the championship. In other cultures, the
expectations are different. Men in Jordan, for example, walk and dance
with their arms around each other. Differences in haptics relate to expecta-
tions of closeness as well.
Proxemics
Proxemics refers to spatial behavior or what distances are appropriate in
different types of communication. It is helpful to conceptualize space as
concentric circles around ourselves; our notions of space depend on our
socialization and cultural preferences.
In the United States, spatial preferences generally entail four zones
(Hall, 1968; Jones, 2017): public, social, personal, and intimate space (see
Figure 8.4). We require considerable space in public functions (about 12
feet), less at a formal get-together (4–12 feet; conversation at a conference,
teacher–student distance in the classroom), even less among friends (1.5–4
feet; friendly conversation at a party), and little space with intimates (0–1.5
feet; interactions with close friends and family).
As with other aspects of nonverbal communication, space is locally
navigated: Who are we speaking to? What is the purpose of the interaction?
Even the same interactants might need more space if their exchange is
contentious rather than pleasant. Added to the personal variability of
spatial needs are, of course, socially patterned preferences, from narrower
to broader cultural influences. In urban areas, where space is more limited,
public space
12 feet
social space
self
Figure 8.4 Proxemic zones in the United States (based on Hall, 1968 and Jones,
2017)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
186 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
people live and interact with greater physical proximity than in rural areas.
When our cultural patterns and personal experiences are a mismatch with
the expectations of our interlocutors and the local setting, we feel out of our
‘comfort zone,’ an apt metaphor for mismatches that can lead to confusion
regarding our intentions or actions.
After having spent thirty years in the United States with only brief visits
to Hungary, I got to spend six months in my home country on sabbatical
a year ago. Standing in the line at the bakery, I – reflexively, based on my
habit in the United States – stood a comfortable 3–4 feet away from the
person in front of me, who was ordering. I felt that the person behind me
was really crowding into my personal space, and in fact, she ended up going
ahead of me, thinking that I was waiting around for someone else and not
actually standing in line. The salesperson smiled when I finally got my turn
and told me that next time I should stand ‘properly’ close to the window.
During the six months, I still often found myself feeling uncomfortable
with how little space people seemed to need, but I became more aware of
the source of my discomfort, recognizing that less space was culturally
more appropriate in that tightly spaced urban area.
Many different reactions may be appropriate when we encounter spatial
miscalculations (whether we are the perpetrators or others). Depending on
the interactional context, we may ignore a transgression, apologize, express
discomfort with a glare or a sharp excuse me, or react with anger or humor.
Although there are occasions when territory is deadly serious (e.g., gang
violence), usually, a smile, an apology, or a humorous comment can be
effective tools for diffusing an embarrassing but unintentional mishap,
which is most often the case in intercultural encounters.
In addition to personal space, the notion of private and public space is
also relevant. From our primary space – the home, imbued with cultural
references pertaining to socioeconomic status, cultural heritage, etc. – to
the neighborhood and region (e.g., city or state), the country and the
physical environment all shape social and power structures, and the iden-
tities of people who live there. Do we keep the doors closed in the house or
is the space more open? Is there a well-funded school in the neighborhood?
Is it safe to live there? Is there public transportation? Hiking? Watersports?
These so-called cultural spaces (Martin & Nakayama, 2018) influence
communal practices, including how people do their shopping (e.g., at
the supermarket, the local market, small shops), how they interact with
each other (e.g., in cafés or the public square), and how much contact they
may have with other cultural groups.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
8 Intercultural Communication 187
Chronemics
Chronemics refers to temporal aspects of nonverbal communication, and,
according to Andersen (1999), include:
1. biological time: physiological pattern of life, such as the circadian
rhythm; disruptions of this rhythm affect our ability to function and
cope with novel situations;
2. cultural time: notions of time as viewed by cultural groups
polychronic time: preference for a flexible schedule; not knowing
when something will happen or arriving late to events is
acceptable or even preferred;
monochronic time: preference for a set schedule, punctuality, and
completing one task at a time; activities and time are broken up
into smaller units; participants (e.g., guests, colleagues) are
expected to arrive on time for events or work;
3. personal time: individual perceptions of time (i.e., fast or slow);
orientation towards the past or present (past-oriented individuals
spend a great deal of time on visiting old friends, while future-
oriented individuals spend their time planning out their next actions
and goals). Polychronic cultures tend to be past-oriented, whereas
monochronic cultures are more likely to be future-oriented
(Neuliep, 2018); and
4. physical time: the pattern of days (e.g., availability of sunlight), years
and seasons, which can affect emotional states (e.g., prompting happi-
ness or sadness).
Two contexts illustrate these concepts. First, in many African cultures,
time is viewed as cyclical and polychronic (Nwosu, 2009). Historical
orientation is very important, but present-time schedules are flexible,
leaving enough flexibility to allow for lengthy greeting rituals.
Relationships are prioritized over schedules, “because time is not
a quantifiable commodity” (Nwosu, 2009, p. 171). In contrast, in mono-
chronic cultures, like Germany or the United States, punctuality is of the
essence in most social situations. Whereas arriving a few minutes late to
a party is acceptable, we must arrive on time to class and we ‘must hurry!’ if
a flash sale ends in just an hour. However, social roles impact how people
view time: a doctor may come late to an appointment in the United States
and ‘get away’ with a quick apology (or none at all), whereas the patient is
expected to be on time. Similarly, the Chief Executive Officer of
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
188 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
a company may show up late for a meeting (in some cultural contexts), but
other participants are expected to be punctual, barring a solid excuse for
tardiness.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
8 Intercultural Communication 189
Tuareg continually interpret nonverbal cues, as they explore each other’s
intentions, wanting to stay safe from any threats but also seeking human
companionship during their long, lonely, and arduous journey. As this
example illustrates, multiple channels of communication converge to
encode meaning, which others have to decode accurately, relying on
affective and cognitive cues.
Recent scholarship (Pentland, 2008; Scherer & Brosch, 2009;
Schuller & Batliner, 2014; Vinciarelli, Pantic, & Bourlard, 2009;
Vinciarelli & Mohammadi, 2011) posits that the affective and cogni-
tive processes that guide our interpretive process of external stimuli
(e.g., body movements, facial expressions, gestures) reflect cultural
predispositions, impacting, and often limiting, our ability to interpret
and convey subtle social signals nonverbally in new cultural settings.
In other words, we process the world around us mostly automatically,
and anything different – that which violates our expectations (Floyd,
Ramirez, & Burgoon, 2008) – requires significant cognitive and
affective work.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
190 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
To change this trend, instructors can implement numerous strate-
gies to teach nonverbal communication. While the summary table in
the next section and the activity sets provide detailed recommenda-
tions, some general guidelines are worth pointing out here. For each
aspect of nonverbal communication – signs, personal presentation,
kinesics, haptics, proxemics, chronemics, and vocalics – authentic
resources can provide contextualized presentation to identify broader
cultural trends, variation among smaller cultural groups, and inter-
personal uses of nonverbal communication in locally situated interac-
tions. Since it is impossible to learn every single nonverbal
communicative tool used by another culture, learners should become
adept at recognizing and interpreting patterns among members of
a cultural group they identify with, and decide which of their non-
verbal communication tools they wish to adopt to present themselves
in novel cultural contexts. In order to explore those patterns, all three
analytic approaches can be deployed complementarily: the ethnogra-
phy of communication can be a useful starting point for analyzing the
communicative context, interactional sociolinguistics can identify the
verbal content, while multimodal analyses can explore how verbal and
nonverbal communication co-create meaning. The analyses should be
followed by practice, so that learners have the opportunity to develop
procedural knowledge, and reflection, so that they can make informed
decisions about the nonverbal communication they want or need to
adopt. Given this complex structure, the two activity sets described
later in this chapter are reiterative and extended, with multiple view-
ings of authentic filmic materials to ensure a solid understanding of
nonverbal behavior portrayed by the participants.
Summary
This section lays out explicitly the existing and potential further con-
nections between nonverbal communication and intercultural commu-
nication, in terms of the guidelines presented in Chapter 2 (Byram, 1997;
Hua, 2014; Kramsch, 2009; Liddicoat & Scarino, 2013) and the concepts
discussed in Chapter 1 (e.g., Hall, 1976; Hofstede, et al., 2010;
Kluckhohn & Strodtbeck, 1961). Several suggestions emerge out of
these discussions to support an intercultural communication-oriented
pedagogy.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
8 Intercultural Communication 191
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
192 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
(cont.)
ACTIVITY SET 1
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, learners will develop an awareness of the
following:
1. Different personal presentations and some social connotations associated
with them (e.g., hairstyle, clothing, jewelry).
2. Several kinesic preferences and their meaning in context (e.g., facial
expressions, eye contact, gestures, body movement).
3. Preferences for haptics (e.g., touching, kissing), proxemics (e.g., perso-
nal and public spaces), and time (e.g., punctuality, orientation) among
different relationship types.
Depending on pedagogical objectives, learner interest or practical
considerations (e.g., available time), instructors may wish to focus on only
one or a few of these objectives.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
8 Intercultural Communication 193
Analytic approach
Ethnography of communication and multimodal analysis.
Viewing
1. Watch the next 10-minute segment, providing students with one or two
content questions to ensure comprehension (multiple-choice or true-
false for speed) and a few questions that ask learners to focus on ONE
aspect of nonverbal communication in the show (e.g., proxemics, hap-
tics, or self-presentation).
2. Discuss the answers to the comprehension questions, as well as learners’
findings regarding nonverbal communication.
3. Watch the next 10-minute segment of the episode, including content
questions to support comprehension, as well as a few questions pertaining to:
• the same nonverbal communication aspect, if that is the focal point
of the day,
• a different nonverbal communication aspect for the entire class, or
• different aspects of nonverbal communication for small groups of
learners.
Sample questions might include:
• What were different characters wearing to various events in the
show? What does their appearance suggest about how they wish to
present themselves to others?
• What did the gesture by Character X mean? How was it used to
support the character’s verbal message? What were the other char-
acters’ responses to the gesture?
• How did different characters greet each other nonverbally? (e.g.,
kinesics, haptics)? What information did their greeting seem to
communicate about their relationships towards each other (or
towards other participants)?
4. Discuss the answers to the comprehension questions, as well as the
groups’ findings regarding nonverbal communication.
• Depending on learners’ access to the L2/Lx-speaking community, they
could request further information by vetting their hypotheses with
people who have more experience living in that culture/those cultures.
• If students perceived the character’s appearance differently, this
could help them realize that aspects of nonverbal communication
can be decoded in divergent ways.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
194 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Post-Viewing
Using pictures from magazines, drawings or an online program,10 learner
groups create a storyboard for a scene for a future episode of the TV series,
including verbal and nonverbal communication that characters would use;
groups present their stories.
Analysis
In this activity set, the emphasis is on the process of observation – identifying
nonverbal communication and recognizing other participants’ responses to
it – and the locally contextualized interpretation of nonverbal communication.
Thus, learners should not draw generalized conclusions about individuals’
behavior, but focus on how nonverbal communication is enacted in specific
interactions, until sufficient data has been compiled to be able to draw
tentative conclusions about patterns of behavior and the social meaning they
intend to convey. At the same time, learners’ reactions are acknowledged,
since they are a natural component of the interpersonal nature of encoding
and decoding nonverbal communication (e.g., strong reactions to nonverbal
behavior suggesting or depicting violence). During the analytic process,
learners’ reactions and the reasons for those reactions should be part of the
conversation, especially how they impact the interpretation of others’
nonverbal messages. As an important pedagogical point, learners do not need
to learn all the technical terms (e.g., emblems, illustrators, affect displays,
regulators, or adaptors). Instead, questions and activity prompts should help
them develop the procedural knowledge for observing and interpreting
nonverbal communication.
ACTIVITY SET 2
10
www.storyboardthat.com/storyboard-creator
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
8 Intercultural Communication 195
Learning Objectives
By the end of the term, learners will have achieved the following:
1. Have developed in-depth knowledge of nonverbal communication used
in several everyday routines and special rituals.
2. Be able to synthesize complementary, contradictory, or confirmatory
information about nonverbal behaviors in specific contexts and on
a specific topic.
3. Be able to conduct systematic observations, supported by evidence,
regarding nonverbal behaviors in specific situations in the L2/Lx culture.
Analytic Approach
Ethnography of communication and multimodal analysis.
Task
Over the course of the semester, learners – either individually or in small
groups – create a multimodal/multimedia montage of a communicative
event they are interested in. Learners present their reports during 15-minute
presentations in class, engaging their peers in reflective discussions about the
event. The presentations can be done orally, on PowerPoint, in a wiki or
blog, via social media or other collaborative platforms (e.g., Facebook or
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
196 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
• What is the event and what is its significance for the community in which
it takes place?
• Who are the participants in the event?
• What signs (symbols, icons, indexes) are a part of this event, if any
(including music)? What is the meaning and origin of these signs? Has
the meaning of these signs changed over time? What are the affective
connotations of these signs, and do these vary for different cultural
groups?
• How do people present themselves and with what purpose?
• What kinesic features are particular to this event (facial expressions, eye-
contact, gestures, body movement)?
• How do participants touch each other as part of this event?
• How do participants view space in this event (personal space, public
versus private space)?
• When does this event occur? Why then? What is its orientation towards
time (past or future)? How long does it last? How frequently?
This task can be done via telecollaboration as well, with members of both
cultural groups sharing their knowledge and expertise. The dialog between
each group’s emic and etic understanding of nonverbal communication can
lend itself well to awareness raising and in-depth analysis.
Post-Task
As a possible post-task, learners could write a brief reflective paper
describing what they learned from this research project, what they had not
expected but found. The objective should be trying to understand how
multiple channels of nonverbal communication contribute to meaning-
making in this communicative event.
Analysis
As throughout the book, the emphasis is on the local situatedness of
communication; thus, learners should focus on one event, and perhaps one
social group, to help prevent stereotyping or overgeneralizations. Imagine,
for example, how a family with small children might celebrate Halloween
(our example from Chapter 4) as opposed to a group of college students or
colleagues at their workplace. Each will have unique ways of celebrating the
holiday as well as sharing some nonverbal features.
It is important to remember that the communicative events that students
research and report on should not be only big cultural events (e.g., holidays,
religious or government celebrations), but also include everyday culture,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
8 Intercultural Communication 197
1. What are some significant icons, indexes, and symbols that you
encounter regularly in your various cultural affiliations? In what contexts
are they used, and what is their meaning for those who use them (they
might have divergent connotations for members of different cultural
groups)?
2. What are some hand gestures that are common in your own cultural
experience? What do they signify, who can use them and in which
situations? Do they have the same meaning in cultures in which your L2/
Lx is spoken?
3. What are the cultural practices regarding time in the communities where
your L1 and L2/Lx are spoken (e.g., orientation, punctuality, meta-
phors)?
4. Observe how people use eye-contact in everyday communication in
your current cultural context. Who makes eye-contact (first)? Are there
power-distance rules for eye-contact or gender-role expectations? How
does eye-contact vary across different social situations (e.g., in classroom
interaction, with a sales associate at the store)? If possible (while staying
safe), flout expectations and record what you observe. What was your
affective response to the experience? How did people react?
5. Observe proxemics and haptics in your own cultural practices. What are
comfortable ways of touching friends and colleagues? What is not
allowed? Are there power-distance rules or gender-based differences in
permissible touching? How close can people stand to each other? How
does the permitted distance vary according to social situation?
6. Analyze a speech event in terms of the verbal and nonverbal commu-
nication participants enact. How do multiple modes of communication
co-create meaning?
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:48:43, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.009
chapter 9
Chapter Overview
Cultural knowledge is a fundamental part of participating in intercultural
communication. Our knowledge of the world – including history, current
issues, belief systems, values, and daily practices – helps us navigate both
the surface-level meaning of language and its cultural significance. Cultural
knowledge allows us to participate in social/cultural practices (e.g., know-
ing how to participate in the educational system or attending prayers).
Conversely, without adequate or appropriate cultural knowledge, the
message of our interlocutors remains incomplete, even if we comprehend
the lexicon or grammar accurately, as the following example between
a graduate student from the United States studying in London, England
and a potential landlord illustrates:
flat owner: Hello, Mick.
student: Hi, my name is Jack. I’m calling about the room you’re renting out.
Is it still available?
mick: Oh, yeah. Like it said in the advert, it’s ehm £240 a week, furnished.
We share the bathroom and the kitchen, and there is a garden in the
back that we share with the downstairs flat.
student: That sounds great. Could I come and take a look at it?
mick: Yes, can you come tomorrow at half three?
student: O.K. [pause] Half three. I’ll be there.
mick: Ehm, do you know how to find it?
student: Yeah, it’s just off of the Camden Town tube station, right?
mick: Yes, just up Kentish Town Road a ways, then you’ll see the Abbey
Tavern Pub on the right, turn right there . . . [gives directions]
Since Jack is a native speaker of English, he recognized all the words uttered
by his interlocutor, and he could draw on his background knowledge to
understand most of Mick’s responses. Jack was able to figure out from the
written advertisement that flat meant apartment. He also knew that in
London he was going to have to pay in pounds, and he was aware after
198
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
9 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Cultural Knowledge 199
reading multiple advertisements that rent is often listed as a weekly rather
than monthly fee like in the United States. He was also able to read the
map and locate the flat and was aware of the public transportation system
in London and how it related to the map of London (i.e., locate stations
and orient himself from the destination station). Yet, some of the informa-
tion remained unclear. Different ways of stating time, for example, made it
difficult for Jack to interpret the meaning of the expression half three, and
whether it referenced half an hour before or after three o’clock. He showed
up a little before 2:30 pm, then walked around the area until 3:30, which
turned out to be the correct time. He also needed to figure out expectations
for punctuality, how to address Mick or where to live as a student, among
other relevant information.
In order to participate in a community, we must commit to learning
relevant cultural knowledge; the longer and more intensely we wish to
participate in that community, the deeper we have to understand the
historical, political, artistic, and other social forces that have shaped it
and that continue to shape its culture. Since cultures are dynamic, learning
about them is an ongoing endeavor. As Byram and Masuhara (2013)
observe, knowledge about a culture is as important for intercultural com-
munication as training in skills, behaviors, and attitudes.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
200 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
refinement, literature and the fine arts, patterns of life, and the totality of
life. He suggested emphasizing patterns of life in particular, in order to help
learners manage “life situations of every kind and the rules and models for
attitude and conduct in them” (p. 210). Similarly, Lafayette’s (1988) model
included both everyday and special aspects of culture:
1. facts about high culture (e.g., major geographical monuments,
historical events, institutions);
2. knowledge about popular, everyday culture;
3. values and affect (i.e., developing respect towards other peoples and
societies);
4. respect for various ethnic groups and people in other cultures as well as
learners’ own culture(s); and
5. critical knowledge about interpreting culture (i.e., being able to eval-
uate the validity of claims about cultures).
To capture this duality, cultural knowledge has been described as ‘small
c’ and ‘big C’ culture (Brooks, 1971; Seelye, 1993). Small-c culture subsumes
those components of life with which we deal every day, the mundane and
quotidian: when we begin and stop working, how we spend our weekends,
when we eat and with whom, or how to get a cellphone. Big-C culture, in
contrast, refers to the more idealized sense of a community, “the best of
everything in human life” (Brooks, 1971, p. 20): famous people and events
in history, great works of music, literature, and the arts, and national- or
supranational-level celebrations. Since culture “embraces all aspects of
human life” (Seelye, 1993, p. 22), it is impossible to list everything pertinent
in these pages, therefore Table 9.1 presents only a sampling of cultural
information we need to learn and understand to function in society.
Additionally, Seelye (1993) recommended several metacognitive goals to
be included in L2/Lx pedagogy, including that students should:
1. develop an interest in other cultures, and empathy for members of
those cultures;
2. recognize how social variables (e.g., age, sex, class, religion) and role
expectations impact our language use and behavior;
3. understand that culturally conditioned images and expectations shape
how we think, act, and react to the world;
4. recognize that our behavior is shaped both by both situation-specific
and broader societal forces;
5. realize that people make socially conditioned choices regarding their
basic physical and psychological needs;
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
9 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Cultural Knowledge 201
Table 9.1 Examples of cultural knowledge
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
202 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
formulated a comprehensive agenda for teaching foreign languages that
included a systematic introduction to the culture(s) in which the L2 is
spoken. The resulting standards (Lafayette, 1996) outline a three-pronged
understanding of culture:
1. perspectives (e.g., meanings, values, attitudes – both historical and
current),
2. practices (e.g., “what to do when and where” – p. 46), and
3. products (e.g., books, music, food – what they are, and what perspec-
tives they reflect).
The objective for language instruction was to help learners understand the
relationship between these three components of culture (i.e., how practices
and products reflect a social group’s perspectives). Additionally, the stan-
dards advocated for including a comparison of learners’ L1 and L2 (i.e.,
metacognitive analysis), academic and professional communication, and
interactions in genuine L2-speaking communities (National Standards,
2006).
More recently, Spencer-Oatey (2012) described cultural knowledge as
consisting of:
1. basic underlying – mostly subconscious – assumptions that guide our
behavior (e.g., nature of human activity or relationships to time and space);
2. values that we are more aware of (e.g., funding public education or
not, espousing military intervention or preferring diplomatic
action); and
3. artefacts and creations that are visible and audible representations of
underlying values (although it is not always easy to figure out what
values they represent and how).
In somewhat of a contrast, Brijs, Bloemer, and Kasper (2011) suggest
organizing cultural knowledge in terms of country-images – the way we
subconsciously or consciously understand our own country and other
countries as follows:
These aspects overlap in some ways with Byram and Masuhara’s (2013)
conceptualization of knowledge as including “knowledge of self (and own
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
9 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Cultural Knowledge 203
society/social group) and of the other (and their society/social group)”
(p. 148):
• historical and contemporary relationships between one’s own and one’s
interlocutor’s countries;
• the means of achieving contact with interlocutors from another country
(at a distance or in proximity), of travel to and from, and the institu-
tions which facilitate contact or help resolve problems;
• the types of cause and process of misunderstanding between interlocu-
tors of different cultural origins;
• the national memory of one’s own country and how its events are
related to and seen from the perspective of one’s interlocutor’s country;
• the national memory of one’s interlocutor’s country and the perspective
on it from one’s own;
• the national definitions of geographical space in one’s own country and
how these are perceived from the perspective of other countries;
• the national definitions of geographical space in one’s interlocutor ‘s
country and the perspective on them from one’s own;
• the processes and institutions of socialization in one’s own and one’s
interlocutor’s country;
• social distinctions and their principal markers in one’s own country and
one’s interlocutor’s;
• institutions, and perceptions of them, which impinge on daily life
within one’s own and one’s interlocutor’s country and which conduct
and influence relationships between them;
• the processes of social interaction in one’s interlocutor’s country.
(p. 148)
With reframing Byram and Masuhara’s culture as a layered construct, as
this book has advocated, this list can serve as a roadmap for the body of
knowledge L2/Lx learners and users need to gain about new cultural
contexts.
