Sander 1972
Sander 1972
Eric K. Sander
Cleveland State University, Cleveland, Ohio
The age at which specific consonant articulations emerge differs greatly among
children. Widely quoted age tables represent upper age limits rather than average
performance, and stress-sound mastery in all word positions as opposed to customary
usage. A new graphic way of summarizing information about age in relation to
consonant development is proposed.
When do children acquire the various speech sounds of our language? Con-
sidering the wealth of schedules in such areas as motor behavior, physical
~ o w t h , and cognitive skill, the question might be assumed to be an easy one
for the student of child behavior to answer. Surprisingly, however, a concise
reply to the question is difficult to provide, for reasons that have only partly
to do with the available research knowledge.
One area of confusion arises from the fact that most normative accounts of
articulatory development stress the developmental ages at which a child's
failure to produce a given speech sound ought to concern a speech clinician or
a parent; they are not portraits of average performance.
T h e first large sample investigation of the articulatory status of U.S. children
at various preschool ages, by Wellman, Case, Mengert, and Bradbury (1931),
established norms reflecting not median performance but rather the ages at
which 75% or more of the children produced a sound correctly. Poole's widely
quoted 1 articles (1934, 1998) carried the Wellman logic one step further by
listing the ages at which correct articulation of specified speech sounds was
achieved by all (100%) of the children tested.
T h e pioneer reports by Wellman et al. and by Poole were updated when
T e m p l i n (1957) published additional articulatory test findings from 480
children. Before assigning an earliest age level to a sound, T e m p l i n insisted
1Widely quoted because Poole was the first to attach a single age level to an entire sound
category. Wellman et al. were quite content merely to list the ages at which a required per-
centage of children produced a sound correctly in a specified word context.
55
that three-fourths of the children of a particular age produce that sound cor-
rectly in all positions of a word (initial, medial, and final).
These widely publicized group standards reflecting upper age limits rather
than average performance are a recurring source of misunderstanding in dis-
cussions of articulatory development. A second cause of confusion is that most
authors who wish to speak at less than monographic length about articulatory
development fail to distinguish different indices of speech-sound achievement.
MASTERY VS C U S T O M A R Y PRODUCTION
TABLE I. Number of children (out of 60) at various age levels correctly articulating / t /
a n d / s / in three different word positions (from Templin, 1957).
Initial/t/
(toes, table) 60 60 60 60 60 60 60 60
Medial/t/
(skating, outing) 16 16 20 17 19 53 50 50
Final/t/
(bat, skate) 45 41 45 45 46 51 56 57
Initial/s/
(seat, silk) 42 51 46 48 47 46 55 58
Medial/s/
(myself, glasses) 39 49 46 45 47 43 53 57
Final/s/
(mouse, grass) 35 41 44 46 46 47 53 56
TABLE 2. Percentage of children at various ages who have laughed aloud, shouted for atten-
tion, and said one clear word (from Griffiths, 1954).
Laughs
Aloud 0 35 98 100 100 100 100 100 100
Shouts for
Attention 0 0 4 37 84 94 98 100 100
One Clear
Word 0 0 6 28 57 90 94 98 100
says his first clear words at nine or ten months. Not much danger of misinter-
pretation here.
Now compare these findings with what occurs when one attempts to chart
the specifics of articulatory skill. Templin, unfortunately, studied no children
below age three, by which time they had already averaged 60% correct produc-
tion of the consonant elements. A few sounds, however, including the voiced
and voiceless th and the / v / , 2 were still said incorrectly by the majority of
three-year-olds. Correct production of these sounds (initial position) as a
function of age (in years) is charted in Table 3.
TABLE3. Percentage of children at various ages correctly articulating initial voiced th, initial
/v/, and initial voiceless th (from Templin, 1957).
Initial/~5/
(those, there) 23 32 57 60 62 83 97 98
Initial/v/(valentine,
vacuum cleaner) 12 30 40 47 55 80 88 98
Initial/o/
(thinner, thumb) 27 28 48 60 67 85 90 97
S U M M A R I Z I N G AGE I N F O R M A T I O N
2Templin did report correct production o f / v / in the medial position (as in driving) by
36 of 60 three-year-olds. For two-year-olds, Wellman et al. observed no correct productions of
initial/v/, 7% correct articulation of the initial voiced th, and 20% correct articulation of
initial voiceless th.
table the essential information about articulatory development. Given the ex-
pected variability among children, tables with average ages of customary
consonant production will send some needlessly anxiqus mothers scurrying to a
speech clinician. To insist that three-fourths or 100% of all children articulate
a sound in all word positions before assigning it to an age level tends to
promote equally erroneous interpretations of normal sound acquisition. This
dilemma may be easily resolved, however, by including in the age summaries
of consonant development both an average age of customary production and
a measure reflecting the traditional upper age limits for sound acquisition.
