0% found this document useful (0 votes)
24 views88 pages

GroupsChapter May2009

Uploaded by

denizhancay
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
24 views88 pages

GroupsChapter May2009

Uploaded by

denizhancay
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 88

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/277839313

Sports teams as a model for workplace teams: Lessons and liabilities

Article in Academy of Management Perspectives · August 2001


DOI: 10.5465/AME.2001.5229533

CITATIONS READS
110 7,783

1 author:

Nancy Katz
Brandeis University
19 PUBLICATIONS 3,169 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Nancy Katz on 30 October 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


GROUP BEHAVIOR AND PERFORMANCE

J. Richard Hackman and Nancy Katz


Harvard University
May, 2009

Contents

Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
Part One: Groups and Their Attributes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
Group Purposes and Performance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
Attributes of Purposive Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10
Part Two: Approaches to the Study of Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16
The Psychodynamic Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17
The Network Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
The Action Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21
The Process-Focused Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
The Decision-Analytic Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25
The Complex Systems Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28
Part Three: Current Frontiers of Group Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30
Assembly of Member Attributes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30
Use of Member Information and Expertise . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33
Group Learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35
Group Stability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37
Compositional Diversity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40
The Context of Group Behavior and Performance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42
Temporal Issues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45
Team Leadership . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47
Collaboration in Dispersed Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50
Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55

To appear in: Fiske, S. T., Gilbert, D. T., & Lindzey, G. (Eds.). (2010). Handbook of social
psychology (5th ed.). New York: Wiley.

Preparation of this chapter was supported in part by National Science Foundation Research Grant
REC-0106070 to Harvard University, with support from Fred Ambrose and the Intelligence
Technology Innovation Center. We gratefully acknowledge the research assistance of Sanden
Averett and Susan Choi, and the helpful comments provided by Ruth Wageman and members of
the Boston area GroupsGroup workshop.
7May09

GROUP BEHAVIOR AND PERFORMANCE

J. Richard Hackman and Nancy Katz


Harvard University

Although a scattering of influential empirical studies on group behavior and performance


were conducted in the first half of the 20th century (e.g., Shaw, 1932), the psychological study of
small groups did not begin to flourish until the war years of the 1940s. In the United States,
nearly all available resources, including the nation‘s scientific talent, were brought to bear on
winning WW II. Among the matters for which scientific knowledge was required were the
dynamics and performance of small groups—for example, to identify the factors that shaped the
performance of infantry squads in combat, or to properly design and lead groups that provided
back-home support for the war effort.

The fruits of the research carried out in those years are summarized in the extensive
compendia of empirical findings about group behavior and performance published by Hare
(1962) and by McGrath and Altman (1966), and in the informative sampling of that research
reprinted and discussed in Cartwright and Zander‘s influential Group Dynamics (1953;
subsequent editions in 1960 and 1968). Despite often-conflicting findings, gaps in knowledge,
and a paucity of theory, it appeared that a reasonably solid platform of empirical knowledge
about groups had been established. On that platform, surely, there would develop a steady
stream of research that, in the Lewinian tradition, would simultaneously advance social
psychological theory and provide evidence-based guidance about how best to structure, support,
and lead groups of various kinds.

Those lofty aspirations were not fully realized. What had been a robust and fast-flowing
stream of social psychological research on groups had by the 1970s gradually diminished to a
trickle. Ivan Steiner, who himself had published a landmark text in the field (Steiner, 1972) was
eventually driven to publicly inquire of his scholarly colleagues ―Whatever happened to the
group in social psychology?‖ (Steiner, 1974). The answer, Steiner suggested, was that the
revival of interest in individual attitudes documented by McGuire (1969) in the second edition of
this Handbook had captured social psychologists‘ attention to such an extent that group research
had entered a state of hibernation.

Research responds to the mood of the times, Steiner hypothesized, but with about a
decade lag. The earlier flowering of group research had indeed been in response to the war years
and the relatively tranquil period that followed. And it was the pervasive individualism of the
1960s that prompted the subsequent focus in social psychology on intra-individual processes.
The social unrest that characterized the late 1960s and early 1970s, he predicted, would soon re-
invigorate small group research.

As John Levine and Richard Moreland documented in the previous edition of this
Handbook, it didn‘t happen (Levine & Moreland, 1998; see also McGrath, Arrow, & Berdahl,
2000; Moreland, Hogg, & Hains, 1994; and Steiner, 1986). What happened is that small group
-2-

research gradually but definitively changed its home address. Although group behavior and
performance once again is a highly active field of study, it has moved out of the field‘s ancestral
home in social psychology. As social psychologists increasingly have drawn upon cognitive
neuroscience and evolutionary theories to explain social phenomena, small group research has
migrated to the periphery of the field. And the discipline of sociology, now increasingly reliant
on the models and methods of economics, has not taken up the slack. These days, it is mainly
scholars in schools of communication, organizational behavior programs in business schools, and
industrial-organizational psychology programs who are doing the bulk of the research on groups
(see, for example, Frey, 1999, 2003; Hogg & Tindale, 2001; Ilgin, Hollenbeck, Johnson, &
Jundt, 2005; Kozlowski & Bell, 2003; Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006; Mathieu, Maynard, Rapp, &
Gilson, 2008; Salas, Goodwin, & Burke, 2008; Salas, Stagl, & Burke, 2004).