A synthesis of these models offers an overview of cultural knowledge that
we build over time and which includes, but is not limited to, the following:
• myths: creation and history;
• stories: fairy tales, historical narratives;
• beliefs: religious, historical, folk stories;
• history: people, events (including reasons for events happening and
their outcome);
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
204 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
• politics: current, past, development, and progression, structure of
various layers of culture (nation vis-à-vis smaller communities and the
individual), governmental organization;
• geography: natural and social (e.g., natural resources and their impact
on society);
• art: visual arts (painting, sculpture, multimedia), music (national
songs, folksongs), literature, artists old and new;
• language: historical changes, proverbs, expressions, national language
policies, personal language choices;
• practices: daily habits (ranging from the supranational to the indivi-
dual, transportation, when to eat what, banking, relationship to time,
etc.), special occasions, customs, and rituals, personal/family stories and
histories.
These domains of knowledge guide our everyday lives as well as special
occasions. They inform what events we celebrate and how, as well as how
we conduct ourselves day-to-day. As these models also show, we navigate
a complex system of knowledge in order to understand and participate in
interactions or make regular as well as unique decisions every day. For
example, in 2018 retirees in Japan volunteered to clean up nuclear waste,1
exposing themselves to radiation in order to protect younger generations
from dangerous diseases. In this case, long-standing historical values of
serving a community larger than the self interacted with the specific
situation of the retirees, who were predominantly engineers, factory design
experts, and former power station workers, and are mostly above seventy
years of age. These broad societal codes (or ethos) interact with those we
develop at more personal levels, socialized by our families and smaller
communities or cultures we participate in. The interaction between dif-
ferent types of background knowledge guide our culturally contextualized
actions. As Figure 9.1 presents, facets of cultural knowledge align along two
axes: concrete versus abstract knowledge on the vertical axis, and the
personal/individual versus broader societal codes that guide our beliefs,
behaviors, or values on the horizontal axis.
As the preceding discussion demonstrated, cultural knowledge spans all
aspects of everyday life and unique events. We need to develop
a considerable amount of this knowledge to be able to participate in our
own and other cultures. As L2/Lx learners gain this knowledge, they face
two challenges. They must become aware of what they only know
1
www.bbc.com/news/world-asia-pacific-13598607
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
9 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Cultural Knowledge 205
concrete
– national celebrations and holidays (when, what, – personal and family celebrations (how, when, what to
and how to celebrate) celebrate)
– national/supranational institutions – language choice in family or small group settings (e.g.,
– language policy (e.g., official language[s]) at a community center)
– laws and regulations – personal preferences for behavior, clothing, fiscal
– available information sources decisions, etc.
– available health care – work-life balance
– educational choices (e.g., elementary schools, – personal relationships (e.g., how we maintain them)
middle or high schools of various kinds)
– available natural resources (e.g., food, geography)
– important works in the arts
– important events, past and present
– structure of governance (e.g., democracy, – personal codes of ethics (e.g., work ethic)
monarchy) – personality
– national history – orientation towards others
beliefs, myths – personal world view
– national/supranational notions of heroism (e.g., – personal beliefs
what it takes to earn a nation’s or supranational – relationship between national / supranational
organization’s respect) expectations and our personal attitudes towards them
– national/supranational relationships across the (e.g., whether we aspire to them or reject them)
globe
– reasons of considering certain people and events
important
abstract
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
206 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
from a wide range of everyday and scholarly backgrounds – such as
anthropologists, linguists, musicians, journalists, teachers, or doctors, just
to name a few possibilities – who study the patterns, behaviors, practices,
and products of a culture can also offer valuable insights, as long as they
base their views on observation and analysis (i.e., based on factual and valid
sources of information). Such observation and analysis can be facilitated by
developing culturally appropriate frameworks of knowledge.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
9 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Cultural Knowledge 207
need to be revised or created anew. To illustrate the way existing and new
schemata work, let us examine an interaction in depth and identify the
different types of cultural knowledge required to make sense of it. In series
1, episode 3 of the British television series The Good Karma Hospital,
Maggie Smart, one of the main characters, is walking down the street
with her husband, Paul (at 10:45–11:28 minutes).
maggie: Oh, look, look. ((coughs)) . . . Can I have two please? ((holds up index and
middle finger of right hand, palm facing towards a street vendor selling drinks))
paul: You don’t even know what that is.
maggie: Oh, it’s traditional. Something called bhang. ((they both drink from small
plastic cups))
paul: It smells like disinfectant! What’s in it?
seller: Better not to smell, sir. Just . . . ((pinches nose)) . . . drink.
maggie: Come on. One, two . . .
paul: If I go blind, I’ll come looking for you.
maggie: . . . three! Oooh! ((both she and Paul cough and bend over))
seller: ((laughs)) For you!
maggie: ((laughing)) Pay the man!
paul: Here you go, mate.
seller: ((puts hands together, touching his forehead and bows slightly))
maggie: Thank you. That was strong. ((laughs))
In order to understand this polylog, we need linguistic, cultural, and
situational knowledge. In linguistic terms, the words are easy enough to
understand for individuals who know basic English, since words like pay,
hands, or know are high-frequency lexical items.2 The least frequent lexical
item, disinfectant, can be guessed as something unpleasant from the con-
text, even if a viewer cannot glean the specific meaning of the word.
Examining the conversation with a slightly broader lens, we can decipher
its overall meaning if we have a script of purchasing something to drink
from a street vendor, either by having participated in or heard of such an
exchange. This script is not unique to the local culture, but instead shared
across various cultures. It entails the buyers expressing what they would
like to purchase, the seller providing the item(s), the buyers paying the
seller and consuming what they had ordered.
In addition to the buying script, we need to access or learn several frames
and scripts pertaining to Indian cultures in order to understand the
interaction more fully. As the videoclip shows, the community is celebrat-
ing holi, where people celebrate the end of winter by throwing colorful
2
Frequency numbers 1236, 174, 47, respectively, according to www.wordandphrase.info/frequencyList
.asp
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
208 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
powder into the air, dance in the streets, wear vibrantly colored attire, and
play drums.3 The celebrations include thandai, a traditional fruit-based
drink made with bhang4 (a compound ground from cannabis leaves). The
community of fans who watch this television series can appreciate this
scene even more completely, because they are aware of several locally
situated frames as well. This is a moment of character development, as
a timid woman from England lets go of her inhibitions and embraces life.
This festival is of great personal significance for Maggie, because shortly
before this scene she had been diagnosed with cancer and decided to live
her remaining life to the fullest.
Our – correct, incorrect, or incomplete – knowledge of the cultural
context that surrounds an interaction informs all aspects of communica-
tion: vocabulary, grammar, pragmatics, paralinguistics, and nonverbal
communication. In fact, much of the information needed for comprehen-
sion is found in shared contextual knowledge and not in the utterances
themselves (Labov & Fanshel, 1977). What social groups are we a part of?
Which of our cultural selves are we enacting in any given conversation?
How much respect do we express to whom? Answers to questions such as
these are all part of our contextual knowledge even before we begin
speaking, signing, or writing.
When implemented accurately, schemata can speed up our lives and
communication. In such situations, our inferences are usually correct, and
we feel confident in drawing them. However, difficulties may arise when
participants hold different assumptions, come from different cultural or
ethnic groups, generations, or gender identities. The more group affiliations
are dissimilar (or at least the group affiliations that are salient to a particular
interaction), the more challenging inferencing becomes. Unfortunately, there
are also situations where our underlying interpretive frameworks mislead us.
We might not have an accurate or complete knowledge of the information we
need, and our communication falters or falls apart completely. By selecting
the sources we use to build our cultural knowledge carefully, we can improve
our chances for more successful intercultural communication.
3
http://time.com/5182369/what-is-holi-2018/
4
www.bbc.com/travel/story/20170307-the-intoxicating-drug-of-an-indian-god
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
9 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Cultural Knowledge 209
member
ooks ship
s, b in
ovie soc
,m ial
re ,p
tu ol
ra it
Broad societal
te
ica
, li
l&
influences
ion
su
vis
pra
ele
nat
rs, t
iona
newspape
l organizations
Mid-level
community Personal
contextual contextual
factors factors
pa
es
re
at
nt
m
s
,e
lde am
rs, te
fam tes,
ily, ma
mem lass
bers & traditions, c
Figure 9.2 Interplay of broader societal and local, personal cultural influences
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
210 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
of men and women helped propel groups of women all over the country, but
especially in large urban centers, to push for women’s right to vote, so that
they would have representation in governance as well. These groups formed
a mid-level layer, populated mostly by well-to-do women who shared the
main goal – “to achieve the right to vote for women through constitutional,
peaceful means”5 – while being willing to risk arrest. Their numbers and
vocal approach allowed the issue to be sustained and eventually to be taken
seriously. However, their movement may have taken much longer, were it
not for contributions made by some specific individuals. Women like
Millicent Garrett Fawcett,6 from a wealthy and politically progressive family,
who published regularly to convince others of their cause, or Emmeline and
Christabel Pankhurst, among other leading figures of the women’s suffrage
movement, were willing to speak out consistently for decades, often at great
personal peril. Additionally, Emmeline Pankhurst’s husband, Richard
Pankhurst, was a lawyer who wholeheartedly supported the women’s suffrage
movement and proposed two laws (the Married Women’s Property Acts of
1870 and 1882), which allowed women to keep their property acquired before
and after marriage.7 Thus, the passage of the Equal Franchise Act of 1928,
which finally gave women the right to vote,8 emerged out of the interrela-
tionship of societal, mid-level, and personal factors.
In addition to such cultural impact on our decision-making, beliefs, and
actions, we also utilize various sources of knowledge, and it is our respon-
sibility to determine how these might shape our understanding of people,
places, events, or practices. In order to fulfill this responsibility, we need to
select these sources carefully and with a critical eye.
5
www.parliament.uk/about/living-heritage/transformingsociety/electionsvoting/womenvote/over
view/earlysuffragist/
6
www.britannica.com/biography/Millicent-Fawcett
7
www.bbc.co.uk/history/historic_figures/pankhurst_emmeline.shtml
8
www.parliament.uk/about/living-heritage/transformingsociety/electionsvoting/womenvote/over
view/thevote/
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
9 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Cultural Knowledge 211
process. Such learning does not entail reflection or analysis, and there is
little discussion involved about the values underscoring expected beha-
viors. Explicit learning, conversely, emerges as a result of asking questions
about why and how things happen, examining, analyzing, and interpreting
evidence, reflecting on assumptions, and being willing to revise or refine
our understanding of the world when necessary and appropriate.
Identifying reliable sources of information is part of explicit learning, as
it requires reflection and conscious choice.
The sources we draw on for information depend on a number of
factors. What is it that we are learning about? A coach can explain the
upcoming team schedule, for example, but official government sources
are required for finding out how to apply for a work permit overseas.
A scientific organization’s work is best suited to understand recent
epidemics, while a spiritual leader may be the right person to ask
questions about one’s beliefs. Importantly, the availability of informa-
tion also influences the outcome of our quest. In many parts of the
world, freedom of the press is protected, representing multiple voices
and perspectives in print and audio-visual media, and individuals have
a right to express their ideas freely as well (e.g., Norway, Australia, South
Africa). In other places, however, people face significant limitations,
because the government controls access to information and restricts
freedom of speech (e.g., China, Iran, North Korea). When multiple
sources of information are available, we should examine several of
them and seek reliable and complete knowledge.
With the advent of the Internet, there has been an explosion of
available information online over the past decade. Although many
sources are well researched and readily accessible across the globe
(e.g., health information from the Mayo Clinic is a resource now
available to nurses in training in Africa), not all of them are equally
trustworthy, and learners need critical analytic skills to evaluate the
material they read, in order to ensure that our information is as
reliable and accurate as possible:9
1. Use multiple sources of information:
• Does the source present multiple perspectives?
• What are different views regarding the same issue?
9
https://guides.library.ucsc.edu/writing/evaluate
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
212 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
2. Use reliable sources of information (be aware of bias):
• Do the authors use factual information themselves and provide
their sources?
• Do they include links to other reliable sources that offer diverse
perspectives?
• Are the authors (e.g., a government agency or university) reliable?
• What is the source domain: .com, .org., .edu? Which are you likely
to trust?
4. Seek out timely information that is current and up to date.
5. Identify the purpose of the source: Does it offer opinion or seek to
persuade (e.g., sell you something, convince you of a stance) or does it
present factual information?
6. Speak with trusted sources to get multiple viewpoints to try to make
sense of cultural perspectives, practices, and products.
7. Recognize and remain cognizant of the pervasive influence of your
own cultural views on how you understand another culture; resist
projecting your own frames of reference onto another culture and
individual members of that culture (Galloway, 1992).
Observing these guidelines may be tedious at first, but over time we learn
which sources are more or less trustworthy. For more rigorous assignments,
such as scholarly papers, the review should be even more thorough. In
response to the explosion of information and misinformation available
online, many universities have sought ways to identify reliable sources,
including Cornell University (http://guides.library.cornell.edu/criticallya
nalyzing) and the University of California Berkeley (http://guides
.lib.berkeley.edu/evaluating-resources). With the support of reliable
sources, the knowledge learners develop will serve them well in their own
cultural contexts, not just when they explore new ones.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
9 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Cultural Knowledge 213
At the same time, access to sometimes overwhelming sources of informa-
tion, some unreliable, has made culture teaching more challenging.
Therefore, instruction must include safeguards for learning accurate infor-
mation about other cultures. We also must recognize that not all teachers
and students have the same access to technology, and pedagogical planning
needs to take specific learning and teaching contexts into consideration.
Another challenge of teaching cultural knowledge is a lack of awareness
by many learners about their own cultural practices, attitudes, and per-
spectives. As Kumaravadivelu (2003) observed, such limitations likely
impact what teachers and their learners can achieve in the classroom in
terms of critical cultural understanding. In order to become intercultural
learners, however, what was subconscious before must become conscious.
Additionally, we tend to identify closely with our values and habits, so
potential challenges may be perceived as threatening. Therefore, teaching
culture requires instructors to develop the ability to guide difficult con-
versations meaningfully but sensitively, because we want to encourage
learners to explore their beliefs and practices and adapt them to new
cultural contexts. Ideally, the process of developing cultural knowledge
aims to understand another social group from an emic point of view (i.e.,
from the standpoint of members of that culture), rather than an etic or
outsider perspective. Retaining an etic stance raises the possibility of view-
ing another culture only as differences, in a sensationalistic way, instead of
examining similarities as well as differences, while also recognizing varia-
tion within culture(s) and across individuals.
Another important consideration for teaching culture is that it is a long-
term process. We cannot teach everything right from the beginning.
Instead, culture should be incorporated into the L2 curriculum in stages,
which allows us to teach it in the L2/Lx (since many instructors feel
uncomfortable with using the L1 in the classroom, if that is shared
among the students). Facts and figures, key events in history and how
they developed, important geographical aspects of a culture and their
bearing on the economy or history, everyday issues like shopping, and
other factual details are readily accessible for students in their first two years
of L2/Lx learning. Major historical, literary, and artistic movements and
their impact on modern society can be introduced in the second year and
beyond. At this stage, learners should also gain more familiarity with
everyday needs, such as renting an apartment, registering for university
courses, or setting up a bank account, to help them prepare for living
abroad. More complex ideas about belief systems, society, politics, the arts,
as well as historical and sociopolitical issues are easier to address in years 3
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
214 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
and 4, when learners’ L2/Lx proficiency is advanced enough to discuss
difficult cultural concepts – which can be sensitive and require extensive
research – with sufficient nuance and depth.
Summary
This section lays out existing and potential further connections between
cultural knowledge and intercultural communication, connecting this
chapter to the concepts and guidelines presented in Chapters 1 and 2.
Specifically, that the discussion yields several suggestions for an intercul-
tural communication-oriented pedagogy.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
9 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Cultural Knowledge 215
(cont.)
2. Accept that as an instructor you are not responsible for knowing everything about the
cultures where the L2/Lx are spoken; take advantage of external sources of information
(guest speakers, telecollaborative partners, documents, multimedia sources, etc.).
3. Accept also that you are not the sole representative of the culture in which the L2/Lx is
spoken; you are one multifaceted representative of that culture, with a wealth of
information, but cannot speak for the entire diverse community.
4. When possible, include nonnative members of the culture; their experiences are
uniquely relevant for your students, who may wish to transition to the new culture,
whether for work or studying or other reasons.
5. Include analytic tasks for students to learn new schemata that are relevant for their new
cultural environment, since they can speed up and facilitate interactions and reduce
miscommunication in unfamiliar contexts.
6. Utilize longer authentic materials – particularly movies, documentaries, or TV shows –
that depict important social, historical, and literary movements from multiple
perspectives. As a complement, include materials that portray everyday social realities
(e.g., how work is viewed and organized, how families and friends interact).
ACTIVITY SET 1
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, learners will develop the following:
1. An awareness of various types of cultural knowledge needed to complete
four to five everyday tasks they complete in their own lives.
2. Skills necessary to interview members of the L2/Lx community regard-
ing their daily routines (the same or similar types of tasks that the
learners identify in their own lives).
3. The language knowledge to ask and answer questions regarding daily
routines in the L2/Lx.
Analytic Approach
Ethnography of communication and multimodal analysis.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
216 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Analysis
In this activity, learners first develop an awareness of aspects of their daily routine
that they had completed mostly subconsciously beforehand. Simultaneously,
they improve their ability to observe, articulate what they see and do, and reflect
on cultural influences that shape their practices (perhaps also perspectives,
depending on the tasks they choose, such as a university or sports event).
Learners also improve their interviewing skills; the ability to ask questions is
essential for learning about other cultures. If several members of the L2/Lx
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
9 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Cultural Knowledge 217
culture are interviewed, learners may begin to note how social variables (e.g., age,
sex, class) impact language use and behavior, and what role, if any, individual
preferences play. Would a grandparent perform the same tasks, in the same way,
as college students? Do all college students complete the same tasks the same
way? Recognizing patterns of similarities and differences can help learners focus
on what is shared across a cultural group and what are individual preferences,
thus limiting overgeneralizations. Importantly, the interview subjects do not
have to be native speakers of the L2/Lx; a returning student from a study-abroad
program can provide useful information for learners interested in similar
opportunities, and insider (emic) knowledge is not the sole purview of ‘native’
members of a culture.
ACTIVITY SET 2
Learning Objectives
By the end of this activity, learners will learn the following:
1. Information about two or three topics related to current events in depth
(e.g., historical development of an issue).
2. How to conduct research pertaining to the cultural background of
current events.
3. How to evaluate the reliability of information sources in the L2/Lx
context.
Analytic Approach
Multimodal analysis, interactional sociolinguistics, and ethnography of
communication.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
218 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Task
Weekly, over the course of the term, learners – either individually or in
small groups – research one or more current events and analyze the
background knowledge in detail: both historical and current issues that
pertain to this event, the impact on people’s lives (at various levels of
culture, from the personal to the broader societal level). They seek answers
to the questions listed in pre-task (3) or similar questions. Learners present
their findings to their peers in the L2/Lx.
Post-Task
As a possible post-task assignment, learners could analyze the same issue
presented in two or three news sources, comparing and contrasting the way
it is discussed (e.g., symbolism, language choice), including any author bias
and the impact of the intended audience.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
9 Intercultural Communication: Teaching Cultural Knowledge 219
Analysis
This activity simultaneously builds learners’ contextual/background
knowledge of current events and their ability to conduct research by
examining evidence from diverse, reliable sources. By delving into
sociopolitical, socioeconomic, or sociocultural aspects of the L2/Lx culture,
learners develop an appreciation of how culturally conditioned images and
expectations shape how members of the culture think, act, and react to the
world. Learners should also (begin to) understand (or investigate) several
broader societal variables that shape people’s behavior and how different
social groups might react differently to similar events. If learners continue
their language studies into the third or fourth year at university, they are
likely to have a significant interest in participating in a culture where the L2/
Lx is spoken. Therefore, in-depth knowledge is required, as well as the
ability to gain that in-depth knowledge through systematic research,
investigation, and analysis. Research projects help learners widen and
deepen their knowledge of relevant cultural schemata, expanding networks
of knowledge they can access quickly and reliably.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
220 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
4. Think about one of your typical days and reflect on the types of
knowledge that enable you to function in the different cultural groups
to which you belong. Which of Byram and Masuhara’s domains of
knowledge do you utilize every day and which ones do you need less
frequently? How did you develop this knowledge? How much of this
knowledge are you in command of regarding the culture(s) where your
L2/Lx is spoken?
5. Interview someone who is a relative newcomer to your own culture
(e.g., someone who has lived there for 1–3 years). What cultural
knowledge had they mastered prior to their arrival, how accurate and
useful was that knowledge, and what did they need to learn after they
arrived? How could you incorporate their insights into your own
pedagogical practices?
6. Thinking of beginning L2/Lx learners, design two activities that teaches
students (a) an everyday practice and (b) a special event, famous person,
or topic of cultural significance (i.e., small-c and big-c culture). The
task should encourage learners to consider multiple perspectives of an
issue (e.g., the history of one-child versus two-children policy in
China).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:28, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.010
chapter 10
Chapter Overview
Assessing intercultural communicative competence can be challenging for
a number of reasons. First, there is little consensus regarding the precise
definitions of subcomponents of intercultural competence, making it
difficult to identify specific constructs to assess. Second, the “overall
external outcome of intercultural competence is defined as effective and
appropriate behavior and communication in intercultural situations”
(Deardorff, 2011, p. 66; emphasis original), but the “same behavior or
skill may be perceived as competent in one context but not another or
[by] one perceiver but not another, and thus no particular skill or ability is
likely to ever be universally ‘competent’” (Spitzberg & Changnon, 2009,
p. 6). Third, in a related vein, some aspects of intercultural communicative
competence might not be suitable for classroom assessment, which is why
this chapter is located at the end of the pedagogical section of the book and
does not follow aspects of the intercultural journey (Part III). This issue is
addressed in more detail towards the end of this chapter.
In spite of these challenges, for personal and institutional purposes,
assessing learners’ progress in intercultural communicative competence
remains an important educational objective. Thus, this chapter reviews
several theoretical concepts in L2/Lx assessment, relating them to inter-
cultural communicative competence and illustrating them with three case
studies.
221
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
222 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
assigning grades or determining qualifications). As a complement, the
authors suggest two additional conceptualizations of assessment: “assess-
ment for learning and as learning” (p. 124), both of which are better
addressed with ‘alternative’ forms of assessment, such as portfolios, per-
formance, peer- and self-assessments, and observations or tasks. Each focus
may be appropriate, depending on the purpose of specific assessments.
Before selecting appropriate forms of assessment, however, a number of
issues must be considered: validity, reliability, authenticity, practicality,
and washback effects. Validity pertains to the question of whether an
assessment actually measures the construct it aims to measure, whereas
reliability ensures that a measurement tool can elicit the same result on
repeated administrations or if two versions of a test elicit the same results
(Bachman & Damböck, 2018). Authenticity reflects language that is as
natural as possible, in meaningful contexts, adhering to real-world com-
munication, while practicality addresses questions such as how feasible it is
to develop, administer, and interpret the results of an assessment (e.g.,
large-scale assessment may preclude extensive portfolio evaluation).
Finally, washback effect refers to the effect an assessment has on pedagogy
and learning (Brown & Abeywickrama, 2010).
Different types of assessment support these constructs to varying
degrees. Psychometric measurement tools (e.g., the Test of English as a
Foreign Language) have high reliability and validity, are practical for their
purposes, but have negative washback effects (if students study for the test
at the exclusion of more real-world language), and rate low on authenticity.