Since the most significant progress in articulatory development takes place
between the ages of one and three, age norms ought to arrange for a stop or two
spaced along this busy route. Bullowa, Jones, and Duckert (1964), for example,
provide a fascinating narrative of one child's progress (from 11 to 22 months)
in articulating the word shoe. At 13-14 months, report Winitz and Irwin
(1958), only 16% of the words in an average child's limited repertoire are
free of phonetic error. T h e percentage of phonetically accurate words rises
to 24 at 15-16 months and to 38 at 17-18 months.
By age two, the average child has sufficient vocabulary and intelligibility to
allow specific testing of a variety of consonants. Published data on the articula-
tion of two-year-olds comes mainly from Wellman et al. (1931), who, re-
grettably, tested only 15 (probably brighter-than-average) children, not all of
them completely. (Future investigations at this age level, using more children,
might alter these pioneer findings somewhat, although probably not greatly.)
By including data from the Wellman et al. two-year-olds in addition to that
collected by T e m p l i n at later ages (three through eight) a tentative table of
average age estimates for customary consonant production may be constructed.
Customary production of a sound might reasonably be assumed if a majority
of children articulate it clearly in at least two of three word positions. One
method, then, of translating previous research findings into a table of average
ages of customary consonant articulation would be to assign each sound to the
earliest age at which it is correctly produced in two positions by 5I% or more
of the children. Although these calculations are in most cases easily accom-
plished, many sounds are testable in only two different word positions. T o
standardize matters, each sound has therefore been placed at an age level
where the combined test average at the various word positions exceeds 50%
correct production. 8 Sounds assessed at age two (the earliest age level tested)
and showing a combined average exceeding 70% correct production have been
somewhat arbitrarily placed in a "before age two" category.
For convenience, and to avoid a misleading sense of precision, fractional age
norms have been disregarded. As added information, each sound is followed
3For example, although 53% of Templin's three-year-olds correctly produced /" or /d3/ in
the initial position, the comparable percentages in the medial and final positions are 30% and
22%, respectively, resulting in a combined average of 38%. The minimum combined average
for age placement is 51%. The sound is therefore placed at age four where the combined
average is 69%.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
Requests for reprints should be addressed to Eric K. Sander, 915 Nelaview Road, Cleveland,
Ohio 44112.
4At age four,/v/received a combined average of 51% despite the low initial position per-
centage. Three-fourths of Templin's children of that age produced it correctIy in the medial
position.
5In constructing this figure, use was made of the available fractional age norms. (Mainly for
convenience of memory, fractional age norms were disregarded in the previous table of average
ages.) In addition, / f / and / y / were interpolated at 2.5 years, since they showed combined
age-three averages exceeding 70% correct production. Consonants whose averages exceeded
70% at age two were extrapolated downward to 1.5 years.
6The zh o r / 3 / sound received a combined age eight average of 86%; its upper limit has
been extrapolated to 8.5 years.
AGE LEVEL
3 + ~ G ? 8
[ Em
Figure 1. Average age estimates and upper age limits of customary consonant produc-
tion. The solid bar corresponding to each sound starts at the median age of customary,
articulation; it stops at an age level at which 90% of all children are customarily pro-
ducing the sound. (From Templin, 1957; Wellman et ah, 1931.)
REFERENCES
BULLOWA, M., JoNrs, L., and DUCKERT, A., The acquisition of a word. Lang. Speech, 7, 107-111
(1964).
GRIFFITHS, R., The .4bilities of Babies. New York: McGraw-HiU (1954).
IRWIN, O. C., Phonetical description of speech development in childhood. In L. Kaiser (Ed.),
Manual of Phonetics. Amsterdam: North Holland (1957).
POOLE, I., Genetic development of articulation of consonant sounds in speech. Elem. Eng.
Rev., 11, 159-161 (1934).
POOLE-DAVIS, I., The speech aspects of reading readiness. Yearb. Dept. elem. schl. Princ., natl.
educ..4ss., 17, 282-289 (1938).
TEMPLIN, M., Certain Language Skills in Children. Minneapolis: Univ. Minnesota (1957).
WF.LLMAN, B., CASE, I., MVNGERT, I., and BRADBURY, D., Speech sounds of young children.
Univ. Iowa Stud. Child Well., 5, 1-82 (1931).
WINITZ, H., and IRWIN, O. C., Syllabic and phonetic structure of infants' early words. J. Speech
Hearing Res., 1, 250-256 (1958).