Groups are Changing

It is a time of transition not just in who is doing research on group behavior and
performance but also in the very phenomena under study (Mortensen, 2009). Traditionally,
groups tended to be intact, stable, and tightly bounded social systems. Now, the composition of
many groups shifts so often that it can be nearly impossible to pin down who actually is a
member (Wageman, Nunes, Burruss, & Hackman, 2008, chap. 2). Traditionally, group members
tended to be co-located and to interact almost exclusively face-to-face. Now, many groups are
so widely distributed across geographies and time zones that members may never even see one
another, relying instead on an ever-expanding set of technological resources to coordinate their
activities (O‘Leary & Cummings, 2007). Traditionally, most task-performing groups generated
some identifiable product, service, or decision. Now, groups handle a much wider variety of
tasks that often have considerable complexity and uncertainty, such as providing organizational
leadership (Berg, 2005), carrying out negotiations (Behfar, Friedman, & Brett, 2008), and
managing organizational change initiatives (Hackman & Edmondson, 2008). Traditionally,
work groups in organizations had a designated leader. Now, increasing numbers of groups are
self-managing with members sharing leadership responsibilities (Manz & Sims, 1987).
Traditionally, groups operated within a single organizational context. Now, groups often include
members from two or more different organizations that may have different policies, practices,
and cultures (Dess, Rasheed, McLaughin, & Priem, 1995). And, finally, groups traditionally
have been created top-down by an organizational manager or, in the laboratory, by the
experimenter. Now, increasing numbers of groups are self-created, often using electronic
technologies, to explore shared interests (Shirky, 2008) or to pursue activist social agendas
(Andrews, Ganz, Baggetta, Han, & Lin, in press).

An iconic group from the past would be a coal-mining team, a clearly bounded and
highly stable group whose members are deeply dependent on one another for safely carrying out
their collective work, and who commonly spend a considerable portion of their non-work time
together as well (Goodman & Leyden, 1991). An example of the new kinds of groups that are
seen these days is a research team consisting of both university researchers and pharmaceutical
company scientists that is charged with investigating a new compound that may have therapeutic
potential. These scientists work in laboratories in three different countries located in three
different time zones, they communicate and coordinate using electronic technologies exclusively,
and group composition gradually changes over time in response to the different expertise that is
-3-

needed at different stages of the project. This team is emblematic of what Hackman and
Wageman (2005b) call sand dune teams—dynamic social systems that have fluid rather than
fixed composition and boundaries. Just as sand dunes change in number and shape as winds
change, teams of various sizes and kinds form and re-form within a larger social system in
response to changing external demands and requirements.

Chapter Plan

The new group phenomena just described will require those of us who study them to
reconsider our traditional conceptual paradigms and research methodologies. In particular,
group scholars will need to move beyond reliance on laboratory experiments and survey-based
field methods in studies of group dynamics and performance, and give greater attention to
structural, temporal, and contextual forces that powerfully shape group behavior. In the words of
Moreland, Hogg, and Hains (1994), we may need to go ―back to the future‖ if we are to generate
conceptually interesting and practically useful theories and findings about new forms of group
life. Several approaches to the study of group behavior reviewed in this chapter, although
prominent in the early days of the field, have fallen from favor. As will be seen, these
approaches offer some possibilities for research and theory that may be especially well suited to
the emergent new realities of group life.

Part One of the chapter provides an analysis of just what is meant by the concept
―group,‖ exploration of what it means to say that a group is ―effective,‖ and identification of the
major dimensions that distinguish various types of groups. Part Two reviews and analyzes six
different approaches to the study of groups, each of which provides a distinctive ―lens‖ through
which groups can be observed and analyzed.

Part Three explores a number of specific issues that lie at the frontier of scholarly work
on group behavior and performance. Rather than provide a comprehensive review of all the work
that has been carried out on groups since the excellent review by Levine and Moreland (1998) in
the last edition of this Handbook, the chapter emphasizes those issues that appear to lie on the
present cutting edge of the field—some of which have received substantial attention by
contemporary group researchers, but others of which have not. Throughout, the focus is
exclusively on the group qua group; other group-related phenomena, such as group influences on
the beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors of individuals and the dynamics of inter-group relationships,
are not addressed in detail (for an early review of group influences on individuals, see Hackman,
1992; for analyses of the dynamics of inter-group relations see the chapters by Dvoidio &
Gaertner and by Yzerbyt & Demoulin in this Handbook).
-4-

PART ONE:
GROUPS AND THEIR ATTRIBUTES

The term ―group‖ is commonly and casually used to refer to an enormous variety of
social forms, which is one reason why empirical findings about group and team behavior (we use
the two terms interchangeably) have been less cumulative than one would hope and expect. This
chapter deals only with the behavior and performance of purposive groups—that is real groups
that exist to accomplish something.

Numerous definitions of groups have been proposed in the scholarly literature (e.g.,
Baron, Kerr, & Miller, chap. 1; McGrath, 1984, chap. 1; Mohrman, Cohen, & Mohrman, 1995;
Offermann & Spiros, 2001). Our use of the term, following Alderfer (1977), is relatively simple
and inclusive: A group is an intact social system, complete with boundaries, interdependence for
some shared purpose, and differentiated member roles. It is possible to distinguish members of
groups from nonmembers, even if they do not have regular face-to-face contact and even if
membership changes frequently. Moreover, members depend on one another in pursuing their
collective purposes, and they develop specialized roles within the group as they do so. Casual
gatherings of people who have no shared purpose lie outside our domain, as do reference groups,
identity groups, and statistical aggregations of the attributes, estimates, or preferences of people
who do not actually interact with one another (e.g., the average SAT scores of a college‘s
―group‖ of incoming students).

As social systems, groups are perceived as entities by both members and nonmembers
(Hamilton, Sherman, & Rodgers, 2004), they create and redefine realities, and they generate
outcomes that can be legitimately attributed to the group as a unit. The proper concepts for
describing groups, therefore, are those that are situated at the group level of analysis, not those
that describe the cognitive or affective processes of individual members (Larson & Christensen,
1993). To describe a collective entity such as a group as having thoughts and feelings is to
significantly increase the difficulty of explicating how the states and processes of individual
persons combine to shape group-level structures and interactions (Hutchins, 1995; James, Joyce,
& Slocum, 1988).

Group Purposes and Performance


Because most groups have multiple purposes—some manifest and explicit but others
latent and implicit—it is useful to partition them into three sets—specifically, those that have
mainly to do with (1) accomplishing the work of the group, (2) strengthening the capabilities of
the group itself, and (3) fostering the well-being of individual group members.

Accomplishing Work

Most groups, whether in laboratory experiments or field settings, have some piece of
work to accomplish—some product, service, decision, or performance. Researchers commonly
rely on relatively simple outcome measures to assess how well those groups have done, such as a
group‘s score on an experimental task in the laboratory (e.g., the number of anagrams solved
-5-

correctly) or organization-specified performance measures in field research (e.g., number of units


produced, or an index of customer satisfaction).