These types of tests represent the majority of assessments of learning. In
contrast, alternative assessment methods may score lower on reliability
(although carefully designed rubrics can improve consistent evaluation)
and may be time-consuming (i.e., are less practical), but hew closer to real-
world communication and tasks. As a result, their washback effect tends to
be positive, in that they also promote real-world language use in prepara-
tion for the test and demonstrate to learners that such language use is
valued. Moreover, the information learners receive can serve as assessment
for and as learning, since learners can use the feedback they receive from
their peers or instructors to advance their intercultural communicative
competence. Consequently, Liddicoat and Scarino’s (2013) argument that
“Assessing language learning within an intercultural perspective is more
fruitfully conceptualized within an alternate assessment paradigm” (p. 125)
seems convincing. Alternative forms of assessment are also more in line
with sociocognitive views of L2/Lx learning, as they measure not just
cognitive, but also social aspects of learners’ progress. Nonetheless, a
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
10 Assessing Intercultural Communication 223
variety of assessment tools can be implemented to measure intercultural
communicative competence, and the most important consideration is to
ensure that the assessment matches the information educators and learners
would like to evaluate.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
224 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
constructs. Additionally, each construct needs to be carefully aligned with
an appropriate measurement tool that can elicit the information we wish to
understand (e.g., Has the student learned to recognize varieties of a
language pertaining to the lexicon?).
Liddicoat and Scarino’s (2013) four-step model for assessing intercul-
tural communication offers a useful framework for ensuring alignment
between the construct and assessment tools:
1. conceptualizing: identifying the construct we wish to assess, and what
is the best way of getting that information;
2. eliciting: gathering evidence of learning and knowledge;
3. judging: identifying the criteria we use for evaluating the evidence;
4. validating: explaining and ensuring the quality of the evaluation
process.
The three case studies that follow apply this process to specific assessment
situations in the L2/Lx classroom. It is important to keep in mind, though, that
assessment should not be a one-time affair; instead, “individuals [should] be
given opportunities to reflect on and assess the development of their own
intercultural competence over time … ” and assessment should be iterative,
integrated throughout the curriculum (Deardorff, 2011, p. 68; emphasis mine).
Each case study includes the concepts, the elicitation technique, and the
evaluation criteria; the issue of validity is addressed after the third case study.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
10 Assessing Intercultural Communication 225
• identifying six lexical items the learners already know, their L1
equivalents, and the source of their knowledge (e.g., learned this
word in class, guessed its meaning from the context, or recognized
it, because it is a cognate with a language they already know) (9
points – half for each word, half for each correct L1 equivalent, and
half for each label).
2. Navigating the cafeteria website:
• finding the location of each cafeteria by clicking on the link
‘Mensen’ and using GoogleMaps (they can list one major attrac-
tion near each cafeteria for better orientation) (4 points – 1 for each
correct address);
• exploring hours of operation (by clicking on each of the four
cafeteria links) (4 points – 1 for each correct address);
• identifying two menu choices that they might like and learning
their prices, using the same cafeteria links (4 points – 1 for each
menu choice, 1 for each correct price).
3. Reflecting on differences and similarities with learners’ own university
meal opportunities:
• commenting on meal choices (e.g., values reflected in price or focus
on natural foods).
Rubric:
• 4 points for including both a similarity and a difference that
demonstrate an awareness of shifting ‘between cultures’
using observation and evidence (e.g., both my university and
Göttingen seem to value healthy food choices; but in Göttingen,
the food is more affordable, making healthy choices more
accessible);
• 3 points for including both a similarity and a difference that
demonstrate some awareness of cultural patterns, using some evi-
dence (e.g., both universities have many options to choose from; in
Göttingen, the meals seem to be pre-determined);
• 2 points if only a similarity or difference is provided, or if both are
provided but the answers reflect superficial differences and lack
critical thinking (e.g., both universities serve chicken; difference: in
German the word for salad is Salat);
• 1 point if only a similarity or difference is provided and the answer
reflects superficial differences (e.g., both universities serve chicken);
• 0 points for missing or irrelevant responses.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
226 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
4. Reflecting on learners’ experience as intercultural interpreters (thus
communicators):
• identifying surprising and unexpected issues that arose while
exploring the cafeteria websites, and new objectives for interacting
with authentic materials.
Rubric:
• 2 points for a thoughtful response that identifies a challenge (and
how the learner overcame it) or a discovery made during this
activity (e.g., I was able to use my general knowledge about how
menus are organized or I could recognize a number of words that
helped me make sense of the menus or I found navigating the website
difficult at first, but just decided to click around and see what would
happen);
• 1 point for a response that reflects minimal engagement with the
activity or the website (e.g., it was interesting to visit a German-
language website);
• 0 points for a missing or irrelevant response.
Learners’ answers to sections (1) and (2) are assigned points for correct
answers, much like in traditional assessment. In section (3), responses will
vary and may or may not align with the interpretation of the instructor,
since they reflect personal experience and values. Therefore, a rubric that
emphasizes evidence-based analysis is used, including criteria such as
accurate information drawn from the website (appropriate at the novice
level), a comparison of both similarities and differences with learners’ own
experiences, and a reflective analysis of the available information. Finally,
section (4) can be evaluated based on learners’ ability to provide at least one
observation they made during their explorations that can help them
become better and more aware participants in the process of developing
intercultural interpretive (receptive) competence, offering a meta-analysis
and demonstrating awareness of the discovery process itself (cf. Byram,
1997).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
10 Assessing Intercultural Communication 227
pragmatic, and paralinguistic information (i.e., elicitation), they can
perform skits with an instructor, another student, another Arabic-
speaking volunteer who acts as the taxi driver, or online using gaming
or artificial intelligence. This activity can be conducted as assessment
of, as, or for learning, depending on the students’ or institutional
needs. Learners would have learned about and participated in similar
interactive tasks. The interaction should include the following
components:
• appropriate greeting;
• stating the destination;
• asking about the cost of the ride, whether credit cards are accepted or if
the driver can provide change for a larger bill;
• participating in basic interaction (phatic communication, such as Are
you here for vacation, This is a nice time of the year to be in Cairo or Is this
your first visit to Egypt?);
• paying and tipping;
• leave-taking;
• self-reflection by the learner (either in dialog with the instructor/inter-
actant or as a journal entry).
Modern Standard Arabic (MSA) is the typical variety that L2/Lx
learners study in the classroom context. In real-world interactions in
Cairo, however, students will encounter other varieties, especially the
colloquial style. Learners will have to demonstrate some flexibility in
adjusting their receptive language skills and work with their interac-
tants to navigate genuine exchanges. They will have to be able
to recognize and respond to truncated questions, such as where to
instead of the more likely classroom form of where would you like a
ride today? At the same time, their interactants will likely accommo-
date some of their language needs (e.g., taxi drivers will be quite used
to driving tourists and want their customers to be content). The
assessment should reflect real-world interactions as much as possible,
including possible language accommodations. For example, students
should be allowed to use their smart phones to point out their
destination, in addition to stating the address, and there can be
multiple repetitions of questions and answers during the assessment
event. Learners’ performance can be evaluated with the holistic rubric
in Table 10.1.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
228 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Table 10.1 Holistic rubric assessing learners’ locally situated, plausible (i.e.,
semi-authentic) interaction
9–10 points • Recognizes colloquial expressions (even if only responds using MSA).
• Maintains each section of the exchange with situationally appropriate
responses (e.g., greetings, terms of address, leave-taking, politeness
formulae).
• Participates in the interaction actively by initiating some questions.
• Provides responses without significant lexical or grammatical errors
that prevent comprehension (occasional errors are acceptable, as is
real-world grammar usage).
• Includes culturally appropriate content (e.g., currency, locations).
• Upon reflection, is aware of different language variety used, recog-
nizes the need for adjustment in listening skills and expresses curi-
osity regarding this new experience.
7–8 points • Recognizes some colloquial expressions (even if only responds
using MSA).
• Maintains most sections of the exchange; responses are generally
situationally appropriate (e.g., greetings, terms of address, leave-tak-
ing, politeness formulae).
• Participates in the interaction by initiating one or two questions.
• Provides responses without significant lexical or grammatical errors
that prevent comprehension (occasional errors are acceptable, as is
real-world grammar usage).
• Includes culturally appropriate content (e.g., currency, locations).
• Upon reflection, is aware of different language variety used, recog-
nizes the need for adjustment in listening skills.
5–6 points • Has difficulty recognizing colloquial expressions.
• Maintains half of the exchange with situationally somewhat appro-
priate responses (e.g., greetings, terms of address, leave-taking,
politeness formulae).
• Participates in the interaction by asking one question.
• Provides responses with some systematic lexical or grammatical errors
that may interfere with comprehension.
• Includes some culturally appropriate content (e.g., currency, locations).
• Upon reflection, is aware of different language variety used but does
not recognize the need for adjustment in listening skills.
3–4 points • Has difficulty recognizing colloquial expressions.
• Partially maintains the exchange with responses that are not consis-
tentlysituationally appropriate (e.g., greetings, terms of address).
• Does not successfully initiate questions.
• Provides responses with partially correct lexicon and grammar that
makes comprehension difficult.
• Content is not entirely culturally appropriate (e.g., currency, locations).
• Upon reflection, may not be aware of different language variety used,
does not recognize the need for adjustment in listening skills, and
may expresses frustration at having to interact with someone using an
unfamiliar communication style.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
10 Assessing Intercultural Communication 229
Table 10.1 (cont.)
Note: circle all criteria met; the row in which most responses are circled should be the grade
assigned
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
230 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
the written report that reviews the historical context of the debate, statis-
tical data and other evidence to support the learners’ argument, a
PowerPoint presentation (e.g., with pie charts, graphs, collages), or a
Table 10.2 Analytic rubric assessing learners’ oral and written presentation on
culturally relevant topic
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
10 Assessing Intercultural Communication 231
Table 10.2 (cont.)
4
background information, presented clearly.
3 Some organization evident in the multimodal presentation; mostly
effective use of background information, although the presentation
may be somewhat unclear.
2 Limited organization, modality, use of background information; the
presentation is somewhat difficult to follow.
1 Little or no evident organization, not multimodal, limited, or incorrect
use of background information; the presentation is difficult to follow.
3 Asks complex questions about a topic of current cultural interest; is able
to answer these questions from multiple perspectives; can suspend
Openness and curiosity*
* These descriptors are adapted from the value rubrics set forth by the American
Association of Colleges and Universities (www.aacu.org/value/rubrics)
video representing impact statements, and so on, with a final oral pre-
sentation in class. Each stage should provide learners with feedback from
the instructor and their peers. Learners’ performance can be evaluated with
the analytic rubric in Table 10.2.
Before each assessment, the evaluation criteria should be shared with
students, so that they have a clearer understanding of what is expected of
them. This can enhance the impact of assessment for and as learning. The
format of assessment materials should also reflect classroom pedagogical
practices, in order to make the link between teaching and testing more
closely linked, making both – hopefully – more authentic in the process,
creating a positive washback effect.
At the other end of the process, after evaluating learners’ performance (not
only in these case studies, but also as a general practice), the instructor –
ideally, with input from the learners – should collect information regarding
the effectiveness of the assessment process for fostering intercultural
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
232 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
communicative competence, as part of validation (Liddicoat & Scarino,
2013). This reflection should include not only the assessment tools, but
also the evaluation criteria, to determine whether they were clear and
encouraged learners to develop intercultural communicative competence.
These reflections can help identify what can and cannot be addressed with
the available assessment tools.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
10 Assessing Intercultural Communication 233
and geographic locations. They often explain common, everyday phrases
(e.g., több is veszett Mohácsnál [‘more was lost at Mohách’] in Hungarian
refers to a devastating loss Hungary suffered in 1526, but now means
roughly the equivalent of ‘no point crying over spilled milk’). These facts
and figures can easily be assessed with traditional assessment tools as well.
The crucial thing to remember is not to equate cultural background
knowledge only with facts and figures, but rather to assess it more fully
(see Chapter 9), evaluating cultural understanding from multiple perspec-
tives, including intracultural variation. This can be achieved creatively,
using portfolios, presentations, video projects, among other forms of
alternative assessment that show the process of learning, the ability to
interpret information and develop intercultural perspectives. The e-port-
folio described in the third case study above is an assessment that includes
cultural background knowledge, both as an assessment of and for learning.
Alternative assessment can also test skills of discovery, the ability to shift
our interaction to accommodate others, the skills to compare and under-
stand source materials, the ability to interpret ideas in context, or the
ability to reflect, using metalanguage skills (Kramsch, 2009).
However, there are aspects of intercultural communicative competence
that perhaps should not be included in classroom assessment at all, such as
attitude, identity, or the personal journey, which is why this chapter is
positioned here instead of at the end of the book. Measuring attitude, for
example, is problematic. Although valid survey instruments are available to
test learners’ attitudes within the intercultural communicative context (cf.
The University of Michigan at www.crlt.umich.edu/interculturalcompe
tence or the Cross-Cultural Adaptability Inventory at http://ccaiassess.co
m/index.html), how would you measure development over one ten-week
term? What if someone already has positive attitudes to begin with and
consequently does not show any evidence of growth? Additionally, attitude
is relational (Abrams, 2020) and varies across contexts; a person can have an
overall positive attitude towards other cultures and yet struggle with a
particular interaction or interactant.
Similarly, it is difficult and even undesirable to set learning objectives for
assessing identity. First, this process might require learners to share aspects
of their identity that are private (e.g., if I can relate to another person due to
shared religious affiliations, would I need to share that with my instruc-
tor?). As Borghetti (2017) rightly cautions, there are ethical considerations
for assessing intercultural communicative competence, whose “conse-
quence for individuals [is] that there is a need to make even personal and
private preferences visible and explicit” (p. 1404). Second, being able to
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
234 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
participate in another culture does not necessarily mean that we have to
adopt aspects of identity from it (e.g., being able to work in a country
where women’s roles are more limited is possible, even though I do not
adopt a female identity that adheres or ascribes to those cultural values or
practices). As Liddicoat and Scarino (2013) note, an intercultural existence
entails “in essence, moving between languages and cultures” (p. 137), but
this movement is situational and therefore near impossible to measure in
the L2/Lx classroom setting. The personal journey (see Chapter 11), as the
name indicates, is personal. Progression is not linear in the adaptation
process, and many individuals do not experience significant growth (result-
ing from the stress-adaptation cycle) until they are living in a new cultural
context. Also, personality traits may influence the paths people take to
become intercultural beings, and, unwittingly, education may end up
penalizing learners “for not having particular desired personality traits,
such as ‘interest in cultures’ or ‘positive self-image,’ which are traits which
have been identified as characteristic of the effective intercultural person”
(Secru, 2004, p. 78).
Nonetheless, these constructs may be measured, but need to be assessed
long term. Having students compile portfolios from the beginning of their
L2/Lx learning endeavors until they are finishing their language studies two
or four years later is a better frame of reference to reveal growth in these
three areas, because it recognizes that “developing an intercultural perspec-
tive is a long-term process of cumulative experiences and reflection”
(Liddicoat & Scarino, 2013, p. 132). During this process, instructors need
to be cognizant of their own values, beliefs, perspectives, and practices, so
that any short- or long-term assessment is sufficiently objective.
Summary
Developing intercultural communicative competence is a complex process.
Some of its components can be assessed using traditional assessment types,
such as lexical, grammatical or cultural knowledge, skills of discovery and
interpreting, just to name a few. More beneficial, however, would be
alternate forms of assessment, including skits, creative writing, portfolios
(on- or offline), performance assessments, or formal presentations that are
better suited to glean insights about learners’ ability to implement various
aspects of their intercultural communicative competence – entailing com-
ponents covered in Chapters 4–9 – in practice. Yet other components
pertaining to personal motivation or attitude, personality traits or identity
do not lend themselves well to assessment.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
10 Assessing Intercultural Communication 235
Thus, an important reconsideration in assessment vis-à-vis intercultural
communicative competence requires a reconceptualization of how we use
assessment in the first place. Given the fact that intercultural communica-
tion is even more importantly interpersonal, and what is ‘appropriate’ is
determined in a dynamic way within the local, immediate context, in the
classroom setting, the emphasis should be on assessment as and for learning
(Liddicoat & Scarino, 2013). Borghetti (2017) describes this issue in even
starker terms:
[W]hile it remains true that leaving assessment out can entail both the risk
that IC [intercultural competence] be considered less significant than other
educational goals, and that students not be ready to be assessed in their
intercultural competence, one may also argue for the importance of not
assessing IC: leaving IC assessment out can convey a crucial, indirect
message to students, namely that not everything must be proved and
assessed to be valuable; and this may well represent a new, challenging
educational aim in the present ‘assessment era’ (Broadfoot and Black,
2004). (Borghetti, 2017, p. 1632)
A majority of our learners come to our language classes because they have
a deep interest in learning another language and are fascinated by the
cultures in which that language is spoken. Although assessment can ensure
that we take seriously what is taught in class – and, conversely, what is not
assessed may not be valued as highly – in this case, pedagogy can lead the way
and prioritize what is our actual learning objective, namely, helping learners
develop intercultural communicative competence. We can trust that the
subject matter is sufficiently useful in real-world contexts to remain of great
interest to learners and educators alike, without overemphasizing assessment.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
236 Part II Pedagogical Implementation
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:02, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.011
chapter 11
Cultural Transitions
Chapter Overview
The history of the world is a tapestry of people on the move. For thousands
of years, war, conquest, economic demands, colonization, natural disasters,
or the urge to discover unknown regions has led to migration. This process
continues today, with approximately “244 million international migrants
in the world in 2015, which equates to 3.3 per cent of the global population”
(McAuliffe & Ruhs, 2018, p. 2). The International Organization for
Migration (IOM) in Switzerland defines a migrant as
any person who is moving or has moved across an international border or
within a State away from his/her habitual place of residence, regardless of (1)
the person’s legal status; (2) whether the movement is voluntary or invo-
luntary; (3) what the causes for the movement are; or (4) what the length of
the stay is.1
While the term migrant sometimes carries negative connotations, it is
used in this chapter as a neutral label, in accordance with IOM’s definition,
to depict a wide range of complex experiences. It includes people whose
journeys are brief and those who must find permanent new homes; those
who are welcomed by the host-communities and those who encounter
serious challenges. As appropriate, other terms are introduced to illustrate
the diverse paths that cultural journeys take; however, in spite of their
diversity, these journeys also share common characteristics: the need to
recognize and adapt to new situations, people, beliefs, practices, and ways
of communicating. To understand the diversity of personal journeys, this
chapter examines the process of cultural transitions, the change from one’s
old self to an intercultural one.
1
www.iom.int/who-is-a-migrant
239
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
240 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Beginning the Intercultural Journey
When we embark on an intercultural journey, we face linguistic, cultural,
psychological, social, and possibly physiological changes, which Agar’s
(2007) reflections on cultural contact highlight:
[C]ulture is more than just something a group has. It’s something that
happens to people when they realize that their way of doing things isn’t
natural law, that other ways are possible. Something they’ve just heard,
something that jolted their sense of who they are, invites them into a
different way of seeing. The concept of ‘culture,’ like the concept of
‘language,’ has to change.
The two concepts have to change together. Language, in all its varieties,
in all the ways it appears in everyday life, builds a world of meanings. When
you run into different meanings, when you become aware of your own and
work to build a bridge to the others, ‘culture’ is what you’re up to. Language
fills the spaces between us with sound; culture forges the human connection
through them …
If you do start to see things this way, you change. The old ‘self,’ the one in
your heart and mind and soul, mutates as it comes into relationships with
others. The self stretches to comprehend them all. A life of Being turns into
a life of Becoming. (p. 23)
The intensity and impact of changes we undergo depends on the reasons
for, the duration of, and the particular context of each person’s journey, as
the next sections explain.
2
www.unhcr.org/en-us/1951-refugee-convention.html
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
11 Cultural Transitions 241
voluntary involuntary
such groups include the Uighur,3 who are sent to ‘reeducation’ camps in
China, or Muslim Rohingya refugees fleeing ethnic cleansing in Myanmar;
their lives are permanently impacted by forced removal (Habib, Jubb,
Ahmad, Rahman, & Pallard, 2018).
While groups at the ends of the continuum might be relatively easy to
categorize, the distinction between voluntary and involuntary migration is
often less clear. A young employee from South Africa might be transferred
to her company’s headquarters in Singapore (involuntary), but she is also
interested in gaining international work experience and living in a thriving
financial center (voluntary). Similarly, some international students may
place themselves all the way at the ‘voluntary’ end of the spectrum, while
others see their sojourn as more ‘involuntary,’ if they are driven by
significant parental or peer pressures. Thus, the continuum is more appro-
priately viewed as a flexible representation of the intercultural journey,
where individual experiences may vary from the position of the current
labels.
As we can see, migration, whether “imagined, desired, resisted, experi-
enced, managed, and represented … is a multifaceted reality” (Carling &
Collins, 2018, p. 909), often driven by conflicting and dynamic reasons
(Van Hear, Bakewell, & Long, 2018). Labor demands, political and social
factors, education, personal interests, or family and friends who already live
in the host communities can drive intercultural movement (Chirkov,
Vansteenkiste, Tao, & Lynch, 2007; Martin, 1993; Richmond, 1993).
These factors push or pull migration (Carling & Collins, 2018):
• Push factors typically refer to negative conditions in one’s home culture,
such as a poor employment prospects, poverty, war, political and/or
religious persecution, famine or natural disasters.
3
www.bbc.com/news/av/world-asia-45959533/uighur-detentions-searching-for-china-s-hidden-
camps
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
242 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
• Pull factors are positive conditions in a new cultural context: improved
economic and/or educational opportunities, improved living condi-
tions, available land for settling and agriculture, political and/or reli-
gious freedom, better healthcare, natural beauty, etc.
The journey is similarly diverse once migrants arrive to their destination.
They may or may not be welcomed by the existing community, whose
attitudes are often revealed by labels they use for the newcomers. Erdal and
Oppen (2018) report, for example, that many countries in the European
community identify migrants as
forced or voluntary to sort them into bureaucratic categories such as asylum
seeker, refugee, labour migrant, family reunification migrant and interna-
tional student, amongst others; where the first two categories are seen as
forced migrants and the latter three are seen as voluntary migrants. (p. 982)
The authors note that discursive practices can have significant con-
sequences for migrants in host countries, where the existing popula-
tion, out of fear or prejudice, may see the newcomers as a threat or
undeserving of support, and consequently label them as the “illegal
immigrant” or “bogus asylum seeker” (p. 983), even if the migrants
have valid reasons for leaving their homes (e.g., war, famine, persecu-
tion). Negative labels have tangible and intangible consequences if the
migrants cannot receive work permits, financial, social and political
support, acceptance, or status (e.g., eligibility for permanent residence).
In contrast, if migrants are viewed in a positive light, the host com-
munity is more likely to help them to assimilate, which eases the
cultural adaptation process for both groups.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
11 Cultural Transitions 243
welcome guests at a hotel or sell souvenirs. Thus, tourists participate in other
cultures without any substantial adaptation.