In fact, it is conceptually and empirically challenging to develop measures of group


performance that are both meaningful and psychometrically adequate. As Pritchard and Watson
(1992) point out, the concept ―group productivity‖ has been used in group research far too
casually. A valid measure of productivity, they suggest, must consider both the efficiency of the
group (i.e., its output relative to inputs) and its effectiveness (i.e., its output relative either to its
own goals or to the expectations of others). The Pritchard-Wilson approach, although attractive
in theory, requires data that can be difficult or impossible to obtain in practice. An alternative
that aspires to finesse that difficulty is to identify those who are served or affected by the group
and rely on their assessments of the group‘s performance (Hackman, 2002). Although this
approach accepts, for better or for worse, whatever criteria the assessors use, it does recognize
the fact that consequences for a group are far more likely to derive from its clients‘ evaluations
than from any researcher-constructed performance measure.

Strengthening Group Capabilities

Some groups have as an explicit purpose building the collective capability of the group
qua group—for example, a research team or an educational seminar that seeks to enhance the
learning capability of the group as a whole. Other examples include a crisis management team
that rehearses its response strategy so the team will be ready when and if a real crisis occurs, or a
student string quartet that does not expect to perform publicly but whose members seek to
continuously improve their ability to play together. Even the mere act of engaging in
synchronous activities, such as marching, singing, or dancing, apparently can strengthen a group
by fostering individual members‘ attachment to it (Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009). All of these
activities can increase the ―social capital‖ of the group which, according to Oh, Labianca, and
Chung (2006), is beneficial both to the group and to its members.

Even though a group can explicitly decide to build its social capital, purposes that have to
do with strengthening the group itself often are latent and therefore not explicitly acknowledged
or addressed by group members or their leaders. For this reason, group-focused purposes have
for the most part been analyzed more by scholars in the psychodynamic tradition (e.g., Rioch,
1975) than by those who conduct empirical studies of group performance in laboratory or
organizational settings.

Fostering Individual Well-being

Groups also exist that have no specific task other than to foster the learning or well-being
of their own members. Such groups are found mainly in schools (Aronson & Patnoe, 1997;
Slavin, 1980; Thelen, 1981), in workshops intended to help individuals develop their
interpersonal and group skills (Argyris, 1993; Jaques & Salmon (2007), in self-help groups
(Alcoholics Anonymous, 2001; Zemore, Kaskutas, & Ammon, 2004), and in therapeutic settings
that rely on group techniques to help individuals deal with emotional problems (Joyce, Piper, &
Ogrodniczuk, 2007; Yalom & Leszcz, 2005). In all of these settings, the success of the group is
a direct function of degree to which individual members are helped.
-6-

Less well recognized, and less frequently studied, is the impact of group experiences on
the members of groups whose manifest purpose is something else, such as accomplishing a piece
of work or strengthening the group as a performing unit. In a study of a large number of
professional symphony orchestras, Allmendinger, Hackman, and Lehman (1996) found that most
orchestras accomplished their main work of performing concerts quite well—but at considerable
cost to individual orchestra members. The mean job satisfaction of orchestra players ranked
seventh of thirteen different kinds of groups that had been studied. And players ranked ninth of
thirteen on satisfaction with opportunities for personal growth and development, just below a
group of federal prison guards and just above an industrial production team. Group experiences
clearly can contribute positively to member well being, but they also can have the opposite effect
when the group is structured and led in ways that give collective accomplishment priority over
individual well-being.

Group Effectiveness

The simplest way to assess the overall effectiveness of a given group would be to simply
determine the degree to which it has achieved its purposes in each of the three domains just
identified. Specifically, a robust assessment of group effectiveness might involve collecting data
to generate answers to each of the following three questions (Hackman, 2002).

1. To what extent does the productive output of the group (i.e., its product, service,
decision, or performance) meet the standards of the team‘s clients—the people who
receive, review, or use the output?

2. To what extent do the group‘s social processes enhance members‘ collective


capability to work together interdependently?

3. To what extent does the group experience contribute positively to the learning and
personal well-being of individual members?

In practice, there are several nontrivial challenges and complications in using a


multidimensional, cross-level criterion of effectiveness such as this. As the orchestra study
mentioned above illustrates, there commonly are tradeoffs among the three purposes. There, the
work was accomplished at some cost to its members; one readily can imagine other
circumstances in which the reverse would be true, or in which building the capabilities of the
group itself would be at the expense of the group‘s task performance. In an experimental study
of group decision-making by Kaplan (1979), for example, some groups received an intervention
intended to improve the quality of members‘ interpersonal relationships. Compared to control
groups that received an innocuous intervention, members became so entranced by their
interpersonal explorations that they gave little attention to their work on subsequent tasks and
group performance deteriorated significantly.

Moreover, some purposes may be irrelevant or latent for some groups in some
circumstances. If a group were charged with a one-shot task of extraordinary importance such as
defusing a bomb before it exploded, one would not worry much about the impact of the group
-7-

experience on the group‘s capabilities or on members‘ learning. By contrast, those latter two
criteria would be highly relevant for the long-term effectiveness of a police department bomb
squad. Even though purposes having to do with group capability and individual learning might
be latent rather than manifest, the squad‘s effectiveness over the long term clearly would be
affected by the degree to which those latent purposes were accomplished.

Ultimately, the selection of the criteria to be used in assessing the effectiveness of a


purposive group is a value choice. But that reality is often unrecognized or unacknowledged—
for example, when a researcher constructs a task that has a simple performance measure such as
the right solution to a problem or the number of puzzles solved in a given time, or when a
practitioner finds something that can be readily counted, counts it, and then uses that as a
measure of the group‘s effectiveness. Both research and practice would benefit from more
thoughtful consideration of the criteria that are used to assess how well a group is performing.
Indeed, the mere act of actively discussing with group members the criteria that are most
appropriate for assessing their performance can make explicit that which may have been
unacknowledged and implicit—and, thereby, increase the chances that the group‘s performance
processes will be well aligned with the very criteria that are being discussed.

How Well Do Groups Perform?