Sojourners, in contrast, reside in another culture for a longer period of
time, from a few weeks to several years. They are interested in learning
from another culture and are willing to change their beliefs and behaviors
(Byram, 1997). International students, employees who are assigned to other
countries, expatriates who move away for work or personal reasons, reli-
gious missionaries, and guest workers belong to this category (Jackson,
2014). Some sojourners – such as international volunteers (e.g., Doctors
without Borders) – might also influence the other culture. So-called ‘third
culture kids’ form a unique subcategory of sojourners; these children are
raised outside of their parents’ home cultures (when their parents work
abroad) and may establish limited relationships with the host culture(s),
while also not forming connections to their parents’ cultures (Pollock &
Van Reken, 2009).
The final group of people – immigrants and refugees – live in new cultural
settings the longest, often inhabiting those cultural spheres permanently.
As explained earlier, some of these transitions are a result of involuntary
relocation into often hostile host cultures. This fact can lead to some
immigrants living between cultures permanently: forced to leave their
home without being able to join the new culture (Jackson, 2014). Apart
from tourists, all migrants face the process of transitioning into another
social environment but follow divergent paths of adaptation.
Types of Adaptation
The process of adaptation entails changes that groups or individuals
undergo “as a result of contact with another cultural group” (Berry,
Poortinga, Breugelmans, Chasiotis, & Sam, 2011, p. 464). Both the host
community and the individual might change, adjusting their belief sys-
tems, values, practices, and behaviors; the newcomers typically experience
linguistic and cultural socialization (Duff, 2010). The acculturation process
often entails trade-offs between maintaining one’s original culture, lan-
guage, and social identities on the one hand, and forming new cultural and
linguistic affiliations and identities on the other (Berry, 2006). The strate-
gies involved in balancing these competing forces yield four possible out-
comes, according to Berry (2006): assimilation, integration, separation
(segregation), or marginalization.
The first outcome, assimilation, describes a process in which individuals
retain little of their original cultural identity and adopt the beliefs, values,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
244 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
and practices of the host culture(s). These individuals are likely to use the
language(s) of the host community and often abandon their L1, resulting in
the loss of the familial language and cultural practices within a generation
(Jackson, 2014). Many immigrants to new countries experience this, pro-
vided they are motivated and allowed to participate in new cultural
practices, and have the resources to do so. This process describes
my experience as an international student from Hungary. Studying in a
mid-sized city in Wisconsin without other Hungarians around, I soon
assimilated into the host culture: using its language, attending college,
befriending American students and my host family, and enthusiastically
embracing life in my adopted home. Since having children, we have
developed a more hybrid cultural existence (see below), incorporating
aspects of Hungarian culture into our American lives, adopting intercul-
tural practices and perspectives.
Integration, in contrast, happens when intercultural individuals adopt
certain aspects of the host culture and develop a positive relationship with
it, while also maintaining their original cultural and linguistic heritage.
They may seek out other people who speak their L1 and share their cultural
practices in their new home (Martin & Nakayama, 2018). Nineteenth-
century Norwegian settlers in Minnesota exemplify this experience well
(Gierde & Qualey, 2002). They arrived in large enough numbers, and into
an environment without a dominant ‘American’ culture, to have a large
impact on cultural practices to this day; Lutheran churches, Norwegian-
themed restaurants and shops, a commitment to teaching Norwegian, and
particular speech patterns in English reflect this heritage. Over time, these
settlers integrated into the U.S. American culture, while maintaining close
ties to their Norwegian ancestry. Integration often happens in cultural
groups with high ethnic group strength, groups that enjoy large numbers and
are respected by the host community. They are more capable of retaining
their own practices in a new culture than groups with less power.
Large numbers do not always reflect successful integration, though.
Instead, some large cultural groups experience separation or segrega-
tion, if they remain isolated from the host community or broader
cultural context. If the distance is initiated by the individual or
cultural group (Martin & Nakayama, 2018), perhaps because they
wish to differentiate themselves from the dominant society and main-
tain their own cultural or linguistic identity, then we speak of separa-
tion. The Amish community in the United States is an example of
this phenomenon: due to religious traditions, they remain relatively
isolated from the influences of popular American culture and embrace
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
11 Cultural Transitions 245
their own values, practices, and language (Nolt, 2016). Extreme cases
of separation can have devastating consequences, however, including
honor killings committed in Germany4 and Canada,5 when daughters
in immigrant families date members of the host community.
Conversely, segregation is a function of the host society shunting
immigrants, refugees, or minority ethnic and racial groups into geo-
graphically and socially isolated areas (Martin & Nakayama, 2018).
Segregation has significant socioeconomic consequences for members
of nondominant cultural groups, ranging from a lack of access to
education, financial resources, or healthcare to an individual’s sense of
identity; this is a recurring finding in research on African American
and Latinx communities in the United States (Charles, 2003; Smedley,
2012; Uzogara, 2019).
Marginalization reflects a process in which the immigrants’ or refugees’
original culture is not maintained, yet there is no adaptation to the host
culture either (Jackson, 2014). Some international students report feeling
marginalized; they find it difficult to maintain ties to their original culture
or communicate with other speakers of their L1, while also do not learn the
L2/Lx well enough to develop sufficiently positive connections with the
new community to participate in the new culture. This is also the experi-
ence of many refugees (e.g., Syrian refugees in Lebanon6), who cannot
maintain their own cultural practices but also cannot become full partici-
pants in their host community, given the complex geopolitical situation
that necessitated their fleeing Syria.
Martin and Nakayama (2018) describe a combined experience as well –
cultural hybridity – in which migrants assimilate to the host culture in some
respects and integrate with or remain separate from it in others. With
increased mobility, more and more people find themselves reflecting such
cultural hybridity, creating a “‘salad’ society, where each group retains a
distinctive flavor but blends together to make up one great society,” as
opposed to the metaphor of the American ‘melting pot,’ which requires its
members to assimilate to some common multicultural self (Martin &
Nakayama, 2018, p. 331). While cultural hybridity is an increasingly com-
mon phenomenon, it can make intergenerational adaptation challenging.
Children may be more able and willing to adapt to the host culture (e.g.,
language, personal appearance), while their parents long to retain more of
4
www.ehrenmord.de/doku/2018/doku_2018.php
5
www.cbc.ca/news/canada/montreal/story/2011/11/14/qc-shafia-murder-trial-wiretap.html
6
www.executive-magazine.com/economics-policy/the-fate-of-syrian-refugees-in-lebanon
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
246 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
their original culture. Alternately, children within the same family may
react differently, whereby one adheres to her parents’ practices, while her
siblings may assimilate more thoroughly into their new cultural context.
Finding a balance of assimilating, integrating, and retaining aspects of
previous cultural practices is possible, however, such as the case of a recent
immigrant who prefers to live in Chinatown in San Francisco (social
separation), uses Chinese at home and English in the broader community
(linguistic integration), and works at an international shipping company
(economic assimilation).
Importantly, the host community’s receptivity or attitudes towards the
newcomers – openness and tolerance or disdain and disrespect – plays a
significant role in the adaptation process. A diverse community that
accepts different cultural perspectives and practices is a more comfortable
landing place for immigrants than communities with little experience with
outsiders, as these communities might be more fearful and less welcoming,
creating a less pleasant environment for all participants, and potentially less
safe for the immigrant population (Kim, 2012). Even in contexts where the
overall sentiment is welcoming, migrants can encounter discrimination
from individuals based on their ethnic, racial, gender, religious, national,
or linguistic identities. For example, in 2018, two women in separate
incidents were attacked in London for speaking Spanish.7 Similarly, people
in South American countries that belong to the Mercosur trade organiza-
tion8 (Argentina, Brazil, Paraguay, Uruguay, and Venezuela) and that have
relatively open immigration policies, generally view skilled immigrants as a
welcome addition to society, yet immigrants are frequently blamed for
rising unemployment, housing costs, and drops in educational quality.9
Such sentiments are often heard where cultures collide, as it is easier to
blame ‘the other’ than explore and solve deeper causes of social dissatisfac-
tion (e.g., the root causes of rising unemployment, automatization, con-
flict, or environmental conditions).
7
www.independent.co.uk/news/uk/crime/woman-punched-racist-attack-london-overground-speak
ing-spanish-tfl-police-a8604436.html
8
www.cfr.org/backgrounder/mercosur-south-americas-fractious-trade-bloc
9
www.usnews.com/news/best-countries/articles/2017–07-10/south-americas-progressive-immigra
tion-laws-begin-to-fray
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
11 Cultural Transitions 247
While age (youth, in particular), higher skill and educational level, and L2/
Lx proficiency may help pave the way for smoother transitions, this section
focuses specifically on psychological variables – which impact the inter-
cultural experience in combination rather than isolation – that are more
malleable in the L2/Lx learning context: tolerance of ambiguity, motiva-
tion, and attitude.
Tolerance of Ambiguity
Tolerance of ambiguity refers to one’s ability or inability to cope with
situations “in which participants lack information they need to understand
the situation or possible outcomes” (Endres, Camp, & Milner, 2015, p. 2).
This trait is domain specific, meaning that we may be more tolerant of
ambiguity in one setting (e.g., learning grammar) but less so in others (e.g.,
open-ended use of the L2/Lx), so conversely, feeling uncomfortable in one
setting does not mean we feel that way in all situations (Chu, Lin, Chen,
Tsai, & Wang, 2015; Durrheim & Foster, 1997). Practicing speaking in the
L2/Lx with less reliance on the L1, using learning strategies that help us
remember vocabulary better or increase our comfort level regarding practi-
cing the L2/Lx with others in social situations can increase our tolerance of
ambiguity in new cultural situations as well (Ely, 1989; Oxford, 2002). A
solid self-concept, self-esteem, flexible attitudes, empathy, and the ability
to consider others as complex (e.g., identifying similarities and differences,
avoiding stereotypes) can help reduce stress during intercultural transitions
(Jackson, 2014). Additionally, mindfulness training may alleviate
difficulties for individuals with low tolerance of ambiguity and build
resilience, which may lead to improved chances for adaptation as well
(Brendel, Hankerson, Byun, & Cunningham, 2016).
Motivation
Motivation, which has long been recognized as a key ingredient of L2/Lx
learning success, is also essential for intercultural adaptation, because it is
one of the main driving forces behind learners’ willingness to engage with
other cultures and languages (Gardner & Lambert, 1972). Robert Gardner
(2006), one of the leading scholars on this topic, considered motivation to
be comprised of affective, cognitive, and behavioral aspects that include,
but are not limited to, goal-directedness, a commitment to effort, persis-
tence, self-confidence (self-efficacy), attention, positive affect, and making
“the language part of the self” (p. 4).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
248 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
This construct has often been separated into integrative and instrumen-
tal motivation (Gardner, 2006). Integrative motivation refers to an interest
in another culture, a desire to interact with its members, and a generally
positive attitude towards that culture. Integrative motivation correlates
very highly with language learning success. Importantly, the positive
interest and attitude (i.e., openness) do not have to be limited to a single
culture, but rather refer to “other cultural communities in general (i.e., an
absence of Ethnocentrism and authoritarianism, or the presence of
Xenophilic attitudes, etc.)” (Gardner, 2006, p. 7). In other words, if
someone develops positive attitudes towards learning one new culture,
that person is more likely to express openness and tolerance towards others
as well. In contrast, instrumental motivation reflects a more practical
orientation towards language learning (e.g., finding learning language X
useful). While instrumental motivation may be less powerful than inte-
grative motivation, Gardner (2006) argues that it is in fact “the intensity of
the motivation … that is important” (p. 12; emphasis mine). In other
words, the type of motivation is less significant than how motivated
someone is.
MacIntyre, Mackinnon, and Clément (2009) reiterate the importance
of three factors for language-learning success:
• integrativeness: a sincere personal interest in communicating with, tak-
ing on characteristics of, and perhaps identifying with members of
another culture;
• attitude: a positive orientation towards and enjoyment of the learning
situation; and
• motivation: the desire to learn another language, enjoyment of the
learning process and a consistent effort to improve one’s L2 skills.
The authors note that the relationship among these components is
mutually supportive, in that “integrativeness and attitudes toward the
learning situation combine to support motivation. In turn, motivation
supports the behaviours necessary to learn a language” (p. 44).
Recently, psychologists (Locke & Schattke, 2019) have revisited the
concept of motivation, by adding achievement motivation to intrinsic and
extrinsic motivational factors. Intrinsic motivation (similar to Gardner’s
integrative motivation) is experienced when an individual takes pleasure
in a particular activity for its own sake, such as learning a language,
traveling for fun, establishing meaningful relationships, striving for perso-
nal growth, or contributing to social causes, without concerns regarding a
particular outcome or external reward (Chirkov et al., 2007; Locke &
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
11 Cultural Transitions 249
Schattke, 2019). Conversely, extrinsic motivation (Gardner’s instrumental
motivation) is “doing something as a means to an end” (Locke & Schattke,
2019, p. 277). Here, the source of motivation resides outside of a task (but not
necessarily outside of an individual), such as learning a language in order to
get a job or building relationships that will yield some benefit. The new
component in this model, achievement motivation, pertains to a desire to do
well in something, whether or not an individual finds personal pleasure in the
task, such as performing well on a language test, regardless of enjoyment of
the learning process or the particular language. The focus, instead, is on
making progress towards a higher standard from one’s current abilities (Klein,
Cooper, & Manahan, 2013). Locke and Schattke (2019) concur with Gardner
(2006) that no type of motivation is inferior or superior to the others in terms
of outcome. Moreover, the authors argue, motivations can shift, when some-
thing that was originally external becomes personally valuable. For example,
we may start to learn a language because it was required, but then meet other
speakers of that language and begin to enjoy using and learning it.
L2/Lx research – including works by Higgins, Klein, and Strauman
(1985), Markus and Nurius (1986), and Dörnyei (2009) – connect motiva-
tion to a model of a dynamic ‘L2 self system’ that mediates personal
behavior. This system consists of multiple complex selves, changing over
time and across situations (MacIntyre et al., 2009). Higgins et al. (1985)
and Dörnyei (2009) distinguished the ideal self (how individuals wish to be
seen) from an ought-to-self (how individuals think they should be, to meet
expectations placed on them by other people), selves that we could become
(multiple possible selves), and selves we are afraid of becoming (what a
person fears). If possible, learners should identify ideal selves that are
realistic and in harmony with their ought-to-selves, while also powerful
enough to countereffect feared selves. These notions of self need to be
accompanied by actual strategies for implementing them, which might
include creating images of the speaker being able to speak the L2 skillfully
or interacting successfully with members of the culture whose language
they are learning (Dörnyei, 2009; MacIntyre et al., 2009).
From a socially oriented perspective, Ushioda (2009) suggests a “person-
in-context” view of L2 motivation, which she sees as being “emergent from
relations between real persons, with particular social identities, and the
unfolding cultural context of activity” (p. 215). The premise of her under-
standing of motivation is the complex social relationships among indivi-
duals and other people (e.g., friends, family, co-workers), places, and
cultural practices. She also argues that motivation may be experienced
differently in Western and Eastern cultures, further highlighting the
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
250 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Table 11.1 Self-determination continuum
Internalized
Intrinsic motivation extrinsic motivation Introjected motivation External motivation
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
11 Cultural Transitions 251
food, and constantly meet new people, among other sources of stress. Even
if we are excited about life in a new cultural setting, the sheer amount of
new experiences may lead to culture shock and language fatigue or shock.10 In
our home culture, we are comfortable with the physical and social signs
that help us navigate everyday life, but when we enter a new cultural
context, our existing schemata may be irrelevant or ineffective, and until
we build up new ones that are appropriate in the new culture, we may feel
disoriented and overwhelmed. For example, German hospitals, which face
significant staffing shortages, regularly recruit doctors from other countries
to fill positions. Klingler and Marckmann (2016) interviewed several such
doctors about their experience and found that most of them struggle in
some way, with language skills, with cultural knowledge about the orga-
nization of the German healthcare system, available treatments, or rules
about patient privacy. Many of the doctors also found the more equally
distributed power structures between themselves and the patients or nurses
confusing, since in many source countries, doctors had a distinctly higher
status than either of the other groups. Such experiences of disorientation
are shared by many sojourners who leave a context, where they process the
world largely subconsciously, for a new culture, where even the smallest
decisions might require conscious thought and deliberate effort to navigate
(Arthur, 2004). Transitional challenges are often exacerbated by the overly
romantic images of the other culture we create prior to arrival, to which it
cannot live up. Fortunately, the stresses experienced during transition do
abate, as we improve our skills and adopt better to our new cultural
context.
The process of cultural adaptation is often described as a U-shaped curve
(Lysgaard, 1955); however, scholarship on intercultural communication has
recognized the need to expand the adaptation process to situations in
which migrants – temporary or long-term – return to their original cultures
(or the cultures of their parents and ancestors), resulting in a W-shaped
model (Gullahorn & Gullahorn, 1963). While these two models have been
criticized for being overly simplistic, with low predictive ability (Berardo,
2006, 2012; La Brack, 2010; La Brack & Berardo, 2007; Ward, Okura,
Kennedy, & Kojima, 1998; Ward & Kennedy 2001), they are used exten-
sively in presojourn orientations inside and outside of academia (e.g., by
study-abroad programs and global corporations), and they provide a useful
10
International students often report that it is not any one thing that triggers culture shock, but rather
the fact that many or most things, behaviors, practices, places are new all at once.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
252 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
shared language to describe divergent experiences that students (and their
instructors) may have during intercultural journeys.
Honeymoon Adaptation
phase phase
Adjustment
phase
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
11 Cultural Transitions 253
in a sense of loss, not unlike grief. Jackson (2014) identifies several chal-
lenges that can have emotional, psychological, behavioral, cognitive, and
physiological repercussions:
• irritability and mood swings • seeking out others from home (at the
• powerlessness and vulnerability exclusion of the host culture)
• anxiety, sadness, and loneliness • difficulties with decision-making and
• physical ailments (e.g., body aches, problem-solving
headaches) • negative comparisons between one’s
• homesickness home and the new culture(s)
• romanticized views of home • hostility towards members of the host
• fear of novel experiences (meeting new culture (i.e., ‘us’ versus ‘them’)
people, trying new food, etc.) • refusal to learn or use the L2/Lx
• feelings of inadequacy (self-doubt)
• confusion about sense of self
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
254 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Importantly, some sojourners never experience culture shock, or experi-
ence it only briefly and with minor effects, possibly because they adapt so
successfully to the host culture that they view any irritation as easily
resolved. For others, this phase is relatively short and entails only moderate
discomfort. Surprisingly, more culture shock does not mean having a
worse experience in the new culture. Quite to the contrary, often more
communication with ‘natives’ (socializing with peers, host families, and
friends) in the new culture leads to more pronounced feelings of culture
shock, but also more satisfaction with the overall experience and better
adaptation, possibly because the same social interactions that cause culture
shock also provide the individual with social support (Rohrlich & Martin
1991; Zimmerman, 1995).
Phase 3, adjustment, represents a more peaceful stage of the intercultural
journey, including the ability to appreciate or laugh about mishaps in the
new cultural setting and integrate aspects of one’s original and new cultures
with increasing effectiveness (Martin & Nakayama, 2018). During this
period, the sojourner learns how to inhabit the new culture, use the new
language, and manage day-to-day life successfully. He or she is increasingly
more comfortable in the new cultural context, accepting differences with-
out feeling overwhelmed or threatened by them. Because at this stage
language skills have improved as well, it is easier to form social connections.
Ideally, the adjustment process leads to adaptation, at which stage sojour-
ners are able to participate in their new culture, have better-developed
social networks, see the differences and similarities between their original
and adopted cultures (perspectives, behaviors, practices, products), and
navigate them effectively, if not always easily (Jackson, 2014). This stage
often entails bi- or multilingualism and a complex intercultural identity.
Many sojourners and immigrants do not progress through all four stages
or not linearly. Instead, individuals may make progress in some areas, only
to backslide in others, as they try to integrate existing and novel beliefs and
practices or adapt to new expectations. This progression may alternate
“between feeling relatively adjusted and experiencing culture shock,” ide-
ally, with an overall positive development (Martin & Nakayama, 2018,
p. 342). Kim (2012) describes this progression as stress-adaptation-growth
cycles (see Figure 11.3) or a cyclical process, in which stress leads to adapta-
tion and growth over time (Kim, 2001a):
The stress-adaptation experiences bring about change and growth – the
creative responses to new circumstances. Over time, the stress-adaptation-
growth dynamic plays out not in a smooth, linear progression but in a cyclic
and continual ‘draw-back-to-leap’ pattern … Each stressful experience
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
11 Cultural Transitions 255
stress
new creative
experiences solutions
growth adaptation
[causes the sojourners to] ‘draw back,’ which then activates their adaptive
energy to help them reorganize themselves and ‘leap forward …’ [in a
process of] engagement and disengagement. (p. 238)
This growth process can be facilitated by several individual variables
(Kim, 2012):
1. preparation: how prepared a sojourner is for the journey,
2. ethnic proximity/distance: how similar or different the migrant is from
the host community culturally, ethnically, racially, and
3. personality disposition: openness to new information, strength, resilience,
and positivity (i.e., optimistic outlook).
Intercultural growth is worth striving for, since it confers significant
benefits, including improved functional fitness (ability to manage tasks in
new environments), psychological health (ability to communicate well
enough to overcome culture shock and establish new social networks in
novel cultural contexts), intercultural identity orientation (the ability to see
oneself flexibly connected to multiple cultures, neither rigidly grounded in
the home culture nor overly dependent on the host culture), and inter-
cultural transformation (the ability to inhabit “a new, alternative identity
that is broader, more inclusive, more intercultural” Kim, 2001b, pp.
232–233).
The recursivity of the adaptation process, including surprising periods
of adaptation required for reentering one’s own culture after a sojourn
elsewhere prompted the expansion of the U-Curve model into a W-
Curve one.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
256 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Honeymoon Adaptation
phase Euphoria
phase Adaptation
Adjust- Re-adjustment
ment phase
phase
Re-entry
Crisis / Culture shock phase shock
11
https://international.uottawa.ca/sites/international.uottawa.ca/files/a-exchanges-outgoing-reverse
cultureshock-re-entry.pdf
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
11 Cultural Transitions 257
These feelings can be mitigated by going for walks, meeting with
friends, connecting to others who have lived in the same culture,
showing interest in others, and rekindling old friendships;
• re-adjustment: acceptance of what is good and what is less ideal at
home, developing routines, finding the humorous in everyday mis-
takes, there are fewer health problems;
• adaptation: (added to the guidance materials) participation in the
home culture, expanded social networks that include members from
all cultures that the sojourner was a part of at home and abroad,
integrated perspectives, behaviors, practices, products; bi- or multi-
lingualism and a complex intercultural identity.
There are several reasons why sojourners sometimes find it difficult to
return home. They may have changed after adapting to their host culture’s
values, perspectives, and practices; some people might even prefer their
lifestyle abroad. Therefore, they do not fit into the social structures or
cultural habits of the home culture the same way as they had prior to their
original departure. This may be challenging both for the returnees and for
their friends, family, or coworkers. Perhaps not surprisingly, people who
integrate better and more fully into the host community – by making
friends, adjusting to the language and cultural norms – may find reaccul-
turation more challenging (Jackson, 2014). When the adjustment between
the cultures is significant, the sojourner may also face identity confusion,
the feeling of being “caught between two distinct worlds, the one they left
behind and the one they have returned to. They may not feel that they fit
into either” (Jackson, 2014, p. 208).