That reasonable-sounding question has engendered a surprising level of partisan fervor.


Some scholars, such as Harold Leavitt (1975) enthusiastically tout the benefits of using groups to
accomplish work. In a classic essay titled ―Suppose we took groups seriously. . .‖, Leavitt
suggested that groups generate so many benefits that serious consideration should be given to
using them rather than individuals as the basic building blocks of organizations. Edwin Locke
and his colleagues take the contrary position in a provocative article titled ―The importance of
the individual in an age of groupism‖ (Locke, Tirnauer, Roberson, Goldman, Latham, &
Weldon, 2001). A ―group frenzy‖ has so overtaken organizational life, these authors argue, that
the critical role of individuals, especially in providing critical thinking, is being lost. Other
scholars concur that teams can be time-wasting social forms that coerce people and cap their
human potential (Allen & Hecht, 2004; Barker, 1993).

Both sides can marshal ample support for their positions, from essays and commentaries
to hard empirical data. On one side are books with highly promising titles such as Hot Groups
(Lipman-Blumen & Leavitt, 1999) and The Wisdom of Teams (Katzenbach & Smith, 1993) as
well as a good number of empirical studies that document just how well groups can perform. A
study by Groysberg, Healy, and Gui (2008), for example, assessed the performance of groups of
financial analysts that selected stocks to be purchased. They found that the stocks recommended
by a small selection committee (4-6 members) performed significantly better than either stocks
selected using traditional procedures or those selected by smaller or larger committees. On the
other side is Irving Janis‘s classic work on ―groupthink‖ (discussed later in this chapter) that
documents just how wrong groups can be in making highly consequential decisions; the
voluminous literature on free-riding (also known as ―social loafing‖ or the ―Ringlemann effect‖)
in task-performing groups (Cornes & Sandler, 1996; Karau & Williams, 1993; Mas & Moretti, in
press); and the decidedly mixed research findings about the performance benefits of group
-8-

brainstorming (Dugosh & Paulus, 2005; Litchfield, 2008; Nijstad & Stroebe, 2006; Parks &
Sanna, 1999; Paulus, Dugosh, Dzindolet, Coskun, & Putman, 2002).

Perhaps the most succinct statement of skepticism about group performance is the simple
equation that Steiner (1972) used to explore various models of group productivity: AP = PP –
PL. The actual productivity (AP) of a group, he shows, can be estimated by first identifying its
potential productivity (PP, what the group could achieve if all member resources were used
optimally) and then subtracting from that the process losses (PL) it experiences—for example,
motivation decrements such as free-riding, slippage in coordination, and inappropriate weighting
of members‘ inputs to the group‘s deliberations. Neither Steiner nor subsequent researchers
found it necessary to append a + PG (process gain) term to his equation. In the skeptical view,
then, the question for researchers and theorists is how far below its potential a group will fall, not
how much positive synergy members‘ interactions will generate.

The question of how well groups perform is sufficiently freighted with ideological
considerations that it is unlikely ever to be empirically answered in a way that is satisfactory to
all. Like many other matters that tap into ideological currents, this one will not be resolved
either by meta-analysis or by the design of a ―definitive‖ study whose findings will be
convincing both to the group-optimists and to the skeptics. In fact, it is the wrong question to
ask. It is wrong because it confounds three separate and quite distinct issues:

1. When should groups be used, and when should they not?


2. How does group performance compare to that of individuals?
3. What differentiates groups that realize their full potential from those that do not?

When groups, when not? In some circumstances there is no choice. Consider, for
example, the generic task of flying an aircraft. Only individuals can fly single-seat planes, and
only groups can operate planes that require synchronized input from multiple crew members.
But technological imperatives such as these are a special case. Are there more general
circumstances when groups are an inappropriate or ill-advised means to accomplish a piece of
work? Although little systematic research has been done on this question, some evidence is
available. Creative composition, for example, involves bringing to the surface, organizing, and
expressing thoughts and ideas that are but partially formed or that reside in one's unconscious.
As Miles Shore (2008) has found in his research on dyadic creativity, such work is inherently
more suitable for individual than for collective performance. Even committee reports, mundane
products compared to novels, poems, or musical scores, invariably are better done by one
talented individual on behalf of a group than by the group as a whole writing in lockstep. The
social system context within which a task is performed also can strongly mitigate against
teamwork—for example, when organizational policies, practices, or culture make it impossible
to properly design and support teams (Walton & Hackman, 1986).

Groups tend to be remarkably passive and accepting when given a task that is
inappropriate for a collective work, or when the group is poorly structured or inadequately
supported, or when the broader social context is unfriendly to collaboration. A case in point is
the arrangement of the physical space where the group does its work. As Steele (1973) has
found, groups tend to view their spatial arrangements as fixed even when their work space could
-9-

easily be reconfigured. Members are far more likely to unquestioningly solider on in such
circumstances than to rearrange the space to make it more amenable to teamwork. The same
may be true for other structural or contextual features that impair group processes or
performance: although they potentially are open to group-initiated change, members take them as
given. At present, little is known about the roots of this kind of passivity or what it would take to
increase group proactivity about such matters. It would be good to know more.

Group versus individual performance. Among the first questions addressed in small
group research was the relative performance of individuals and groups (Collins & Guetzkow,
1964), and the matter continues to engage group scholars (e.g., Kerr, MacCoun, & Kramer,
1996; Laughlin, VanderStoep, & Hollingshead, 1991; Stasser & Dietz-Uhler, 2001). In one of
the very first studies of individual vs. group problem solving, Shaw (1932) asked 21 individuals
and five four-person groups to solve an intellective problem. Three of the five groups (60%)
solved the problem correctly, as did three of the 21 individuals (14%). Shaw concluded that
groups were superior at problem-solving, perhaps because group members corrected one
another‘s errors as they worked through the problem.