Sometimes, however, it is not the people who change but the socio-
political context from which they left, especially during times of political or
social upheaval. This was the case for many refugees who left Eastern
Europe during the Soviet regime and returned to their home countries
after the political changes of the early 1990s. During the 30–40 years they
spent abroad, popular culture, family, technology, language have all chan-
ged, making reentry strange and disorienting.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
258 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
intercultural journey. Sojourners and immigrants can gather information
about the host culture via the Internet or by talking to others who are
already there. They can find out about local customs, values, laws, prac-
tices, and language use along with other practical and relevant information.
Identifying aspects of the new culture that may be similar to or different
from their own can help them decide in what ways and to what extent they
may wish to adapt to its beliefs and practices, and what aspects of their own
culture they may wish to retain. For example, the laws and the educational
system might require adaptation (assimilation), but food, language use in
the home and many religious practices can continue to provide an emo-
tional anchor during transition (integration or separation).
Since the skills and knowledge necessary for successful intercultural
transitions are closely related to the aspects of intercultural communication
presented in Chapters 4–9, instead of presenting separate lesson plans, the
pedagogical activities that follow connect the information covered in those
chapters to the material discussed in this one. The L2/Lx should be used as
much as possible, but even if discussions rely on the L1, they are worth the
investment of time, as they may inspire the learners to continue with the
language or undertake study or work abroad.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
11 Cultural Transitions 259
what did they find easy or difficult, what helped them during their
intercultural journey).
• Set up telecollaborative exchanges, especially if several students are
considering studying or working abroad, so that they can establish
friendships with in-group members who can serve as their intercultural
allies upon arrival.
• Address stereotypes about the host community head-on; find stereo-
typical views about the learners’ home culture(s), and discuss how (in)
accurate they are, to help them understand the limitations and sources
of their own stereotyped thinking.
• Ask learners to write an essay imagining their life in the host culture two
years later (describing ideal selves indirectly).
• Have students research their destination, including small- and big-
culture information; they can create a multimodal presentation (col-
lage, PowerPoint, short film) for themselves for their first week in the
host community
In addition, learners can discuss several pre-, during- and postsojourn
strategies that Jackson (2014) and Smith (2002) have proposed, in
order to help ease intercultural transitions for international students.
Table 11.2 presents a synthesis of their suggestions, with a few addi-
tional recommendations.
Summary
As this chapter has demonstrated, successful intercultural transitions
depend on individual and contextual factors, including language skills,
knowledge of the host culture, tolerance of ambiguity, the attitude and
motivation of the sojourner within various social contexts, and, not neg-
ligibly, the attitudes, values, and behavior of the host community towards
temporary visitors and long-term immigrants.
The intercultural transition process has been described as a U- or
W-shaped curve. The former illustrates the adaptation-process
involved in moving from one’s home culture to a new social context,
while the latter includes a migrant’s reentry into his or her original
culture, a transition that many people do not anticipate to be challen-
ging. Another model examines a dialectic view of how discomfort can
lead to an emergent process of adaptation and personal growth. These
models serve as useful tools for describing intercultural transitions but
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
260 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Table 11.2 Strategies to facilitate intercultural transitions
Pre-departure
1. Research your destination.
2. Keep your goals and expectations realistic.
3. Study intercultural communication.
4. Develop a strong sense of humor; the ability to laugh at your mistakes encourages you
to keep trying and can mitigate potential relational mistakes (e.g., it can soften any
offenses committed).
5. Reframe difficulties as useful stepping stones for personal growth, rather than failures
or insurmountable obstacles.
6. Build your L2/Lx skills and practice as much as possible, especially every-day,
culturally appropriate (e.g., colloquial) language.
7. Attend pre-sojourn orientations, if available.
In the new environment
1. Get to know your environment.
2. Be patient, adjustment takes time.
3. Keep in touch with your support network (parents, friends, etc.) at home.
4. Find a good routine to follow.
5. Attend orientation activities.
6. Suspend judgment; be open to new experiences, be adventurous, view the other
culture’s values as neither good nor bad, just different.
7. Form friendships with people from the host community, your home culture, and other
international students.
8. Take temporary breathers; recognize issues that make you irritated, angry, fearful, or
upset; occasionally retreating to something familiar (e.g., meals) can make the
adaptation process more successful.
9. Find a cultural ally who can help you navigate the new culture (either a member of the
host community or another international student who has lived there for a longer
period of time).
10. Set goals for your sojourn and revisit them often; develop habits of self-reflection and
analysis.
11. Analyze values mindfully (i.e., recognize that you do not have to accept or adopt all
practices and perspectives of the other culture; you have a choice).
12. Engage with the culture; practice the L2/Lx in various contexts to improve your
language skills.
13. Embrace miscommunication; seek help to understand its source and resolve it,
apologizing as needed.
14. Understand the limitations of cultural assumptions (e.g., stereotypes).
15. Accept that you will experience setbacks and persevere through them.
16. Ask questions, seek out new knowledge about practices, perspectives, and products
from a variety of sources.
Prior to returning home
1. Prepare for your reentry into your home culture.
2. Recognize that identities likely change as a result of emotional and sensory experiences
in the new culture, which can affect how you might feel in your original culture upon
reentry.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
11 Cultural Transitions 261
Table 11.2 (cont.)
3. Ensure closure; take the time to say good-bye to friends you have made and celebrate
shared experiences.
4. Seek advice online and from experts about successful readjustment.
5. Set realistic expectations for the reentry process; be aware that confusion may
accompany your return home.
6. Reflect on and prepare to apply skills of adjustment (e.g., patience, empathy,
curiosity), since readjustment to the home culture can mirror experiences that you
underwent at the beginning of your journey.
7. Look for jobs or other opportunities in which you can put to use the linguistic,
cognitive, and affective skills and the new sense of self that you developed during your
sojourn; jobs in translation, social justice, or with international companies can help
maintain and expand your skills, while improving the lives of those who benefit from
your skills.
Back home
1. Establish structure, set up a clear schedule and tasks to reduce the many choices that
make reentry confusing.
2. Share your stories in small doses and reciprocally; while relating stories about
adventures, ask questions of and express an interest in the lives and experiences of
people who had stayed behind.
3. Participate in reentry courses if available.
4. Recognize that reentry adaptation takes time and will likely occur in cycles of stress,
growth, and adaptation.
5. Recognize negative thoughts and avoid snap judgments about cultural differences,
events, or people at home.
6. Connect with other returnees through a course, clubs, or international organizations.
7. Serve as a cultural ally to immigrants in your community.
8. Use social media to nurture the international friendships you made during your
sojourn (i.e., stay in touch).
9. Continue to practice and improve your L2/Lx skills.
10. Reflect on your experiences and make concrete plans for future intercultural
experiences.
11. Continue to seek advice online and from experts about successful readjustment.
12. Rekindle old friendships, while recognizing that some relationships may flounder if
your values have changed and are incompatible with those of your previous friends.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
262 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:54:47, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.012
chapter 12
Chapter Overview
Each individual represents a complex network of social relationships,
which begin with primary socialization by parents or caretakers (Duff,
2010; Ochs & Schieffelin, 2008). As we grow up, we get socialized into
more and more social groups, including families, friendships, sports teams,
religious organizations, or professional associations, just to name a few.
These social relationships shape our identity throughout our lives. Some
aspects of our identity are determined for us: our nationality, ethnicity, or
race. Others we control, such as friendships, most working relationships,
athletic organizations, or politics. Some aspects are relatively constant,
since societies and social groups strive for stability, preferring little or
slow change (Byram 1997). Other aspects are malleable. We present
ourselves in different ways depending on specific situations; for example,
we may emphasize our professional or familial selves or our sexual identity
in different interactional contexts.
We can visualize the process of expanding social affiliations as etching
new facets on a crystal (see Figure 12.1), transforming it from its original
form (our infancy, on the left) into a multifaceted prism.
263
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
264 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Studying another language provides a unique opportunity to reflect on
who we are, because our sense of self often gets renegotiated when we
encounter new cultures and navigate new social expectations. To delve into
these issues, this chapter first examines identity as a complex and dynamic
phenomenon, then considers the relationship between language and identity.
Identity as Multiplex
By identity, we generally mean the essence of who we are, how we want to
be seen, and how others view us. Our identity is complex, as the following
meme circulating on Facebook on April 15, 2018, effectively illustrates:
When I went to college, I thought about joining one of those Asian student
organizations. But I wasn’t sure if I wanted to go ‘all in’ on being Asian. I had to
ask myself how integral it was to my identity. I’ve never been very traditional.
I grew up in a white town on Long Island. If I described myself in three
adjectives, ‘Asian’ wouldn’t be one of them. But on the other hand, the world is
going to see race anyway. I view myself as Chinese, but Asian is the first thing
people see. So if it’s how everyone sees you, should you see yourself the
same way?
As the poster’s self-reflection suggests, our identities originate both
within ourselves and externally, encompassing “an individual’s unique
characteristics . . . defined as one’s knowledge of membership in
certain social groups and the social meanings attached to the group”
(Abrams, O’Connor, & Giles, 2002, p. 226). These groups include
friends and family, various social circles to which we belong, nations,
and supranational organizations. Aspects of identity can be viewed as
categories delineated by similarities with and differences from these
groups.
Previous psychological understandings of identity considered it to be
relatively static, homogenized, created and maintained by individuals as
they relate to cultural groups, such as women or men, ethnic or linguistic
groups, or social class (Duff, 2012; Hua, 2014). Identity, according to this
paradigm, is comprised of:
1. individualized identity: the distinction between self and others;
2. familial identity: an individual’s place in connection to others within
a cultural group; and
3. spiritual identity: the sense of self in relation to belief systems (Martin
& Nakayama, 2018).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 265
This theoretical understanding also supposes that, while all three aspects of
identity exist in all cultures, the relative emphasis given to each varies. For
example, in Canada, the individual identity, how people see themselves
(e.g., as smart, athletic, outgoing, or friendly), is emphasized, whereas in
East Asian cultures, the development of relational identities might be
encouraged over the individualist aspect, valuing obligation and responsi-
bility towards one’s elders over an individual’s wants or needs (Martin &
Nakayama, 2018).
More recently, identity has been reconceptualized as a dynamic, multi-
layered construct, emerging out of socially situated relationships, varying
across contexts, and created interactively (Abrams et al., 2002; Gábrity,
2012). Examining the issue from this viewpoint, Hua (2014) and Tracy
(2002) describe four main types of identity, which are presented in Figure
12.2 as layers.
Master identities are relatively stable, with little variation across interac-
tional contexts, such as our age, ethnicity, gender, or nationality. In the
next circle lie personal identities, our personality and character traits, our
attitudes (e.g., towards others, life, learning an L2/Lx), and motivations.
These are more malleable and may change over time. We may become
more or less progressive in our political views or open to new experiences as
we gain confidence. The next layer represents interactional identities, which
include “specific and situational roles people enact in a communicative
context,” such as sibling, student, nurse, or soccer player (Hua, 2014,
p. 202). These identities are more flexible and dynamic, even if fairly
consistent within unique situations. For example, during a job interview,
the hiring committee’s role as interviewer and the job candidate’s role as
dynamic
More
relational
identities
interactional
identities
personal
identities
master
identities
stable
More
Figure 12.2 Layers of identity (based on Hua, 2014 and Tracy, 2002)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
266 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
respondent stay quite steady, but the interactants move out of these roles at
the end of the interview. In contrast, the relational identities that form the
next layer are situationally dynamic, shifting quickly and frequently within
one interaction, depending on our interlocutors. Recently, I had
a conversation with a close friend, who is a fellow faculty member at
a large university in the United States. We started our regular conversation
chatting about our children and the latest updates on our summer plans,
emphasizing our identities as friends and mothers. Then we discussed our
research plans for the upcoming weeks and the courses we would be
teaching next year, thus, highlighting our scholarly, professional selves.
Our chat concluded with an extended leave-taking sequence during which
we talked about our plans for the upcoming holiday weekend and sent
greetings to each other’s family. The variable topics prompted shifting
relational identities.
The word ‘identities’ is used in the plural intentionally, as we embody
a range of possible selves, shaped by emotional, moral, and cognitive forces.
Table 12.1 presents several identities and illustrates them with specific
linguistic examples.
Multiple aspects of identity co-occur, since we simultaneously have
a gender, national, familial, linguistic, and minority/majority identity,
and the unique combination we embody make up our personal identity.
This identity is not necessarily coherent or unified, because some aspects of
it may be in conflict with each other. For example, taking care of elderly
parents may cause tension between a person’s identities as ‘child’ and
‘caretaker’ or when we juggle traditional social roles at home, while having
to adopt modern ones at work.
In addition to being complex at any given moment, our personal identities
also change over time. From early infancy, before any awareness of social
categories becomes relevant for us as newborns, we are ascribed a gender
identity and become a daughter or a son, grandchild, sibling, a citizen of
a nation, and a member of a social class. Later, as we expand our social
experiences beyond our home, we become swimmers, ice skaters, mathema-
ticians, travelers, social rights activists, or actors. As we age, we take on further
social roles: spouses, parents, aunts, uncles, employees, and employers.
Our personal identity is inherently linked to our social identities (which
straddle all four of Hua and Tracy’s categories) which make up most facets
of our identity and reflect various group affiliations and the emotional
significance of such relationships (Liu, Volcic, & Gallois, 2011; Wintergerst
& McVeigh, 2011). We associate with some people because we share
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 267
Table 12.1 Aspects of identity with linguistic examples
Aspect of
identity Definition and description Language example
1
www.soc.ucsb.edu/sexinfo/article/overview-sexual-orientations
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
268 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Table 12.1 (cont.)
Aspect of
identity Definition and description Language example
2
www.maasai-association.org/ceremonies.html
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 269
Table 12.1 (cont.)
Aspect of
identity Definition and description Language example
3
www.theatlantic.com/national/archive/2011/11/racist-anti-immigrant-cartoons-from-the-turn-of-the
-20th-century/383248/
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
270 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Table 12.1 (cont.)
Aspect of
identity Definition and description Language example
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 271
Table 12.1 (cont.)
Aspect of
identity Definition and description Language example
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
272 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Table 12.1 (cont.)
Aspect of
identity Definition and description Language example
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 273
social identities are multiple (e.g. a multilingual, gay British Indian male
student who is a devout Hindu and belongs to Green Peace). Together, all of
our diverse roles and attributes help us to realize our overall social identity,
which continues to evolve throughout our life as our preferences, interests,
financial situation, job status, marital situation and other social dimensions
change. (p. 137)
The group affiliations we establish reflect diverse social belongings,
through which we develop relevant schemata for processing the world,
such as organizing knowledge, getting things done, and interacting with
others. At the same time, social identities are fluid; they describe an
individual’s sense of belonging, which might shift across situations, as
Hofstede (2001) explains:
Populations that fight each other on the basis of their different ‘felt’
identities may very well share the same values. Examples are the linguistic
regions in Belgium, the religions in Northern Ireland, and tribal groups in
Africa. A shared identity needs a shared Other: At home, I feel Dutch and
very different from other Europeans, such as Belgians and Germans; in Asia
or the United States, we all feel like Europeans. (p. 10)
This quote illustrates the changing perspectives that lie behind identities.
The frame of reference impacts which aspects of our identity become
salient (i.e., relevant). A graduating senior from university, majoring in
applied linguistics, is an avid hiker, a bilingual speaker of English and
Spanish, and an aspiring elementary school teacher. She will be working in
Madrid, Spain after graduation, teaching English as a Second Language to
elementary school children. At work, her training in applied linguistics and
plans for teaching will be most salient; consequently, she will use language
with her colleagues and students that reflect her training and professional
interests. At the same time, she can meet fellow hikers (Spanish or inter-
national) to establish friendships with and help reduce feelings of home-
sickness. By seeking out such work-external communities, she can also
practice and continue to improve her Spanish language skills.
Usually, we navigate our various identities seamlessly and subcon-
sciously, but sometimes, we become aware of them, when others’ assump-
tions about us do not align with how we view ourselves.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
274 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
adopt ourselves, subconsciously or consciously. Ascribed identities, in con-
trast, are imposed on us by others. Ideally, avowed identity is aligned with
ascribed identity: college student, spiritual leader, politician, or father. At
other times, there might be a misalignment between the two views. Many
native speakers of English report the experience of studying abroad where
their L2/Lx is spoken and try to interact with native speakers of that
language, only to find that those speakers switch to English immediately.
These French, Arabic, Swahili, or Chinese speakers might not recognize
the L2/Lx learner as capable enough to communicate. These experiences
can be annoying, but other instances of misalignment can have serious
consequences. After the terror attacks on New York on September 11, 2001,
for example, many Americans conflated Islam and terrorism,4 resulting in
a decades-long increase in hate crimes against people of the Muslim faith in
the United States. Moreover, anyone to whom an American ascribed
a Muslim identity could be in danger, regardless of their actual religious
affiliation. Many members of the Sikh community experienced rising hate
crimes,5 as their Sikh identity was erased, replaced by inaccurate ascribed
identities by their attackers as Muslims and terrorists. Such misidentifica-
tions are incorrect in two regards: most Muslims are not terrorists, and
Sikhs are not Muslim.
Scholars who approach intercultural communication from a critical,
advocacy perspective aim to draw attention to discrimination based on
identity, which they view as being shaped and constrained by institutional
and societal structures, including historical, economic, political, and dis-
cursive inequalities (Collier, 2005; Martin & Nakayama, 2018). Often,
multiple aspects of identity intersect, each contributing in unique but
compounding ways to experiences of societal oppression (Crenshaw,
1989). The reasons for and ways in which such oppression differs across
cultures, requiring new understandings beyond what we know of our own
local cultural contexts. Within a critical advocacy framework, avowed and
ascribed identities are often contested (questioned and resisted), if they are
in conflict. Women’s increased political presence in Tunisia is one illus-
trative case of dynamic identity development as a contested site.
Written after gaining independence from France, Tunisia’s constitution in
1959 did not mention women, either regarding legislative power or legal
protections (Hitman, 2018). In spite of “state feminism,” where those in
4
http://theconversation.com/explaining-the-rise-in-hate-crimes-against-muslims-in-the-us-80304
5
www.npr.org/2018/09/14/647426417/people-saw-only-a-turban-and-a-beard-reflecting-on-a-post-
sept-11-death
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 275
power “handed down” women’s rights (Shalaby, 2016, p. 173), men could hold
legal guardianship over women until 1990 (Moghadan, 2005, 2013), and
women’s organizations were under strict state supervision (Hitman, 2018;
Shalaby, 2016). Since the 1990s, Tunisian women have taken an active role
in advocating for women’s rights, which continued throughout the Arab
Spring in 2010, a revolution that aimed to improve sociopolitical conditions
across the Arab world. Unlike other nations that quickly returned to auto-
cratic control, Tunisia adopted a revised constitution and put in place
significant safeguards to protect its democracy. Yet, women were once again
relegated to second-class citizenship; the 2014 constitution accorded them
a complementary role, not equal partnership with men, and removed
a number of protections for women (Hitman, 2018; Shalaby, 2016). The
ensuing nationwide protests spearheaded by women’s organizations led to
revisions that granted women equal rights and equal citizenship (al-Rashed,
2014; Shalaby, 2016). Although women still hold only about 30 percent of
elected seats in Parliament, the changes in women’s situation – and conse-
quently their identities – have been remarkable. Hailed as “moderating voices”
in politics, Tunisian women now embody the identities of political, social,
and economic leadership (Shalaby, 2016, p. 183), whose collective identity was
a crucial force in shaping the new constitution (Hitman, 2018). Due to
persistent ascribed identities as mothers, wives, daughters, it is still a struggle
to implement equality into actual practice, but the changes illustrated by the
situation of women in Tunisia reflects the necessity of political openness for
change, and society’s willingness to embrace that opportunity and drive
further progress (Hitman, 2018). Each of these steps requires a reenvisioning
of women’s complex identities in unique cultural contexts.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
276 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
choices (Ting-Toomey, 2005), which may benefit or disadvantage us
socially, economically, or personally.
One important social benefit is that languages are powerful symbols of
group membership. They can be used to signal positive linguistic affiliation
with a cultural group, such as wearing a heart-shaped sticker with the word
Italiano or Español by L2/Lx learners. For similar reasons, we self-identify
as Francophiles or use jargon to signal our in-group identity as medical
professionals or athletes. The 2019 Democratic debates likewise prompted
many presidential hopefuls to include messages in Spanish in order to
express their commitment to the Latinx community in the United States.6
There are also increasing instances where individuals and communities
have come to accentuate previously marginalized language varieties, help-
ing to revitalize them and raise their prestige. By recognizing diverse
varieties of languages, such as Hawai‘i Creole English (Drager, 2012) or
Kristang in Singapore (i.e., Malaccan Creole Portuguese) (Wong, 2017),
members of these speech communities feel more empowered, recognized,
and legitimated in society. The immense power of language should not be
underestimated: the ability to use a language that one closely affiliates with
positively impacts that individual’s sense of belonging to society, sense of
self-worth, and legitimacy (Lee, 2017).
Unfortunately, the power of symbols can also be negative, as languages
can be used to exclude others. If a government does not provide sufficient
educational materials in certain languages, it restricts academic access to
some social groups and limits their scholarly achievements (Office of the
United Nations High Commissioner for Human Rights, 2017). In
a comparable situation, trainee teachers from Northern England were
asked to reduce their linguistic and regional identities in favor of one
that was perceived to be more prestigious, under the guise of making
themselves more comprehensible to their students and their parents.7
Such incidents can be found in many places around the globe.
In multilingual contexts, it is often sociopolitical forces that shape
linguistic boundaries, resulting in more or less powerful languages vying
for use, respect, and prestige. Power distributions in bilingual and multi-
lingual communities are typically uneven: migrant populations usually
have to adapt to the dominant culture (e.g., Turkish migrants in
Germany, North African migrants in France, or South Asian migrants in
6
www.npr.org/2019/06/27/736520658/spanish-speakers-highlight-democratic-primary-debate
7
www.bbc.com/capital/story/20170523-does-your-accent-make-you-sound-smarter and
www.theguardian.com/education/2016/may/12/trainee-teachers-from-northern-england-told-to-
modify-their-accents
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 277
Australia). This nonreciprocal relationship arises from and reflects clear
power differentials. Such power differentials also arose when colonial
powers in the nineteenth and early twentieth centuries did not take ethnic
and linguistic identity into consideration, when creating national and
political boundaries, thus forcing multilingual and cultural identities on
most inhabitants in West Africa and the Middle East (Myers-Scotton,
2006).
Bourdieu (1991), a French sociologist, proposed that languages have
symbolic capital. That is, some languages enjoy significant prestige and
cultural value, while others lack both. The value society places on
a language – and by extension, its speakers – is usually determined by
social groups who are in positions of power; they decide who is allowed
to join the community and who is not. The driving force most often is
to reinforce the existing power structures. Consequently, it is the power-
ful in society who determine which languages or language varieties are
valuable and which are less so. They also determine who has access to
power through language (e.g., controlling voting ballots, educational
and healthcare materials). Not having access to a qualified interpreter
in legal interactions, for example, not only denies a defendant his or her
human rights but also serves to perpetuate existing power structures.