Many years later, Marquart (1955) corrected Shaw‘s logic by noting that any group
would be successful in solving Shaw‘s problem if it included a member who was able to solve it
alone. Perhaps, he suggested, the performance of a group was more a matter of its composition
than the quality of members‘ interaction. In a replication of Shaw‘s study, Marquart created
―nominal‖ groups by randomly assigning individuals who had worked on the problem alone to
hypothetical groups that never actually met. The number of nominal groups that contained a
member who had solved the problem closely approximated the number of real groups that solved
it. The performance of groups on this problem, he demonstrated, reflected the performance of its
most competent member. Subsequently, Steiner (1966) noted that the problem used by Shaw and
Marquart was a special kind of task for which group performance tracks that of its best member.
He then went further and identified five distinct types of tasks for which different combinatorial
rules apply.

-- Disjunctive tasks, for which the performance of the group as a whole is a direct
function of the performance of its best-performing group member. For example: a
track team whose score is that of its fastest runner, or a team of mathematicians that
succeeds when any member comes up with a proof that works.

-- Conjunctive tasks, for which the group operates at the level of its least competent
member. For example: a study group in a machine-paced language course, or a roped-
together team of mountain climbers.

-- Additive tasks, for which group performance is the sum of members‘ contributions. For
example: a production team in which members are working in parallel, or a tug-of-war
in which the group‘s ―pull‖ is the sum of the pulls of all its members.

-- Compensatory tasks, for which a group estimate is the simple average of individual
members' independent estimates. The expectation is that individuals' errors will be
compensated for by others' errors in the opposite direction. For example: estimating
- 10 -

the number of beans in a jar or the number of people present at an outdoor event,
predicting next year's fossil fuel consumption.

-- Complementary tasks, which can be divided into subtasks that are assigned to different
members. For example: a research task that requires different activities for which
members may be differentially skilled; assembly of a complex device for which
division of labor brings greater efficiency than otherwise would be the case.

By disaggregating the kinds of tasks that groups perform, Steiner laid the groundwork for
much more informative comparisons of individual and group performance than previously had
been possible. The present lack of consensus among scholars about the relative performance of
groups and individuals, noted earlier, reflects in part the fact that groups and individuals are
differentially advantaged for the various types of tasks that Steiner identified. It is regrettable
that contemporary group scholars do not draw more extensively upon his simple but powerful
conceptualization in framing and interpreting their research on group behavior and performance.

Achieving groups’ full potential. A very large group performing a disjunctive task
theoretically should do very well, because its most competent member should be quite talented
indeed. But there is no guarantee, since process problems—in this case, the risk that the best
member‘s contribution will be ignored or lost in the shuffle in a large group—can derail even
groups that have the resources needed to perform extraordinarily well (Straus, Parker, Bruce, &
Dembrosky, 2009).

More generally, competent individuals, either working either alone or nominal groups,
can outperform interacting groups that are poorly structured and supported. And a great group
can generate synergistic outcomes that exceed what would be produced even by extraordinarily
competent individuals (Laughlin, Bonner, & Miner, 2002), and do so in a way that
simultaneously strengthens the group as a performing unit and contributes to the learning and
development of individual members. It all depends on the degree to which the group has, and
uses well, the full complement of resources that are required for exceptional performance.

An emerging theme in current research on group performance is identification of those


aspects of the group‘s structure, its context, and the behavior of its leaders that, together, increase
the likelihood that a group will evolve into an effective performing unit (Ancona & Bresman,
2007; Hackman, 2002; Wageman, Nunes, Burruss, & Hackman, 2008). In this way of thinking,
groups are viewed more as social systems that chart their own development than as mechanistic
entities in which specific causes are tightly linked to specific effects. Although integrated
treatments such as these do draw on research findings about specific factors that affect
performance outcomes, their emphasis is on identifying the general conditions that increase the
chances that a group will be able to use well its full complement of resources in pursuing its
purposes.

Attributes of Purposive Groups


As noted earlier, there has been a proliferation of new forms of groups in recent years,
including many that bear only modest resemblance to those that historically have been most
prominent in group research—the small groups created by researchers for laboratory
- 11 -

experimental studies and the organizational work teams studied by field researchers. As a
consequence, the development of cumulative knowledge about group behavior and performance
has become more daunting than it was previously, since there is now greater risk that what is
learned from research on one type of group may not generalize to groups of very different kinds.
It may be useful, therefore, to identify the attributes that can be used to partition the universe of
purposive groups, thereby facilitating comparisons across types of groups as well as assessments
of the external validity of empirical findings about groups of various kinds.

Four attributes that can be used to distinguish among different types of groups are: (1) the
degree to which responsibility for achieving group purposes lies primarily with the group as a
whole versus with individual members, (2) the degree to which members interact synchronously
in real time vs. asynchronously at their own discretion, (3) the level of authority groups have to
manage their own processes, and (4) the substantive type of work the group is performing.

Responsibility and Synchrony

The first two attributes, taken together, identify four distinct types of purposive groups,
shown in the table below. As will be seen, some of these types are both more common and more
commonly studied than are others.

Responsibility for Achieving Group Purposes

Individual Members Group as a Whole

Real-Time "Surgical" Teams Face-to-face Groups


Interaction

Level of Synchronicity

Asynchronous Coacting Groups Virtual Teams


Interaction

Adapted from Hackman & Wageman (2005)

Face-to-face groups, the upper right quadrant in the table, are what people usually have in
mind when they talk about groups and teams, and most of the existing research literature on
group behavior and performance is about them. Members of such groups are co-located and
work together interdependently in real time to accomplish purposes for which they are
collectively responsible. They most frequently are used when achieving the group‘s purposes
requires coordinated contributions in real time from a diversity of members who have
- 12 -

complementary expertise, experience, and perspectives (e.g., Mathieu, Heffner, Goodwin, Salas,
& Cannon-Bowers, 2000; Stewart & Barrick, 2000).

In the lower right-hand quadrant of the matrix are virtual teams, which sometimes also
are called distributed or dispersed groups. Members of virtual teams also share responsibility
and accountability for accomplishing collective purposes, but they are not co-located and they do
not necessarily interact with one another in real time. With the rapid recent advances in
information and communication technologies, members are able to interact mainly (and
sometimes exclusively) using electronic means and on their own schedules. Virtual teams often
are larger, more diverse, and collectively more knowledgeable than those whose members
interact face-to-face. When they work well, they can bring widely dispersed information and
expertise to bear on the team's work quickly and efficiently (Kirkman, Rosen, Tesluk, & Gibson,
2004; Townsend, DeMarie, & Hendrickson, 1998). Virtual teams are most frequently used when
interdependent work is required but it would be difficult or impossible for team members to
perform it face-to-face. As will be seen later in this chapter, researchers presently are working to
identify the special conditions, beyond the mere availability of sophisticated communication
capabilities, that are required for such teams to overcome their special challenges.