One illustrative example pertains to a Mexican national, who tried to
enter the United States (legally and as an informant on a drug smuggling
operation) and was detained at the border because the government used
an ad hoc rather than a professional, trained interpreter, who mistrans-
lated both the language and the cultural content of a two-hour inter-
rogation (Angelelli, 2015). By conveying the border agents’ questions as
assertions and not being able to see the detainee’s nonverbal commu-
nication that played a crucial role in his communication with nonnative
speakers of his language, the interpreter reinforced the disparate power
relationships among the participants which resulted in the detainee
being sent to jail for two months before he was acquitted. ‘José’s’ case
is not unique by any means; linguistic discrimination is systemic at the
border:
There are no dedicated professional interpreters to serve the communica-
tive needs of this federal office. The agents are told that when they need
interpreting services they have to call a company that provides this service
remotely. When recruiting interpreters, companies providing over-the-
phone interpreting services seem to focus more on the bottom line than
on the quality of the service offered. The interpreter used by the tele-
phone company in this case is not a certified legal/court interpreter . . .
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
278 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
The law establishes that the judges are responsible for appointing and
screening legal/court interpreters . . . [but] judges do not have expertise to
do this. (Angelelli, 2015, p. 201)
While it is typically socioeconomically and historically more domi-
nant languages that enjoy prestige (e.g., English in Kenya), sometimes
cultural groups with fewer members wield linguistic power. Some
examples include Afrikaans speakers in South Africa, Catalan speakers
in the Catalonia region in Spain, and Spanish-speaking Cuban immi-
grants in Miami, who constitute powerful economic and cultural com-
munities, and who enjoy considerable ethnolinguistic vitality, allowing
their members to take pride in their linguistic identities (Myers-Scotton,
2006).
The relationship between language, power, and identity is complicated
sociopolitically. In many African countries, for example, the language
established under colonialism – such as English, French, or Portuguese –
remains an official language and is often seen as a neutral option when
several ethnic and linguistic groups compete for official status. In these
instances, the former colonial language is viewed as “the best of bad
choices” (Myers-Scotton, 2006, p. 48).
Language and identity, however, are not only deeply connected at the
societal level. We regularly deploy our linguistic repertoires at the personal
level as well, to situate ourselves as multilingual and multicultural social
beings during interactions.
Intercultural Identity
The ideal outcome of extensive L2/Lx learning is the development of an
intercultural identity. Reitenauer, Cress, and Bennett (2005) define inter-
cultural identity as one that can traverse cultural situations, based on “the
creation of learned and shared values, beliefs, and behaviors in
a community of interacting people”; this new identity is shaped by “the
many cultural influences that impact us, including nationality, ethnicity,
race, age, gender, physical characteristics, sexual orientation, economic
status, education, profession, religion, and organizational affiliation”
(p. 68). An intercultural identity enables the “intercultural speaker”
(Byram, 1997, p. 32) to participate in intercultural communication in
a way that no interlocutor is considered to be better than the other (even
when one is a native speaker of a language and the other is not), just
speaking on equal footing, presenting social identities they wish to make
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 279
salient in that particular exchange. Interactional success would be mea-
sured in terms of the “effective exchange of information,” as the goal may be
building a relationship rather than a transaction of information (Byram,
1997, p. 32; emphasis mine). Moreover, an intercultural identity allows
individuals to engage with another culture by “exploring the ways language
and culture relate to lived realities – the learners’ as well as that of the target
community” (Liddicoat & Scarino, 2013, p. 49). Such individuals under-
stand social meanings in the different cultural contexts they inhabit and
how these social meanings affect them.
Consequently, intercultural individuals understand and enact their
social identities, knowing what is possible and how to realize it through
verbal and nonverbal communication in various cultural settings. They
also understand the values, practices, and behaviors these social roles reflect
in each cultural context (e.g., how is a business executive supposed to
behave in both settings?). At the same time, intercultural individuals also
know that they can accept some values and practices from each culture
with which they affiliate and simultaneously reject others (while being
aware of the possible consequences for flouting expectations).
The process of developing intercultural identity entails making connec-
tions to others who overlap with facets of our identity; such connections
facilitate the emergence of social group identities, and it is social groups
that can help socialize newcomers into their communities. Students return-
ing from study-abroad sojourns have reported finding friendships through
shared passions for karate, board games, or studying an additional L2/Lx.
In addition to modeling a range of socially acceptable behaviors in a new
cultural context, these identity-based social groups allow learners to see
other people as individuals, with whom they share common ground,
although they might at first glance appear different (e.g., different gender,
nationality, profession). This revelation helps reshape learners form cross-
cultural to intercultural individuals. Whereas prior to their travels they
viewed themselves as Americans versus German and Japanese host cultures,
the students realized that national identity was not the most salient aspect
of their encounters. Instead, their social group identities were more rele-
vant and helped them find a ready connection with their peers.
As mentioned in previous chapters, cultures reflect “complex systems of
thought and behavior that people create and perpetuate in and for associa-
tion” (Omaggio-Hadley, 2001, p. 348). Their beliefs, values, and behaviors
are deeply ingrained in the collective consciousness, and in-group members
may hold them as “universal truth,” labeling their preferences “‘human
nature,’ ‘instinct,’ ‘common sense,’ ‘logic,’” while beliefs, values, and
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
280 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
practices “that don’t fit are different, and therefore either illogical,
immoral, nonsensical, or the result of a naïve and inferior stage of devel-
opment of ‘human nature’” (Galloway, 1992, p. 88). Correcting these
assumptions and learning to think of other people and ourselves as
embodying equally valid multiplex identities is essential for developing
intercultural communication.
8
This issue is discussed in further detail at www.languageonthemove.com/explorations-in-language-
shaming/
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 281
learners may be better speakers, while others are more comfortable with
writing. Some learners may have the same level of control over grammar,
lexicon, or pragmatics as an educated native speaker, yet may retain
a ‘nonnative’ accent in the L2. That accent marks them as nonnative
speakers, even when their other competences reflect outstanding L2/Lx
knowledge.
In addition to language skills possibly limiting the variety of and extent
to which learners can express their identities in the L2/Lx, classroom
contexts offer meager opportunities for practicing diverse social roles.
Learners practice language skills but tend to perform solely the social role
of language learner, and often feel infantilized by the input. Due to such
limited sociolinguistic preparation, they are often unable to inhabit the
typical range of social roles they would normally encounter in the real
world, and which actually reflect their identities (e.g., developing common
interest-based friendships, expressing their style of humor, or being socially
and politically active). To foster successful intercultural identity develop-
ment, then, we need to commit to changing learners’ and our own attitude
towards L2/Lx learning outcomes. Both they and we need to realize that
multilingual learners can express a range of identities, albeit imperfectly, in
another language. They are legitimate participants in L2/Lx communica-
tive practices, who can tap into a wealth of multiple linguistic and cultural
repertoires in their intercultural interactions (Dewaele, 2017).
Another possible reason why L2/Lx learners are hesitant about expres-
sing the full palette of identities is that they do not quite know how those
identities are enacted in new cultural contexts. What verbal and nonverbal
communication should be used to express one’s gender or professional
identity? Are all aspects of one’s identity safe to express in a new cultural
context? Will our way of speaking, be it our pronunciation or pragmatic
knowledge, lead to linguistic discrimination (Hua, 2014; Myers-Scotton,
2006)? A former student, who was very interested in learning Arabic,
hesitated signing up for teaching English abroad in Morocco or Jordan,
because he was unsure how, how openly, where, and with whom he would
be able to express his identity as a gay man, an aspect of identity that is
important to him.
In order to help learners navigate complex multilingual realities, they
need opportunities to become aware of possible identities, both avowed
and ascribed, in the L2/Lx-speaking context and learn how to express those
identities verbally and nonverbally. Awareness raising can be accomplished
by presenting cultures from multiple identity perspectives, and not just
from the perspective of members of a dominant cultural group, and as
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
282 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
engendered through social interaction in dynamic ways (Byram, 1997).
Moreover, L2/Lx pedagogy must focus on “equipping learners with the
means of accessing and analysing any cultural practices and meanings they
encounter, whatever their status in a society,” where ‘status’ should refer to
both the language learner and their likely interlocutors (Byram, 1997,
p. 19). Importantly, L2/Lx pedagogy should foster learners’ ability to be
coequal participants in intercultural communicative events, rather than
secondary to the all-knowing native speaker, the arbiter of what is correct.
Learners should not “model themselves on first [and monolingual] speak-
ers, ignoring the significance of the social identities and cultural compe-
tence” of multilingual, multicultural individuals (Byram, 1997, p. 8).
Increased agency (the ability to determine goals for oneself) provides
more flexibility in learners’ trajectory by allowing them to reject certain
parts or varieties of the L2/Lx, if they are aware of the implications of that
language or variety, reflecting “the individual in relation to the social world
and affective dimensions of identity” (Duff, 2012, p. 413).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 283
echoed by Duff (2012), who recommends that instructors reflect on their
identity vis-à-vis the language they teach: “It is important for teachers
and learners to understand their own stances and positionings, and how
these affect their engagement with (or participation in) language educa-
tion” (p. 420).
Both for language instructors and their students, interacting with a wide
range of ‘others’ (i.e., simply other individuals, similar to each other in
some ways, distinct from one another in others) can help clarify one’s own
identity/identities, moving towards the emergence of an intercultural
identity. This process should include active analysis of one’s own identity
vis-à-vis ‘the other’ in ever-expanding circles of self-reflection (Byram,
1997); it also requires an awareness of one’s own identities and those of
one’s interlocutor(s), as well as empathy and adaptability. Moreover, it
requires an awareness not only of one’s own avowed identities, but the way
members of the other culture view the individual: his or her ascribed
identities.
Barkhuizen (2013) recommends self-reflective journaling to explore
identity formation among L2/Lx learners, an approach supported by
the “narrative or discursive turn” (Pavlenko, 2007, p. 164). Such
journals would be ideal for instructors as well, using autobiography
to explore their L2/Lx development and realize how self-reflection
“contributes to shaping participants’ own understandings and devel-
opment through the act of telling their stories and putting shape on
experience” (Ushioda, 2019, p. 204).
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
284 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
• Selecting a smaller speech event, include tasks that help learners recog-
nize the dynamic nature of interaction, how different aspects of identity
might be more or less salient during a communicative event; observe
how participants emphasize and express that aspect fluidly.
• Using the same video or a new one, analyze aspects of politeness and
impoliteness, to understand how language and power are performed by
social actors.
• Explore terms of address (and corresponding lexico-grammatical
features) to identify social roles and relationships among them (e.g.,
how do people address colleagues or express support for friends and
family?).
• Analyze authentic interactions to see how humor is deployed to express
in-group and out-group identities among different social groups.
• Using various authentic materials as input sources, have learners engage
in role-playing, so that they can enact and embody diverse social roles in
the L2/Lx, using verbal and nonverbal communicative tools creatively.
• Analyze values associated with social and interpersonal roles, as well as
cultural groups from multiple perspectives. Learners should develop an
understanding of intracultural variation in the expression of identities.
Include language samples and works from people who come from
communities with little cultural capital.
• Have learners research a community that reflects one of their social
identities (e.g., skateboarders or anime enthusiasts): How does that
community identify itself? What verbal and nonverbal communication
is unique to them (or is used in unique ways by them)? How are they
viewed by other members of the broader cultural community? This
group can be the learners’ entry point into full participation in the L2/
Lx-speaking community.
• Include research projects regarding cultural and social symbols, images,
objects and their connection to identity (e.g., school mascots in the
United States, including the debates surrounding culturally appro-
priated images; religious songs; peace or environmental movements).
• Have students research a social group’s relation to power, both histori-
cally and the implication for the group at the present time in terms of its
multicultural identities (e.g., Italian colonization in the Horn of Africa;
Trento, 2012).
• Incorporate stories written by immigrants to the host culture that
explore the process of identity development in an L2/Lx-speaking
community.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 285
• Invite representatives (native members as well as people who have lived
there as sojourners themselves) of the cultures whose language you are
teaching to talk about their experiences as intercultural individuals.
What challenges have they faced in new cultural contexts and how did
they solve them (if at all)?
• Explore aspects of identity through telecollaborative exchanges, so
learners can understand better the lives enacted and identities embo-
died by a peer group.
• Help learners become more compassionate with their own linguistic
and cultural mistakes using the L2/Lx by inviting guests who are
adapting to the learners’ own cultural context. This can demonstrate
to learners that making mistakes is to be expected, that people can
express their identities in the L2/Lx without perfect language skills, and
improve learners’ own expectations for themselves as they develop their
intercultural identities.
Summary
This chapter explored the multifaceted nature of identity, which we begin
developing at birth and shape throughout our lives via the affiliations we
form with other individuals and groups. Including academic and athletic
organizations, political and professional associations, gender and age,
ethnicity and race, we wear various ‘hats’ in our lifetime, reflecting our
identities. Language and identity are inherently enmeshed, both at the
societal and individual levels, as they relate both to avowed (self-
determined) and ascribed (other-determined) facets to our sense of self.
Learning an L2/Lx provides ideal opportunities for exploring our
identities, as it is often when we reflect on how we are similar to and
different from others that we have the chance to refine our understanding
of who we are. Learning to speak other languages may reflect or add new
facets to our identity, depending on the reasons behind our studying that
language, whether our background leads us to learning it or a new
language results in the development of new identities. In L2/Lx educa-
tion, it is essential to draw both learners’ and instructors’ attention to
facets of identity, and the dynamic nature of identity, that changes over
time and across situations, including based on each of the languages we
enact. For students and instructors, regular opportunities for self-
reflection can improve awareness of the relationship between language
and identity.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
286 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
1. Fill in the image of the prism below with the various identities you
inhabit, then identify which aspects are relatively stable and which ones
are interactional/situational. Do you inhabit different identities in your
L2/Lx? How do you express these identities verbally and nonverbally in
your L1 and L2/Lx?
I am . . .
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
12 Cultures and Identities 287
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 01:58:48, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.013
chapter 13
Chapter Overview
Miscommunication and conflict are inevitable in human interactions.
Managing them can be challenging even when participants share cultural
and linguistic knowledge, but “[w]hen a second language is involved, the
situation may be exacerbated. Our cultural ignorance or ineptness often-
times clutters our ability to communicate appropriately, effectively, and
adaptively across cultural and linguistic lines” (Ting-Toomey, 2012, p.
279). Our limited language skills may make us misunderstand someone’s
utterances or prevent us from being able to express what we wish to. We
may miss the implications of linguistic and cultural practices or employ
disparate communication styles. Alternately, there may be external reasons
for conflict, that make interaction problematic.
The causes, contexts, and resolutions for conflict are numerous, and we
need to recognize that miscommunication is not a matter of ‘if’ but ‘when’ –
it is a regular feature of intercultural existence, and learning to accept and
manage it is part of intercultural communicative competence. To foster this
competence, this chapter examines communication accommodation for
managing miscommunication, causes of conflict, and possible paths towards
resolving it. The discussion closes with an examination of what it means to
become a more effective intercultural communicator.
288
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 289
modification among speakers of different varieties of English or languages,
such as Welsh (Giles, 2016). Since its inception, Giles has expanded CAT
to include a broader range of adjustments language users adopt in a
coordinated fashion, both cognitively and affectively (Gasiorek, 2016;
Giles, 2016; Ireland & Pennebaker, 2010). During interactions, we may
try to align our utterances to match those of our interlocutors in order to
express affiliation or accentuate differences, in order to create psychological
distance from them. Our attempts at alignment or distancing in verbal and
nonverbal communication, which may or may not be successful, fall into
patterns of convergence, over- and under-accommodation, divergence,
and maintenance (Gasiorek, 2016).
Convergence
The first pattern, convergence, refers to accommodation where speakers align
their verbal and nonverbal communication to be more similar to others
(Dragojevic & Giles, 2014; Myers-Scotton, 2006). Convergence typically
highlights similarities and solidarity between speakers by emphasizing shared
identities. For example, surfers of different nationalities may emphasize their
common athletic interest, making that aspect of their identity most salient
facilitating interaction even in the absence of a shared linguistic code.
Successful communication accommodation is an essential component of
politeness as well, because it improves positive perceptions among participants,
increasing mutual intelligibility and a sense of interpersonal involvement
(Dragojevic & Giles, 2014). Convergent behavior is mostly subconscious,
although participants may also accommodate intentionally (Giles, 2016). A
multilingual instructor of English as a Second Language in Santa Cruz, for
example, regularly helps her students by switching to their native French,
Spanish, or Arabic to help clarify lexical items or answer questions they might
have. This allows the learners to understand the course content better, and also
makes them recognize that their languages serve as useful resources.
Several features of communication adjustment are pertinent to
accommodation:
1. Adjustments can be made when personal variables are more salient
(interpersonal accommodation) or when social group characteristics
are more salient (intergroup accommodation), but when interlocutors
do not know each other, group categories tend to be more salient.
Learning to see individual variation – especially similarities – is key to
building relationships (Dragojevic & Giles, 2014; Giles, 2016). For
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
290 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
example, a student from South Africa may become friends with a
fellow student from Morocco (intergroup similarity) during an intern-
ship program in Japan, because they are both studying biomedical
engineering, which gives them a shared interest (interpersonal
similarity).
2. Self-disclosures by different interlocutors can identify interpersonal
similarities and improve the interactants’ attitudes towards each other,
thus facilitating communication (Gasiorek, 2016). A Chilean student
studying abroad in South Korea can connect with peers who enjoy K-
Pop by wearing t-shirts that reveal his interest in this musical genre.
3. Interpersonal and intergroup salience is dynamic and depends on the
participants’ attitudes, the topic of conversation, among other factors.
In a single online chat with my siblings, for example, I may check to
see how they are doing (sibling identity salience), how our parents are
(child identity), then ask my brother, a doctor, for medical advice
(professional identity).
4. In-group and out-group identities are only partially self-determined,
since others also ascribe identities to us, which may overlap with our
perceptions or differ from them (Coupland, 2007). Self-avowed and
other-ascribed social identities may prompt (appropriate or mistaken)
communication adjustments. For example, a seventy-five-year old
woman might view herself as spry and healthy, but if a younger person
considers her old and frail, they may miscommunicate until and unless
their perceptions become better aligned.
While we regularly synchronize adjustments in interaction in order to
accommodate our interlocutors, sometimes our attempts are not successful.
This is often the case when we are speaking an L2/Lx, since it takes time to
build the necessary skills to be able to adjust our language to local contexts.
Language proficiency level, exposure to and experience with the L2/Lx in
diverse contexts all impact learners’ ability to adjust their language, and no
matter how much they might want to accommodate their interlocutor, they
might not be able to do so. In other cases, non-accommodation may be
irrespective of language proficiency and rather reflects the participants’
attitudes towards their interlocutors.
Non-Accommodation
When adjustments are unsuccessful, they may result in overaccommoda-
tion, underaccommodation, maintenance, or divergence. The source of
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 291
overaccommodation is often stereotyped thinking about another cultural
group, essentializing (i.e., reducing) members of that group to a simplistic
prototype that accentuates intergroup differences and intragroup simila-
rities (Dragojevic & Giles, 2014). Overaccommodation can take the form
of overly simplified grammatical structures and lexicon, unnecessary repe-
titions, a reduced speech rate, or exaggerated phonological articulation
(Hummert & Ryan, 1996). Native-speakers sometimes deploy overaccom-
modative behaviors, when they perceive a nonnative interactant as being
less capable than the individual actually is, which behavior often comes
across as patronizing and infantilizing (Giles & Gasiorek, 2011).
In contrast to overaccommodation, underaccommodation refers to
insufficient adjustment, when it would be warranted. Discussing private
concerns when an interlocutor would prefer not to be included or inatten-
tive and disrespectful behavior, dismissiveness, impolite or indifferent
attitudes may all characterize underaccommodation (Dixon, Schell,
Giles, & Drogos, 2008; Hummert, 2012). Underaccommodation, like
overaccommodation, may be intentional or inadvertent. If two interactants
have different approaches to turn-taking or using backchanneling devices,
and neither knows how to adjust his or her discourse management style to
match that of the other participant, they inadvertently underaccommodate
each other (Palomares, Giles, Soliz, & Gallois, 2016). If done intentionally,
underaccommodation may express the speakers’ desire to distance them-
selves from their interlocutors. Jones, Sheeran, Lanyon, Evans, and
Martincovic (2018), for example, report on nurses’ interactions with min-
ority patients at an Australian hospital, where they cut off young mothers’
questions and adopted an overly reserved, professional stance (attitude
towards the interaction) towards the patients’ parents, if they had an
unpleasant experience with them early on in the interaction. In this case,
underaccommodation had real ramifications, resulting in lower quality
patient care, as well as disparate health experiences and outcomes.
Especially if they occur repeatedly, over- and under-accommodation can
lead to “a range of adverse consequences . . . including misunderstandings,
communication breakdown, and negative evaluations of both interactions
and speakers” (Gasiorek & Dragojevic, 2017, p. 276).
Slightly different in potential outcome, divergence and maintenance may
be negative, neutral, or positive types of non-accommodation. Since verbal
and nonverbal communication reflect important aspects of social identity, we
often maintain communication features when interacting with others to mark
in-group identity or to make out-group distinctions more salient (Dragojevic
& Giles, 2014; Tajfel & Turner, 1986). The results can be innocuous, such as
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
292 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
women and men retaining distinct gender-related language patterns on social
media (Bamman, Eisentein, & Schoebelen, 2014) without endangering com-
munication. However, emphasizing divergent verbal and nonverbal behavior
when interacting with others who cannot use that variety or language can lead
to impolite or hostile interactions (Dragojevic & Giles, 2014). Conversely,
maintenance – of the L1 within an L2/Lx context, for example – may even be
perceived positively. Giving a workshop in Nigeria, Ngũgı̃ wa Thiong’o
(2010), an award-winning Kenyan author, asked his students to translate drafts
that they had originally written in English into their L1s. He was hoping that
maintaining their L1s in the face of the dominance of English in academic
contexts (Lx) would help counter what he calls “linguistic feudalism,” and
allow students to let go of their perceptions that English is the only legitimate
code in a context rich with linguistic diversity (p. 2).
Societal Non-Accommodation
In addition to specific individuals’ ability to moderate communication,
historical and geopolitical forces help shape language, identity, and the
willingness to accommodate others. These forces often serve to create
psychological distance (i.e., subjective barriers) and establish, maintain,
and confirm existing social dynamics, almost always benefitting those in
power (Myers-Scotton, 2006). Examples include using English in school in
Nigeria at the exclusion of indigenous languages or giving preference to one
regional dialect over another (e.g., hiring only speakers of Parisian instead of
Tunisian French) to perpetuate language-based social exclusion. These
historical and geopolitical forces play out in everyday interactions, when
speakers emphasize their differences in an attempt to undermine commu-
nication or force others to accommodate them. In one such incident, on
June 14, 2018, a woman wearing a t-shirt with the flag of Puerto Rico on it
was celebrating her birthday in a park in Illinois with her family and friends,
when a white man approached her, then verbally assaulted and physically
threatened her because of her clothing and use of Spanish.1 His anger was
triggered by the young woman’s support of a contextually nondominant
culture and language. Policies and everyday incidents that prioritize one
language over another can reinforce in-group and out-group thinking, often
relying on the (incorrect) argument that certain languages or language
varieties are less desirable, as are the people who speak those languages and
varieties. Such beliefs are grounded in stereotyped and prejudicial thinking.