Groups in the upper-left quadrant are what Brooks (1995) has called surgical teams.
Responsibility and accountability for outcomes lies primarily with one person, the surgeon, but
accomplishing that work requires coordinated interaction among all members in real time to
ensure that he or she has all the information and assistance that members can provide. Brooks
noted that the best software development teams are of this type, with members working closely
together but with one individual, the lead programmer, having primary responsibility for the
quality of the team‘s product. This kind of team is most often seen when a group‘s purposes
require an extremely high level of individual insight, expertise, or creativity—metaphorically,
the writing of a play rather than its performance.

Responsibility for the performance of groups in the lower-left quadrant, which are known
as coacting groups, also lies primarily with individual members. Each member's work does not
depend upon what the others do, and the output of the group as a whole is simply the aggregation
of individual members‘ contributions. Because there is no particular reason for members to
coordinate their activities in real time, they may or may not work in close proximity to one
another (when they do not, they are essentially the same as the nominal groups that sometimes
are used in research to assess the relative performance of interacting groups). A great deal of
organizational work is performed by sets of people that are called teams but that actually are
coacting groups. Although coacting groups cannot generate synergistic collective products
(because members are merely operating in parallel), they can benefit from social facilitation
when members work in one another‘s presence and therefore can observe others performing the
same well-learned task. And, of course, the presence of coactors also can impair performance
when the work requires production of unfamiliar responses (Feinberg & Aiello, 2006; Zajonc,
1965) or when the group is so large that members are tempted to free-ride on others‘
contributions (Harkins & Szymanksi, 1989; Latane, Williams, & Harkins, 1979). Coacting
groups are most often found in settings where there is minimal need for interdependent work by
relatively homogeneous sets of group members.
- 13 -

Level of Authority

Four generic functions must be fulfilled when a group pursues its particular purposes
(Hackman, 1986). First, of course, is to execute the work. Second is to monitor and manage
work processes, collecting and interpreting data about how the group is operating and then
making corrections as needed. Third is designing the group itself and securing any outside
resources or support that may be needed. Fourth is choosing or defining the purpose itself.
Groups vary in the amount of authority they have to fulfill these four functions—the decision-
making latitude of some groups is highly restricted, whereas others have control over all aspects
of group life and work.

For the most restricted groups, most decision-making authority is held by an external
agent. This agent will be referred to as the group‘s ―manager,‖ although in practice the agent
may be an experimenter, a therapist, or even some other group, depending on the group‘s
purpose and the setting in which it operates. In a manager-led group, members have authority
only to actually execute the task; others monitor and manage performance processes, structure
the group and its context, and specify overall purposes. This type of group was common in
U.S. industry in the decades after the idea of ―scientific management‖ took hold early in the 20 th
century—managers managed, workers worked, and the distinction between the two rarely was
violated (Taylor, 1911). Manager-led groups continue to be seen today, not just in work
organizations where electronic technologies can facilitate continuous monitoring of team
activities, but also for performing groups, such as a football team whose coach sends in every
play or an orchestra whose players‘ only responsibility is to execute competently the conductor‘s
instructions (Allmendinger, Hackman, & Lehman, 1996).

A self-managing group, in addition to its responsibility for executing the work, also has
the authority to monitor and manage its own activities. This type of group has become
prominent in organizations that seek to counter the dysfunctions of scientific management by
fostering member commitment to collective purposes (Cummings, 1978; Kirkman & Shapiro,
1997; Wall, Kemp, Jackson, & Clegg, 1986; Walton, 1985). It also is commonplace in
professional work—for example, a team of research assistants that has the authority to manage
data collection processes within constraints set by, and for purposes chosen by, the principal
investigator. Groups created in the laboratory for experimental purposes generally are either
self-managing or, when instructions are tightly scripted, manager led.

Members of self-designing groups have the authority to modify the structure of the group
itself or aspects of its context that affect group behavior. Although others specify the purposes
of such groups, members themselves have full authority to do whatever needs to be done to get
group purposes accomplished. Many leadership teams in organizations are self-designing, as
are task forces of various kinds. A faculty committee given a charge to review the college‘s
curriculum, for example, would be self-designing if members had the authority to change its own
composition, perhaps by adding student members, or to alter aspects of its context, perhaps by
instructing the registrar to provide enrollment data in a nontraditional format.

Finally, self-governing groups have authority to deal with all four of the functions listed
above: members decide about the group‘s purposes, structure the group and aspects of its
- 14 -

context, manage their own performance processes, and actually carry out the work. Professional
string quartets exemplify self-governing groups (Butterworth, 1990; Murnighan & Conlon,
1991), as do some legislative bodies, corporate boards of directors, volunteer community service
groups, and worker cooperatives.

The level of a group‘s authority powerfully shapes both its internal dynamics and its
relationships with those who create, manage, support, or are served by the group (Smith & Berg,
1987). The less powerful group members are relative to their leader or manager, for example,
the more they exhibit passivity and obedience in their interactions (Ancona & Nadler, 1989).
Authority dynamics rarely are explicitly acknowledged or discussed by group members, in part
because it is anxiety-arousing to do so but also because many of those dynamics operate below
the level of conscious awareness (Argyris, 1969; Bion, 1961). It is challenging, therefore, to
empirically study authority dynamics in laboratory experiments or field studies that rely on
standard research methodologies. Even though such dynamics clearly are present in those
settings (for example, in the role and behavior of the experimenter in laboratory studies, or the
manager in organizational settings), standard research methods are likely to capture only the
surface manifestations of deeper and less accessible forces.