1
www.cnn.com/2018/07/10/us/illinois-puerto-rico-park-officer/index.html
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 293
Stereotypes and Prejudices
We learn stereotypes – widely held beliefs about groups of people (e.g.,
millennials, Eastern Europeans) – from a variety of sources, such as our
families or society (Abrams, 2002). Stereotypes, for the most part, are
negative, showing members of another group as being less desirable in
some way, be it because of their beliefs, behaviors, or other traits. With
stereotyped thinking we ascribe specific characteristics – positive or nega-
tive – to an entire cultural group and essentialize its members to those
characteristics. Even purported positive stereotypes can be detrimental. For
example, a common stereotype in the United States is the ‘model minor-
ity,’ according to which “Asians comprise the racial minority group that
has ‘made it’ in America through hard work and education, and therefore
serve as a model for other racial minorities to follow” (Wing, 2007, p. 455).
This stereotype can make students invisible, as Wing (2007) found in a
study of high school students in Berkeley, California. Asian students there
were regularly viewed as a homogeneous group of aggregate data, instead of
reflecting the diverse cultural heritages they actually represented (e.g.,
Korean, Thai, Japanese, Chinese). Differences in the students’ socioeco-
nomic and social situations were also ignored, and students who lived in
poverty, did not speak English as their L1, were first in their families to
complete high school, and were struggling to meet college eligibility
requirements had limited access to much-needed educational resources
(Wing, 2007). Consequently, the students reported feeling anxious, in part
due to their own pressure of having to live up to the stereotype of the
‘model minority.’
While stereotypes are generalized cognitive beliefs about cultural groups,
prejudices are “unfavorable affective reactions to or evaluations of groups
and their members,” although both may lead to interpersonal or institu-
tional discrimination, which is defined as “differential treatment by indi-
viduals toward some groups and their members relative to other groups
and their members . . . [and] policies and contexts that create, enact, reify,
and maintain inequality” (The APA Resolution on Prejudice, Stereotypes, and
Discrimination, 20062). The consequences of prejudice include microag-
gressions (subtle verbal, nonverbal, and/or visual insults based on some-
one’s identity), limited opportunities for interaction by newcomers to a
community, unfair hiring practices, intentional miscommunication, or, in
extreme cases, violence or genocide. Prejudices can be based on a single
2
www.apa.org/about/policy/prejudice.pdf
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
294 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
encounter with a member of another culture or not even require personal
experience with that culture. Although quick to establish, prejudices are
difficult to eradicate, because they are mostly subconscious and get
continually reiterated in humor, politics, and other cultural texts (e.g.,
movies, graphic art). That stereotypes and prejudices are persistent,
Brislin (2000) argues, is due in part to the cognitive and affective
functions they serve.
First, they can serve a utilitarian function. Sharing the prejudices of one’s
family or friends helps retain in-group membership with them, while
opposing their views may lead to taunts or rejection. It also serves an ego-
defensive function. Holding prejudices about other people allows us to
protect our sense of self, by providing reasons external to us for not
succeeding at something. If I believe that ‘I did not get this job, because
person X got special treatment,’ I do not have to admit that I am not as
qualified for the job as another person. The third function is value-
expressive. Prejudices can provide us with consistency across our values,
norms, practices, and behaviors. Typically shared across larger social
groups, value-based prejudices make us believe that our religion, political
view, or other long-held beliefs or practices are correct, while we ridicule
the perspectives of another group and deem them inferior. Taken to the
extreme, such prejudices may lead to unfavorable government policies,
ethnic cleansing, or even genocide. Finally, prejudices fulfill a knowledge
function. They can help organize and categorize experiences that we accu-
mulate over our lifetime, a process that takes considerable time and energy;
this knowledge system helps prevent stimulus overload and speeds up
decision-making. Consequently, we may not wish to give up our views
too readily, so instead we describe those who hold different views as
ignorant or deficient in some way. Notably, prejudices can serve multiple
functions simultaneously.
While most training on intercultural communication addresses the
knowledge function, the other three functions are just as important,
especially since they reflect complex emotional connections to our world-
view. These prejudices guide the assumptions we make about other people
and our attitudes towards them. Their appearance, accent, ethnic back-
ground, or religious affiliation impacts our interactions with them. We
may decide not to talk to others because we are afraid of them (which can
result in either their or our own isolation) or we may use slower language
because we believe that they will not understand us. Prejudices also shape
sociopolitical power: How much do we ask immigrants to change to ‘fit
in’? To what extent are they able and allowed to hold on to their established
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 295
identities (e.g., Are they welcome to celebrate their festivals in our com-
munity? Are they encouraged to maintain and speak their L1?).
Reciprocally, how we believe others will perceive us also influences our
interactions, informing what and how we reveal about ourselves and which
aspects of our identities we wish to highlight (Martin & Nakayama, 2018).
Changing stereotyped and prejudicial beliefs requires conscious decisions
from all parties – those in power and those who are not; until those changes
are implemented, such beliefs are a significant source of intercultural
conflict.
Sources of Conflict
Human beings sometimes misunderstand or misattribute intentions and
make mistakes in what they say and how. If these misunderstandings are
minor and have no long-lasting consequences, they usually go unnoticed.
Given the complexity of value-laden human emotions, language, and
history, it is inevitable, however, that some misunderstandings rise to the
level of conflict (Mortensen, 2006). Conflict arises “when people have
incompatible expectations, values, norms, interests, or goals in their inter-
actions” (Dai & Chen, 2017, p. 1). Conflict may range from short-term,
mild incompatibilities to severe, ongoing rifts that have devastating effects
on individuals, communities, or nation-states across the world. Conflict
also varies by participants (Abigail & Cahn, 2011; Gudykunst, 2005;
Jackson, 2014):
• intrapersonal conflict: is experienced by one individual (e.g., choosing
whether to adhere to local gender expectations or maintain one’s own
preferences);
• interpersonal conflict: occurs between two or more individuals, when
personal preferences and characteristics are more relevant (e.g., indivi-
duals with different relationship goals);
• intracultural conflict: transpires within one cultural group (e.g.,
debates regarding a country changing its laws to allow women to drive);
• intercultural conflict: arises between two or more cultural groups,
when group membership is highly salient (e.g., a community protesting
the arrival of refugees, whose values they feel they do not share).
Regardless of its duration or participant-structure, resolving conflict
requires us to make sense of complex interactions and identify its causes
accurately. These causes include perceived or real threats to in-group
identity and security or incompatible expectations in one or more of the
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
296 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
following areas (Dai & Chen, 2017; Martin & Nakayama, 2018; Jackson,
2014; Oetzel & Ting-Toomey, 2013; Wilmot & Hocker, 2007; Worchel,
2005):
• Values: different ideologies pertaining to specific issues, such as send-
ing children to a religious or a public school; value conflicts can be
particularly challenging to solve.
• Norms: mismatched expectations for behavior, for example removing
shoes upon entering a house or living in coeducational dorms when it is
not common practice for someone.
• Face-orientations: different preferences for prioritizing one’s own
needs and image (likely in individualistic societies/individuals) versus
those of others (more collectivistic societies/individuals tend to prior-
itize the needs of a social group or one’s interlocutor).
• Resources: misaligned access to or allocation of shared resources (e.g.,
natural or financial assets), such as using funds for public education
versus the military-industrial complex.
• Goals: disparate or incompatible objectives for an event or action, for
example, when a group of students gets together after class to study, but
some participants socialize instead.
• Affective orientations: feelings and emotions are incompatible, for
example notions of who is considered to be a friend as opposed to an
acquaintance or what kinds of behaviors are expected and accepted of
friends.
• Interests: incompatible preferences for a course of action or plan;
Native American tribes opposed the construction of an oil pipeline
through tribal lands in the United States, while the government sup-
ported the project, citing national energy and economic security.3
• Cognition/Perceptions: incongruent interpretations of an event, or the
intentions or language of another person (e.g., for Person A, having
coffee is just to get to know Person B, while Person B views the
interaction as a job-interview).
In addition to these sources of conflict, it is important to understand the
role that our conflict style – the way we engage in and resolve disagree-
ments – might play in reducing or escalating conflict.
3
www.reuters.com/article/us-usa-pipeline-keystone-explainer/what-is-at-stake-in-the-keystone-xl-pi
peline-ruling-idUSKCN1NE23A?il=0
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 297
Conflict Styles
Humans engage in conflict in a variety of styles – accommodation, avoid-
ance, compromise, collaboration, or competition – depending on cultural
and personal factors. Ting-Toomey and Oetzel (2001) define conflict styles
as “patterned responses to conflict” (p. 45), reflecting divergent levels of
cooperativeness and competitiveness. Accommodation (high cooperative-
ness, low competitiveness) helps maintain harmony among participants,
although one party likely has to give up his or her position or needs and
accede to their interlocutor. If participants prefer to withdraw from a
situation rather than engage in what may be construed as conflict, they
are showing a preference for avoidance strategies (in this case, possibly
neither party achieves their goals). Collaboration (high cooperativeness,
high competitiveness) may generate a range of choices for problem-solving,
achieving a win-win outcome for all parties involved, whereas competing
(low cooperativeness, high competitiveness) has a zero-sum orientation (a
win-lose perspective) among participants. The fifth style, compromising,
tries to find a solution to problems and conflicts that is at least somewhat
acceptable to all parties and tries to maintain a relationship; each party will
likely have to give up some of their expectations. Although this model has
been used for decades in business and academia,4 with occasional mod-
ifications of the labels (Rahim, 1983; Rubin, Pruitt, & Kim, 1994) and
categories (Wilmot & Hocker, 2010), it is heavily weighted in favor of
Western values of individualism. Consequently, a new model has been
developed that better reflects values and beliefs across broader cultural
contexts (Hammer, 2003, 2005, 2009). This model is designed as a four-
way matrix, reflecting varying degrees of directness and affect, as Table 13.1
demonstrates.
As this table indicates, each style combines aspects of directness and
affect. The discussion style entails direct communication, with relatively low
emotional expression (Hammer, 2005). The focus tends to be on problem-
solving and factual information. Low power distance, individualistic cul-
tures – such as Northern/Western Europeans or European-Americans,
Australians, and New Zealanders – tend to prefer this style, which empha-
sizes explicit verbal communication (Dai & Chen, 2017).
The engagement style also reflects a preference for directness, but with
strong affective involvement (Hammer, 2005). Substantive issues are
4
www.talent.wisc.edu/home/HideATab/LeadershipManagementDevelopment/ConflictResolution/
AboutConflict/ConflictStyles/tabid/228/Default.aspx
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
298 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Table 13.1 Intercultural conflict styles
(‘Say what you mean and mean (‘Tell me how you really feel’)
what you say’)
Indirect
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 299
The University of Calgary compiled the characteristics of each
dimension in Hammer’s model, to help recognize each conflict style
in action (see Table 13.2).5 Although cultures might have a general
preference for one conflict style over another (Martin & Nakayama,
2018), individuals often differ from the central tendency. For example,
while my friend Rina is from Cyprus, where the preferred trend is the
engagement style, she mostly uses the discussion style, due to her
personality and extensive exposure to the U.S. academic context,
among other factors. Emphasizing that it is “individuals [who] employ
culturally learned strategies for dealing with disagreements and emo-
tional responses to conflictual interaction” (Hammer, 2005, p. 692;
emphasis mine) that helps prevent stereotyped and prejudiced think-
ing, which are key sources of conflict.
Direct Indirect
Meaning is ‘inside’ the verbal message Meaning ‘outside’ the verbal message
Precise and explicit language use Ambiguous and vague language use
Reliance on face-to-face resolution of Reliance on third parties for resolution of
disagreements disagreements
Speaking your mind Discretion in voicing goals
Verbally assert differences of opinion ‘Talk around’ disagreements
Persuasion through reasoned argument Persuasion through facework
Substantive disagreement focus Relationship repair focus
Emotionally restrained Emotionally expressive
Disguised display of emotions Overt displays of emotion
Control emotions by ‘internalizing’ Control emotions by ‘externalizing’
Minimal display of feelings through Visible displays of feelings through
nonverbal behavior nonverbal behavior
Constrained vocalization Expansive vocalization
Sensitive to hurting feelings of Sensitive to constraints on expressing
other party feelings
Relational trust through emotional Relational trust through emotional
maturity commitment
Emotional suppression necessary for Emotional information is necessary for
credibility credibility
5
www.ucalgary.ca/ssc/files/ssc/intercultural-conflict-styles-general.pdf
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
300 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Conflict Management and Resolution
Conflict resolution is a complex process, but it can be an excellent oppor-
tunity for improving communication skills and understanding, and
strengthening relationships (Dai & Chen, 2017). In ideal situations, con-
flict can “serve as an impetus for individual and social changes that need to
be made” (Broome, 2017, p. 14). Also ideally, this process would entail
sustained dialog, leading to harmony, which Broome (2017) defines as:
a generative and creative process in which diverse elements are brought
together to form a complex and inclusive world . . . [where] various elements
enrich one another by forming a relationship in which they mutually
compensate for one another’s short-comings; mutually reinforce one
another’s strengths; and mutually advance each other’s path toward fulfill-
ment . . . [through] a continuous process of . . . give and take. (p. 16)
Reconsidering conflict as a potential source of growth and positive
change, people experiencing it may be more willing to expend the neces-
sary patience, perseverance, kindness, openness, and acceptance that is
required to resolve it. The more serious a conflict situation is, the more
effort and collaboration is required for its resolution (Mortensen, 2006),
but finding shared moral norms and principles – such as human dignity,
equality, ethics of care, justice, sincerity, tolerance, and mutual responsi-
bility – facilitates the process (Broome, 2017; Chen, 2015; Dai & Chen,
2017; Jia & Lai Jia, 2017; Ojelabi, 2010). Whether conflict is resolved
depends on a number of variables:
• Is there a strong, existing relationship among the participants?
• Will the relationship (need to) continue?
• How important is it to resolve the conflict?
• Can the conflict be resolved?
With strong existing relationships, such as solid friendships or positive
collegial ties, participants may not only be more willing to negotiate
conflict situations, but may have better tools to do so, including knowledge
of the other’s conflict styles, shared interpretive frameworks for under-
standing the sources of conflict, and a common language. Participants also
need to consider the second point: Does the relationship need to continue?
Naturally, participants who have to live or work together (e.g., roommates
or students collaborating on a project) do not have the luxury of walking
away from conflict and have to find a resolution. In contrast, in the case of
temporary relationships (e.g., seat-mates in an airplane), conflict might not
even have to be resolved: it just dissipates in a little while.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 301
Another aspect of conflict resolution is the significance of the problem.
If a traveler’s safety is jeopardized in an airplane (e.g., a child seated next to
a drunk passenger), the need for a solution is high, unlike when the issue is
a mere want (e.g., I would prefer to sit with my friend rather than a
stranger). Finally, examining the causes of the conflict and the participants’
willingness to find a mutually acceptable solution, we can determine
whether the conflict can be resolved at all. In a particular cultural context,
can a student talk to the professor about an exam he or she did poorly on?
Can two international students state clear expectations for house rules in
order to avoid conflict? Is there an opportunity to retrain employees to
bring their interaction styles in line with company practices? Sometimes we
can take a step away from the conflict, reframe or reduce an emotional
engagement, and find a compromise solution that is (at least minimally)
acceptable to all parties involved. Solutions are more elusive for large-scale,
global conflict, that has been shaped by long-term historical forces.
Resolving violent conflict (typically arising between ethnic and religious
cultural groups in the same geographic region) requires extraordinary
measures, and attempts often remain unsuccessful (see the conflict in
Syria, the refugee crisis in Europe, etc.). Such conflicts require facilitated
intergroup dialog (Broome, 2004; Obika & Ovuga, 2018) and sustained
peacebuilding (Martin & Nakayama, 2018). Attempts at resolving conflict
at this level must start with members of the community, leaders who are
willing to sit down with their counterpoints from the opposing side and
identify a collective vision for the future, and developing specific action
plans to achieve effective cooperation/collaboration. The conflict resolu-
tion process must acknowledge disparate power relationships and the
relative privilege of each group; it also requires great awareness, under-
standing, empathy, and sensitivity (Broome & Collier, 2012). Such conflict
and its resolutions are beyond the scope of the present volume. However,
we can each strive towards resolving miscommunication and conflict in our
own lives, whether their source is interpersonal or intercultural
misalignment.
Chen and Starosta (2005), Jackson (2014), Moran, Abramson, and
Moran (2014), and Martin and Nakayama (2018) offer several suggestions
for promoting effective conflict resolution and engaging in cooperative
interaction:
1. Identify the causes of the conflict – and the social context in which it
is situated – from the perspectives of all participants. If appropriate,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
302 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
conduct research and ask questions to obtain a more complete
understanding of the issue and the context.
2. Look for common ground and shared goals with your interlocutor(s);
go beyond ‘us’ versus ‘them’ and think interpersonally, not inter-
culturally. Develop empathy and sensitivity towards each other, and
act in cooperation.
3. Listen attentively, especially in situations where participants do not
share the same L1; patience and collaboration can help clarify every-
one’s messages to mutual benefit.
4. Be cognizant of issues of power and privilege during interactions
(being able to use your L1 during conflict is a type of privilege).
5. Focus on the topic at hand, without resorting to ad hominem attacks
or hyperbolic language and de-escalate the situation, if possible.
6. Check in with trusted members of other cultural groups to explore
your understanding of communication and conflict styles, which
reflect both personal and cultural preferences. Be open to learning
new communication and conflict styles and expanding your
repertoire.
7. Tap into and develop your sense of humor, which can promote a
willingness from everyone to let go of negative feelings – admit and
learn from your mistakes.
8. Find creative solutions that combine elements of all perspectives in a
way that everyone can accept: What solution will work interperson-
ally or interculturally? How do we implement it?
9. Ask third-party mediators as needed; reconciling divergent views may
be difficult when the topics are sensitive, such as racial relations,
abortion rights, or when the interactants’ language skills are insuffi-
cient to handle the topic well.
10. Understand that conflict does not have to end relationships. Be
willing to forgive; anger and grudges take up exorbitant amounts of
energy, whereas forgiveness promotes cooperation and peaceful
relationships.
These suggestions inherently reflect skills each of us can develop for
improved intercultural competence. However, a few points merit further
reflection. First, in some instances conflict resolution is not possible, and
participants must walk away from the situation; if individuals are in danger,
they should not feel compelled to stay in contact with their opponent (e.g.,
a stalker). Knowing when conflict can and cannot be resolved is just as
important as knowing how to engage in the resolution process. Second,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 303
people are not limited to their preferred conflict style; they can and should
practice different styles during low-stakes situations, when there is no
actual conflict, when one’s sense of self is not at risk, or when there are
friendly ‘opponents’ available to try out new skills. Equally useful is
observing how members of the in-group or more experienced members
of the out-group manage conflict, learning from them how to use conflict
styles that are not yet familiar or comfortable.
The final recommendation in the above list calls for practicing forgive-
ness, a process that begins with acknowledging another person’s or group’s
feelings of hurt, anger, disappointment, and the need for healing (Zhang,
Ting-Toomey, & Oetzel, 2014). Waldron and Kelley (2008) and Kelley,
Waldron, and Kloeber (2018) recommend practicing forgiveness by (a)
learning to identify when remembering is helpful (to avoid repeating a
mistake) and when forgetting might be more appropriate (a one-time
mistake); (b) recognizing tensions between strong affective responses and
the cognitive, intellectual aspects of conflict; (c) balancing trust and risk
(e.g., rebuilding trust while reducing potential future risk in the
relationship); and (d) considering mercy versus justice, letting go of
angry and hostile feelings, such as vengeance and retribution.
Forgiveness benefits from kind and other-oriented communication,
where the transgressor is still seen to be worthy of friendship, caring, and
respect (Kelley, Waldron, & Kloeber, 2018). The offender contributes to
this process by asking for forgiveness, apologizing, and making some kind
of restitution (e.g., volunteer work, financial compensation). Romig and
Veenstra (1998) highlight the social aspect of forgiveness: “it is an inter-
personal process that represents a possible response to wounds or injustices
encountered in a relational context” (p. 188). However, victims can practice
unilateral forgiveness, letting go of feelings of hurt or anger, without
expecting anything of the perpetrator, so that they are not consumed by
their negative feelings, and cycles of violence may be broken (Obika &
Ovuga, 2018). Participants’ resilience and psychosocial strength may influ-
ence their ability to forgive and the path they take towards forgiveness
(Poston, Hanson, Schwiebert, 2012).
As the preceding discussion illustrated, conflict resolution begins with
an individual’s or social group’s willingness and ability to adapt, to find
common ground with, and accommodate towards others. This process also
requires that the parties commit to mutual nurturance and respect, to
develop social sensitivity and reciprocity with each other, and to be honest
(Haslett, 2017; Mortensen, 2006). Mistakes are opportunities for develop-
ing situationally appropriate knowledge regarding what works and what
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
304 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
does not, so that we can better align our interests with others in future
instances of conflict. From this perspective,
[conflict resolution] may be envisioned as a quest, a journey, and an arduous
process of constant probing, questioning, and testing generated by all the
tensions, contradictions, triumphs, and tragedies of everyday life . . . and
[m]aking sense of productive social outcomes requires the capacity and
willingness to put certain matters to the test of direct exposure to multiple
perspectives and alternative standards of critical personal assessment . . . In
solving problems, we test our own ability and willingness to compare
possibilities, to discriminate between good or bad decisions, to tolerate
better or worse outcomes, and to improve our condition by making selective
choices that enable us to feel better or avoid feeling worse (Mortensen, 2006,
pp. 26–28).
In addition to societal or group-level initiatives, individuals can also
strive to resolve conflict and contribute to peace through citizen diplomacy.
International organizations in support of such endeavors (e.g., the Center
for Citizen Diplomacy) connect people who share a vision of “a world that
thrives because of true global engagement, where individuals and commu-
nities benefit from person-to-person interactions that result in greater
understanding between people and cultures” (www.centerforcitizendiplo
macy.org/).
Projects that embrace this vision, but without a formal organization, are
also possible. In 2012, Ronny Edry,6 an Israeli graphic designer, started a
poster campaign (see Figure 13.1) to work towards peace. Specifically, he
wanted to demonstrate that even though the governments of Israel and
Iran were in conflict, individuals could extend kindness and friendship to
one another. What started out as a single poster by one man became a
positive, affiliative social movement – The Peace Factory7 – as he explains
in his TED Talk.8 Acts like this that start locally can shape international
relationships.
6
https://archive.org/details/RonnyEdry_2012X 7 https://thepeacefactory.org/israel-loves-iran/
8
www.google.com/search?q=ronny+edry+ted+talk&oq=ronny+eddry+&aqs=chrome.1.69i57j0
l5.6127j0j7&sourceid=chrome&ie=UTF-8
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 305
about the current state of our planet and the quality of life of future
generations . . . [who] should work together to solve the major problems
facing humanity” (p. 299). Social responsibility (concern for others), global
competence (open-mindedness, striving for mutual understanding with
others), and global civic engagement (e.g., volunteering or political acti-
vism beyond national boundaries) are core elements of global citizenship.