Because of these limitations, little is known about the differences in authority dynamics
that characterize group interaction in manager-led, self-managing, self-designing, self-governing
groups. Research in the psychodynamic tradition, to be discussed later in this chapter, suggests
that these dynamics may be quite powerful in shaping what happens in groups that have different
levels of authority. But as of this writing, bridges between the psychodynamic tradition and
what has been learned from normal science studies of group behavior and performance remain to
be built.

Type of Work

There is no shortage of research on groups that perform different kinds of work. To


illustrate, here is merely a sampling of the different kinds of groups that have been the subject of
empirical research.

-- Groups that produce things, such as industrial work groups (Abramis, 1990), product
development teams (Ancona & Caldwell, 1992), and software development teams
(Faraj & Sproull, 2000).

-- Groups that provide services to people, such as medical care (Denison & Sutton, 1990),
social and community support (Cline, 1999; Howell, Brock, & Hauser, 2003), and
psychotherapy (Forsyth, 2001).

-- Groups that decide things, such as juries (Hastie, Penrod, & Pennington, 1981; Kerr,
Niedermeier, & Kaplan, 1999; Stasser & Davis, 1981; Tindale, Nadler, Krebel, &
Davis, 2001), personnel selection teams (Tracy & Standerfer, 2003), and policy-
making teams (Janis, 1982).
- 15 -

-- Groups that provide organizational and institutional leadership (Edmondson, Roberto,


& Watkins, 2003; Wageman, Nunes, Burruss, & Hackman, 2008).

-- Groups that advocate and manage change, such as environmental advocacy groups
(Ganz & Wageman, 2009), consulting groups (Sherblom, 2003), and organization
development groups (Hackman & Edmondson, 2008).

-- Groups that conduct research, such as intelligence analysis teams (Hackman


&Woolley, in press) and scientific collaborations (Cummings & Kiesler, 2005).

-- Groups that facilitate learning, such as classroom groups (Aronson & Patnoe, 1997;
Johnson & Johnson, 1998) and student project teams (Druskat & Kayes, 2000;
Gersick, 1990).

-- Groups that mount performances, such as sports teams (Wood, 1990) and musical
ensembles (Murnighan & Conlon, 1991).

-- Groups that deal with adversarial or crisis situations, such as negotiating teams
(Behfar, Friedman, & Brett, 2008), military teams (Salas, Bowers, & Cannon-Bowers,
1995), and crisis management teams (Klein, Ziegert, Knight, & Xiao, 2006).

We have learned a great deal about commonly-studied types of groups, such as juries,
organizational production and service teams, and temporary groups in the special context of the
experimental laboratory. But much less is known about how the nature of a group‘s work affects
either the character of group life or the emergent beliefs and attitudes of group members. It is
one thing to do intellectual work with professionals, as leadership and consulting teams do, but
quite another to put on a performance, to turn out an industrial product, or to engage an
adversary in combat. The generalizability of findings from groups that do one kind of work to
those that do very different things remains mostly unexplored.

It is possible that comparisons across groups that perform different types of work would
be at least as informative as comparisons involving the other attributes identified above—the
degree of synchronicity in group interaction, the locus of responsibility for group outcomes, and
the group‘s level of authority. It is unrealistic, however, to expect that many researchers will
conduct research that explicitly compares groups that differ on these attributes. What is feasible
is for researchers to include in their reports much more detail about the attributes of the groups
they have studied than they typically do. That small innovation could greatly assist other
scholars in assessing the generality of the findings obtained, and perhaps even prompt new
insights about those attributes of groups that are of greatest consequence or conceptual interest.

A standard claim of cultural anthropologists is that any person is in some ways like all
other people, like some other people, and like no other person (Kluckhohn & Murray, 1953).
The same is true for groups: a particular group is in some ways like all others (which invites the
development of general theory), like some others (which invites mid-range theory), and like no
other (which invites case studies). Case studies that focus on the ―like no other‖ reality provide
detailed accounts of specific groups but without reference to other groups of other kinds. They
- 16 -

are valuable for teaching purposes, to be sure. But they also can provide deep understanding of
particular groups that may be of special interest or importance and they can prompt ideas and
hypotheses that would not have surfaced in the absence of detailed description.

The reality that all groups are like some other groups invites the development of mid-
range theories that address the commonalities among all groups of a particular type, such as all
juries or all industrial work teams. Because mid-range theories do not extend to groups of
wholly different kinds, they can divert attention from generalities that actually do apply to all
groups. That raises the third and most ambitious possibility, general theory that does purport to
apply to all groups but that also includes ―translation rules‖ to guide the application of the
general-level constructs to particular kinds of groups and contexts. The clarity of a group‘s
goals, for example, is just as meaningful for an open-source programming group as it is for an
athletic team or an industrial work group—but its salience, manifestations, and potency surely
vary across these settings. Empirical and conceptual work that explicitly addresses the
translation from what is known about groups in general to groups of particular kinds could
contribute substantially to the eventual development of demonstrably general models of group
behavior and performance.

PART TWO:
APPROACHES TO THE STUDY OF GROUPS

As suggested at the beginning of this chapter, group research is in a transitional period —


the phenomena are changing as new group forms emerge, and the scholars who study them now
come from a wider range of disciplines than ever before. It also is likely that new conceptual
approaches will be required to develop robust understanding of the emerging new forms. If that
is so, then it may be instructive to re-examine the scholarly approaches that have guided group
research over the decades. Buried in that history are ideas and perspectives that, if taken off the
shelf and inspected, could help guide contemporary scholars in further advancing research and
theory about group behavior and performance (McGrath, 1997).

This section examines six distinct approaches to the study of groups that have been, or
are now becoming, influential in small group research. They are: psychodynamic, network,
action, process-focused, decision analytic, and complex systems approaches. These six
approaches are complementary lenses for understanding and studying groups. Each lens brings
certain phenomena into sharp focus, yet can obscure other phenomena. Drawing on multiple
lenses in analyzing a group, therefore, can generate a substantially more robust understanding
than otherwise would be the case.