To reach this level of intercultural competence, individuals need to develop
intercultural sensitivity (Bennett, 2017), which rests on the assumption
that “as one’s experiences of cultural difference become more sophisticated,
one’s competence in intercultural relations increases” (Bennett & Bennett,
2004, p. 152). Movement between two end stages of a continuum, ethno-
centrism and ethnorelativism, frames the developmental model shown in
Figure 13.2, with Bennett and Bennett’s version on the left, and Hammer’s
(2012) version on the right.
Ethnocentrism refers to a monocultural worldview in which one’s own
culture is the filter through which all values, perceptions, behaviors, and
communication are processed. Individuals at this stage are not curious about
other cultures and are not ready to engage with cultural differences or
similarities. Any discussion of variation relies on stereotyped images of the
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
306 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
Ethnocentrism
Intercultural
Mindset
integration
adaptation
adaptation
acceptance
Ethnorelativism
Monocultural
minimization
Mindset
defense polarization
denial
Figure 13.2 Models of intercultural development (adapted from Bennett & Bennett,
2004 and Hammer, 2012)
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 307
an interculturally competent person who is “comfortable with many
standards and customs and to having an ability to adapt behavior and
judgment to a variety of interpersonal settings” (Bennett, 1993, p. 26).
Although progression often includes backward sliding (e.g., as a con-
sequence of culture shock) and forward lurches (Stuart, 2012), the further
one moves along the developmental stages, the better they should be able to
understand patterns of interaction both from their own perspective and
from the perspective of the culture they are visiting or in which they live or
work, “shifting cultural perspective and adapting behavior to cultural
context” (Hammer, 2009, p. 5). Progression from the monocultural,
ethnocentric end of the continuum towards the intercultural, ethnorelative
one often parallels the development of L2/Lx competence as well, since
better linguistic skills allow individuals to interact with people and texts (of
all kinds) from another culture and thus broaden their perspectives
(Bennett, 2012; Jackson, 2014).
The nature of the interculturally competent individual has long been of
interest to social scientists and applied linguists. The former group tends to
emphasize aspects of cultural knowledge (often at the exclusion of lan-
guage), while the latter accentuates language without sufficient attention to
its cultural context (Jackson, 2014). Ideally, however, culture and language
exist in synergy, mutually reinforcing each other. From this perspective
emerge several personal traits and skills that define competent global
citizens, a view supported by global organizations (e.g., the International
Red Cross, Oxfam, the Stanley Foundation) and scholarship (Allwright,
2006; Byram & Zarate, 1997; Chen & Starosta, 2008; Donatelli, Yngve,
Miller, & Ellis, 2005; Feuerstein & Mandelzis, 2017; Jackson, 2014; Jandl,
2013; Martin & Nakayama, 2018; Patel, Li, & Sooknanan, 2011). Thus, an
interculturally competent individual
• recognizes the interconnectedness of the world (i.e., raising awareness,
seeking knowledge from beyond traditional, local, national news
sources);
• is motivated to interact with others who differ from him or her
culturally (e.g., by befriending fellow students, colleagues, neighbors
from other cultural backgrounds; studying, working, or volunteering in
programs in different cultural settings);
• focuses on individuals (not stereotyped groups) as representing complex
feelings, thoughts, identities, personalities, histories, and motivations;
• listens carefully and respects diverse perspectives, values, practices – not
seeing them as a threat, but as a richness in the world (NB: you do not
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
308 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
have to adopt all perspectives, values, and practices or respect them
equally);
• considers events and other people in a nonjudgmental way; learns to
observe each from multiple perspectives (cognitive flexibility) and to
describe events without assigning affective or evaluative judgments to
them;
• learns to observe, identify, and achieve goals set forth by all participants
in an interaction to everyone’s benefit;
• seeks feedback (asks others, such as a trusted member of the in-group or
a more experienced member of the out-group, for suggestions for
improving communication or behavior);
• develops intercultural communication skills:
o L2/Lx communication skills (both verbal and nonverbal, the lin-
guistic code, and how language is used in social situations),
o empathy,
o self-awareness (e.g., recognizing anxiety in new situations),
o personal strength (sense of self),
o tolerance of ambiguity (learning to be comfortable with uncer-
tainty, especially while one’s level of L2/Lx competence is still low),
o anticipation of potential sources of misunderstanding and the
ability to negotiate or mediate those,
o psychological adaptability (to new situations),
o background knowledge about one’s own and other cultures (e.g.,
history, political, and social contexts, see Chapter 9), using multiple
reliable sources of information;
• develops the ability to live in cultural situations other than one’s own
childhood culture (e.g., having good coping skills, such as identifying
positive things about the environment, rather than focusing on negative
aspects);
• strives for ethical practices, such as social justice and equality, linguistic
rights, human rights, environmental protection, and sustainability; and
• works towards peaceful coexistence around the globe.
These suggestions – and the process of becoming an interculturally com-
petent global citizen – require a genuine commitment to personal growth,
which can be challenging. Letting go of the familiar and pushing the
boundaries of what is possible takes courage. This process also requires
patience, since it takes years to learn an additional language, and we must
make a real effort to get to know members of a host community. In order to
develop into an intercultural being, we have to be willing to ‘put ourselves
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 309
out there,’ to take risks, to explore beyond our comfort zone, and to learn
to think entirely differently from our current mindset. Learning to be
compassionate with ourselves and others during this process and being
willing to adapt to new people and situations will make the intercultural
journey much easier and more enjoyable. Ideally, this intercultural journey
leads to the emergence of intercultural beings, based on “mutual acknowl-
edgement and mutual respect through a process in which people with
differing cultural identities learn from each other, develop intercultural
personhood, and mediate between different perspectives . . . [and who]
reduce cultural distance, negotiate shared meanings and mutually desired
identities, and establish reciprocal relationships” (Dai, 2017, p. 86). This
process recognizes the need for individuals to remain autonomous and
distinct, while also adapting to others, learning new cultural scripts and
being able to communicate with people from diverse linguistic back-
grounds (Haslett, 2017).
9
www.usip.org/public-education/students/conflict-styles-assessment or https://sielearning.tafensw.e
du.au/MBA/9791F/BusinessServices/LO/1207_020138_605F_02_wi/1207_020138_605F_0220_wi.
htm
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
310 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
cultures. Since such projects regularly offer opportunities for miscom-
munication, they also serve as a living laboratory for implementing the
guidelines presented in the previous section.
• Using the same guidelines, have students create skits that practice these
suggestions of self-reflection and creative problem-solving.
• Ask students to conduct research projects that examine a historical or
present conflict in an L2/Lx-speaking context and analyze it from the
perspectives of each participant, trying to understand the events that led
up to the conflict, its root causes, and the ways in which the participants
or others have tried to resolve the conflict.
• Incorporate a citizen-diplomacy project, especially for students who are
planning to study abroad, that builds mutual understanding and
respect towards an L2/Lx-speaking cultural group (e.g., a practical,
service-learning task).
• Teach students to analyze L2/Lx use as a layered event, influenced by
societal as well as individual forces (i.e., view each speaker as a unique
individual, participating in unique interactions, where the interaction is
situated in a broader cultural context).
• Invite international students to meet with your class and talk about
their experiences with adaptation and integration, discussing difficul-
ties honestly, while also modeling successful intercultural development.
• Encourage your students to study or volunteer abroad, join an inter-
cultural organization, or host international students themselves, to
prepare them for intercultural communicative practices beyond the
classroom.
• Ask students studying abroad to keep a journal of successful and
unsuccessful communicative situations they encounter day-to-day
and reflect on how their skills at conflict resolution and intercultural
communication develop over time.
Summary
Resolving miscommunication and conflict requires us to explore paths
to other ways of being. If participants are willing and are oriented
towards mutual understanding – be it language or content – inter-
cultural interactions can enrich the human experience. Conversely,
participants who are unwilling or unable to work through linguistic
mishaps, the initial stresses of acculturation, and the strains of mis-
understanding may cause (potentially long-term) conflict (Chen,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
13 Miscommunication, Conflict, and ICC 311
2015). Although intercultural communication is challenging, occasion-
ally frustrating, and sometimes devastating, people who are willing to
put in hard work will find “joy and delight in being able to sustain
. . . remarkably fluid sequences of sustained social exchange”
(Mortensen, 2006, p. 43). Developing skills to become interculturally
competent takes time, patience, and commitment, but the experiences
we gain along the way not only provide individuals with the skills and
personal development that are absolutely essential in the twenty-first
century, but are also immensely rewarding personally as well.
As language instructors, we have a unique opportunity to help our
learners become intercultural communicators, since learning a new lan-
guage is an ideal locus of becoming aware of the connections between
cultural practices and language use. In order to take advantage of such
opportunities, we need to reflect on who we are as language teachers, who
our students are, and what goals we wish to establish for them as present
and future participants in intercultural communication.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
312 Part III Intercultural Communication and the Personal Journey
could incorporate into your teaching. Are there strategies that are better
suited for beginning, intermediate, or advanced L2/Lx learners? Explain
your reasoning.
6. Reflect on your goals as language teachers: What objectives will you set
for your learners in terms of intercultural communicative competence?
Working in small groups, design an activity for your likely student
population that teaches a point in this chapter that really captured your
attention. Discuss how this activity might help you and your learners
achieve your pedagogical objectives.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:04:31, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.014
appendix
313
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:44:34, subject to the Cambridge Core
terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108780360.015
Pedagogical focus
1 • Focus intensively on lexical develop- • Foster grammar development by using • Make pragmatics an explicit, emphasized
ment, using sociolinguistically and sociolinguistically contextualized models component of language learning and use.
culturally contextualized vocabulary. as input and scaffolding for output in • Focus on chunked, formulaic expres-
• Prioritize high-frequency, everyday various modalities. sions, basic speech acts (e.g., greeting,
vocabulary, developing a repertoire • Prioritize structures that are used most thanking, inviting, asking for help, leave-
of synonyms, antonyms, and com- often in everyday communication; taking).
mon collocations. include words and phrases that frequently • Recognize broader and narrower cultural
• Introduce learners to formal and co-occur with these structures (e.g., trends (e.g., typical politeness formulae in
casual styles (incl. slang) in ‘stan- adverbs of time with narrating the past). ‘standard’ varieties), while noting the
dard’ varieties of languages. • Introduce learners to the most common importance of individual preferences in
• Introduce skills for conducting mini- genres they are likely to encounter (e.g., pragmatics; raise learners’ awareness of
ethnographies on authentic resources newspapers, Instagram posts for personal situational variables that affect
to identify different language vari- use) or specialized texts (e.g., for students interaction.
eties and styles, and multimodal of tourism or other professions). • Recognize how multiturn, multiparticipant
analyses to recognize the interaction Recognize different grammatical expec- interactions are structured, using richly
of multiple semiotic systems of tations (e.g., computer-mediated com- contextualized models as scaffolding for
meaning-making in the L2/Lx per- munication is less prescriptive than an written and spoken output.
taining to the lexicon. academic essay). • Identify learners’ communicative and prag-
• Help learners reflect on their L1, • Introduce learners to morphosyntactic pat- matic needs in the L2/Lx, and help them
comparing it to the L2/Lx lexicon. terns in ‘standard’ varieties of languages, explore genres of texts they are likely to
• Foster language play and engagement including different standards of pluricentric encounter outside the classroom (e.g.,
with vocabulary of interest for languages, and some formal and casual emailing professors and peers or interacting
learners. styles in these varieties. with clients at a health clinic).
• Help students develop skills for conducting • Include humorous authentic sources to
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:44:34, subject to the Cambridge Core
2 • Develop lexicon further, introducing • Expand learners’ knowledge of grammar • Introduce learners to pragmatic features
academic or professional vocabulary, structures, including more nuanced past, (e.g., hedging, politeness formulae,
depending on learners’ objectives. present, and/or future tenses, a wider speech acts) in ‘standard’ varieties of lan-
• Continue introducing richly contex- range of cases if applicable in the language guages, with some stylistic and regional
tualized vocabulary, multiword and (teach most common first, rarer later), or variation.
idiomatic expressions, in a manner; common moods and aspects. • Expand learners’ understanding and use
introduce word meanings farther • Help learners recognize connections of internal and external modifiers (e.g.,
from the core, including abstract between varieties of the L2/Lx and the downgraders, intensifiers, subjunctive
concepts (primarily receptively). social identities they represent. forms if they are used in politeness).
• Help learners make connections • Build receptive knowledge of stylistic var- • Introduce learners to common themes
between language varieties and iation (casual v. formal language) and and types of humor in the L2/Lx-
social identity, expanding receptive learn about appropriate contexts of use. speaking cultures (e.g., common frames,
knowledge of lexical variation and Discuss explicitly involuntary (e.g., L2/ scripts, and sources of humor in the host
contexts of use. Lx use of the language) and voluntary society). Discuss the importance of
• Increase leaners’ ability to reflect on flouting of grammar forms, such as rap humor as a part of one’s identity, build
their L1 lexicon, exploring similarities music or poetry. awareness of the role of humor in helping
and differences, including intracul- • Deepen learners’ ability to conduct eth- people get through challenging times,
tural variation. nographies and multimodal analyses, such as studying abroad.
• Encourage learners to research voca- using authentic sources, examining how • Continue enhancing learners’ skills to
bulary in-depth that they find inter- grammar contributes to meaning in dif- conduct mini-ethnographies and multi-
esting and/or useful. ferent types of texts (e.g., advertisements, modal analyses to analyze L2/Lx
poetry, lyrics, journal articles). pragmatics.
3 • Emphasize productive knowledge of • Emphasize productive knowledge of • Increase learners’ knowledge of and use of
vocabulary, in diverse casual and grammar in diverse casual and formal broader and narrower cultural trends
formal contexts, including multi- contexts, including an increasingly wider (e.g., politeness formulae and adjacency
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:44:34, subject to the Cambridge Core
(cont.)
Pedagogical focus
• Expand learners’ knowledge of low- • Introduce learners to lower-frequency, • Expand learners’ awareness of situational
frequency, professional and academic professional, and academic structures variables that affect interaction at the
vocabulary, especially targeted to (e.g., embedded clauses), especially tar- local level (e.g., intentional impoliteness).
their personal interests and language geted to their personal interests and lan- • Help learners recognize a broader range
use needs. guage needs. of pragmatic functions in particular con-
• Increase learners’ knowledge of vari- • Increase learners’ knowledge of varieties texts: e.g., What linguistic forms are used
eties of language (in terms of the of grammar use in diverse contexts (e.g., in interaction at a party with friends, at
lexicon), which is appropriate in spoken by whom, when, where, under the doctor’s office, or in an interview with
what contexts (e.g., spoken by what circumstances, with which interlo- an immigration officer? How does our
whom, when, for what communica- cutors, for what communicative pur- audience influence what language we use?
tive purposes) using extended data, poses) using ethnographic analyses of • Expand learners’ understanding of
such as television serials or a novel, extended data, such as television serials or humor in different media – introduce
expanding the repertoire of genres. a novel, expanding the repertoire of learners to a broader range of humor (e.g.,
• Deepen learners’ ability to analyze genres. taboo topics, ‘appropriateness,’ and
the relationship between the lexicon flouting social norms to shape sociopoli-
and cultural values (e.g., language tical practices).
and power). • Develop learners’ skills to conduct mini-
• Build leaners’ lexicon to be able to ethnographies, including with native
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:44:34, subject to the Cambridge Core
4 • Continue building lexical knowledge • Continue refining learners’ grammatical • Expand learners’ knowledge of pragmatic
in diverse casual and formal contexts, knowledge in diverse casual and formal functions to debate, disagree, suggest,
expanding creative language use and contexts. offer advice, etc., which can be face-
metaphorical expression (analyze the • Study model texts and speeches and have threatening, so the nuance between dis-
cultural values they reflect). learners edit their own writing and speech agreeing with humor versus strongly
• Continue to build lexical fluency, based on those models, focusing on assertive language, just to name one
expanding depth of knowledge to grammatical accuracy, to prepare for example, should be examined.
include historical perspectives on academic and professional • Explore in depth the pragmatic skills par-
culturally significant words and communication. ticular to professions that learners might
phrases (e.g., democracy or • Expand learners’ breadth and depth of undertake: What skills do musicians
nationalism). grammatical knowledge to include his- need? Mathematicians? School teachers,
• Expand learners’ knowledge of rele- torical developments and change, so that when interacting with children, their
vant academic or professional voca- they can access older texts. parents, or school administrators?
bularies (e.g., natural or social • Expand knowledge of grammatical struc- • Expand learners’ understanding of
sciences, diplomacy). tures that are relevant for their own aca- humor as a sociopolitical tool, used in
• Increase learners’ knowledge of lan- demic or professional purposes (e.g., ‘appropriate’ and ‘inappropriate’ ways,
guage varieties, including historical structures that might be unique in law, both reflecting and shaping cultural and
developments of social, political, and medicine, diplomacy). sociopolitical practices. What – if any –
economic cultural patterns and their • Improve learners’ knowledge of varieties are the consequences for flouting
effect on the lexicon, using critical of language (in terms of grammar), expectations?
and ethnographic analyses of multi- including historical and sociopolitically • Enhance learners’ skills to analyze
ple data sources. Explore similarities divergent contexts, using ethnographic authentic resources to identify who uses
and differences among the cultures analyses of multiple data sources. the L2/Lx, how, and when via multiple
they are increasingly familiar with. semiotic systems of meaning-making in
• Foster critical awareness of how lin- the L2/Lx as it pertains to pragmatics.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:44:34, subject to the Cambridge Core
Pedagogical focus
1 • Address paralinguistic features expli- • Address nonverbal communication expli- • Address cultural knowledge explicitly in
citly in course materials (e.g., how citly in course materials. course materials.
intonation differs in questions, • Expose learners to authentic materials, so • Expose learners to authentic materials, so
answers). that they can familiarize themselves with that they can familiarize themselves with
• Use authentic materials to familiarize nonverbal communication used by other potential sources of information used by
learners with the rate and volume of speakers of the L2/Lx. other speakers of the L2/Lx.
speech used by other L2/Lx speakers; • Introduce common symbols, icons, and • Introduce commonly known informa-
include extensive pre-viewing and indexes that are meaningful in L2/Lx- tion about both everyday and unique
viewing tasks to support authentic speaking communities, focusing on cur- events (e.g., important historical events
materials instead of adapting or sim- rent practices, as well as the cognitive and and geographical facts; famous authors,
plifying them. affective responses members of the cul- writers, members of popular culture;
• Explore the use of affective devices in ture have to these signs (e.g., the sche- everyday needs, such as names of grocery
computer-mediated communication. mata they activate and emotional stores, electronics websites).
• Introduce basic conversation man- reactions to the signs). • Help students develop the skills for ana-
agement devices, especially ones • Help students develop skills to analyze lyzing authentic resources and interacting
used to hold the floor while thinking, authentic resources as they pertain to with members of the community to gain
and the most common backchannel- L2/Lx nonverbal communication. cultural knowledge.
ing devices to signal continued
interest in conversation.
• Help students develop skills for ana-
lyzing authentic resources for para-
linguistic features of the L2/Lx.
2 • Introduce learners to paralinguistic • Help learners recognize more nonverbal • Introduce learners to further resources
features in more diverse interac- communication in sociolinguistic contexts, that help build cultural knowledge (e.g.,
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:44:34, subject to the Cambridge Core
• Expand the range of conversation • Analyze practices related to proxemics a culture view themselves or their culture,
management devices used in differ- and orientation to time. and their interactions with other cultures.
ent social contexts and how their use • Continue enhancing learners’ skills to • Examine how the culture has viewed
might be perceived. conduct mini-ethnographies and multi- famous authors, writers, members of
• Continue enhancing learners’ skills modal analyses on authentic resources to popular culture and how these views have
to conduct mini-ethnographies and identify how nonverbal communication changed over time.
multimodal analyses on authentic is realized among members of different • Help students develop further skills to
resources to identify how paralin- L2/Lx-speaking communities. analyze authentic resources for gaining
guistic features impact the emotional cultural background and contextual
tone of speakers’ or online users’ knowledge; improve skills of discovery.
utterances in the L2/Lx.
3 • Expand learners’ knowledge of con- • Refine learners’ knowledge and use of • Refine learners’ knowledge of potential
versation-management devices: how nonverbal communication among differ- resources that help build background
to interject effectively, express nega- ent participants and in various social and contextual information.
tive responses to a speaker’s utter- contexts • Explore in depth current and historical
ances, or convey personal interest. • Expand learners’ knowledge of how per- cultural forces that have shaped and con-
• Explore how paralinguistic features sonal presentation, kinesics, haptics and tinue to shape the culture’s perspectives,
can be used to express positive proxemics can be used to convey specific practices, and products.
(affiliative) and negative (e.g., sar- social meaning, including polite and • Enhance learners’ skills to conduct mini-
casm, social distance) affect, includ- impolite behaviors and their ethnographies and multimodal analyses
ing how these features are expressed consequences. on authentic resources to increase their
across interactional situations (i.e., • Continue to explore subcultural variation cultural knowledge, both at the societal
when, how, and with whom). in the use of nonverbal communication and individual levels (i.e., analyze broader
• Introduce learners to diverse dialect (regional and other cultural groups) and social forces, as well as how knowledge of
and register variation as well as non- what social connotations different enact- local factors impacts intercultural
native accents. Examine critically ments of nonverbal communication communication).
how accents are perceived and what carry. • Guide learners to reflect on current social,
social connotations they carry among • Continue to improve learners’ ability to political, and economic issues, exploring
L2/Lx speakers as well as in learners’ conduct mini-ethnographies and multi- similarities and differences among the
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:44:34, subject to the Cambridge Core
(cont.)
Pedagogical focus
4 • Continue to expand learners’ knowl- • Continue to expand learners’ knowledge • Explore the impact of popular culture
edge of nuanced paralinguistic fea- of nonverbal communication, including (including changes over time) on every-
tures (e.g., interviews, celebrations), a historical overview of the origins of day perspectives, practices, and products.
including increasingly wider ranges various features of nonverbal communi- • Learn in-depth about current events,
of emotions they encounter in diverse cation: signs, personal presentation, their historical origins, and possible
interactional contexts. kinesics, haptics, proxemics, and chrone- future trajectories.
• Explore in depth the paralinguistic mics (e.g., from literary or historical • Explore subcultural variation in how
features that might be characteristic documents). members of the culture might view the
of professions learners might under- • Explore nonverbal communication that same events (current and historical) dif-
take: Are there particular ways nurses might be characteristic of various profes- ferently and why.
speak? Business people? sions or in diverse interpersonal commu- • Continue enhancing learners’ ability to
• Expand learners’ critical understand- nicative settings. conduct research, mini-ethnographies,
ing of how paralinguistic features are • Continue to improve learners’ ability to and multimodal analyses in order to
evaluated by society, both positively conduct mini-ethnographies and multi- increase their cultural knowledge, using
and negatively, and explore social modal analysis of nonverbal communi- reliable sources.
movements that try to change discri- cation in the L2/Lx in increasingly diverse • Increase learners’ ability to reflect –
minatory practices. (A positive recent interactional situations. increasingly independently – on current
development has been the addition of social, political, and economic issues,
multilingual speakers, representing exploring similarities and differences
a richer tapestry of accents in some among the practices of their own and
television series, such as the British- other cultural groups (including intra-
French crime drama series Death in cultural variation).
Paradise.) • Continue to increase learners’ critical
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Connecticut, on 17 Aug 2020 at 02:44:34, subject to the Cambridge Core