Other scholars have divided the research into some of the same, but also some different
categories (e.g. Poole & Hollingshead, 2005; Wheelan, 2005). This section does not delve into
the nuances of these different categorization schemes. Rather, the goal of this section is to
provide, for each approach, a vivid depiction of its origins, to highlight important recent
contributions, and to identify its distinctive strengths and prospects.
- 17 -

The Psychodynamic Approach


The psychodynamic approach to the analysis of groups focuses on social and emotional
forces that are hidden from view but that nonetheless can powerfully shape individual and group
behavior. Although this approach draws heavily on psychoanalytic theories, it originated not
with a Vienna analyst but with a French sociologist—Gustave Le Bon. In his book, The Crowd:
A Study of the Popular Mind (1895), Le Bon asserted that when individuals become part of a
group, ―the sentiments and ideas of all the persons in the gathering take one and the same
direction, and their conscious personality vanishes‖ (p. 43). The mechanisms for this
transformation, according to Le Bon, were heightened affectivity, suggestibility, and emotional
contagion.

Le Bon viewed groups as more driven by instincts than individuals, and therefore less
rational. He believed the capacity of a group to make wise decisions was always less than the
capacity of its individual members. This was true for all groups—not just crowds but also elite
groups vested with important decision making authority. For example, even juries and
parliamentary assemblies were, in Le Bon‘s view, incapable of exercising prudence and good
judgment. Imposing democratic structures on such groups, he believed, would not tame their
unruly instincts.

Le Bon‘s pessimistic views were reprised in the early 20th century by British psychologist
William McDougall. In his book The Group Mind (1920), written after his psychoanalysis with
C. G. Jung, McDougall (1920) described groups as ―…excessively emotional, impulsive, violent,
fickle, inconsistent, irresolute, and extreme in action, displaying only the coarser emotions and
the less refined sentiments; extremely suggestible, careless in deliberation, hasty in judgment,
incapable of any but the simpler and imperfect forms of reasoning; easily swayed and led,
lacking in self-consciousness, devoid of self-respect and sense of responsibility, and apt to be
carried away by the consciousness of its own force‖ (p. 64).

But McDougall did not share Le Bon‘s view that all groups were condemned to such a
fate—only simple, unorganized groups were vulnerable. Organized groups, he asserted, could
do better. He identified five conditions that could help in avoiding the worst aspects of group
life: (1) a sense of continuity; (2) a sense of collective self consciousness; (3) specialization of
functions among members, (4) interaction with other groups, and (5) group-generated traditions,
customs, and habits.

Le Bon and McDougall greatly influenced Sigmund Freud‘s thinking about groups, who
cited them approvingly in his essay, ―Group Psychology and the Analysis of the Ego‖ (Freud,
1922/1959). In particular, he resonated with Le Bon‘s notion that unconscious motives can drive
a group‘s behavior but, following McDougall, he also asserted that ―organization‖ can check a
group‘s primitive instincts and enable it to operate rationality. For Freud, organization is to the
group what the ego is to the id; it restrains primitive instincts so that the group becomes capable
of coordination and control.

The first psychoanalyst to apply Freud‘s techniques to a group as a whole was Wilfred
Bion, whose thinking had been shaped by his mentor and analyst, Melanie Klein. As an officer
in the British Army during WW II, Bion accumulated considerable experience working with
- 18 -

groups—he led therapy groups in a military psychiatric hospital and also introduced the
―leaderless group‖ as a device for identifying those soldiers who would make the best officers
(Bion & Rickman, 1943). From these experiences, he concluded that all groups oscillate
between rational and irrational states. When in the rational state, the group is ruled by what Bion
called the ―work group,‖ focused on the stated task. When in the irrational state, the group is
ruled by any one of three ―basic assumptions.‖ The basic assumptions operate outside of
awareness, but profoundly shape the group‘s behavior (Bion, 1952, 1961).

According to Bion, the first basic assumption (―dependence‖) is that the group leader is
omniscient and omnipotent, and the group members need his or her protection. The group might
resent feeling dependent and helpless, but is determined to remain so. The second basic
assumption (―pairing‖) is that the group‘s purpose is reproduction; the group will, at some future
time, bring forth a messianic figure. The group is suffused with optimism. The third basic
assumption (―fight or flight‖) is that the group‘s life is at risk, and the group faces a choice
between battling or running away. The dominant emotion is fear. Only one basic assumption is
in evidence at any one moment, but over time the group can veer from one assumption to
another. These basic assumptions interfere profoundly with the effective functioning of the work
group.

If a group is to function well, Bion argued, it must explicitly acknowledge these basic
assumptions. When a group brings the basic assumptions into consciousness and consistently
interprets them, they lose their destructive power. An essential aspect of competent group
functioning, therefore, is to identify and acknowledge the influence of the basic assumptions and
the strong emotions they foster so the energy of the basic assumptions can serve the work group.

A more recent exemplar of the psychodynamic approach is the work of Smith and Berg
(1987) on the paradoxes of group life. Smith and Berg observe that groups are pervaded by a
wide range of tensions that defy members‘ attempts at resolution and often spawn circular
processes that block forward movement. The tensions and contradictions that members
experience, Smith and Berg argue, actually reflect true paradoxes—that is, both sides of the
tension are simultaneously true. Among the paradoxes of group life they identify is the paradox
of trust: ―For trust to develop in a group, members must trust the group and the group must trust
its members, for it is only through trusting that trust is built‖ (p. 641). The paradox of identity is
―expressed in the struggle of individuals and the group to establish a unique and meaningful
identity by attempting to indicate how each is separate from the other, while all the time turning
out to actually be affirming the ways each is an integral part of the other‖ (p. 639). The paradox
of authority is that ―members must subordinate their autonomy to the group for it to become
strong enough to represent members‘ collective interests; yet, in authorizing the group, members
may diminish themselves and lessen the capacities of the group which derives its potency from
the strength of its members‖ (p. 645). Not all group conflicts should be (or even can be)
resolved, Smith and Berg argue, because when members think in terms of paradoxes rather than
conflicts, it becomes clear that the tensions they are experiencing, although frustrating, are an
integral aspect of group life.

Psychodynamic approaches continue to be seen in research on group dynamics,


leadership, and intergroup relationship—although more as a trickle than a steady stream (e.g.,
- 19 -

Alderfer, 1987; Gillette & McCollom, 1990; Heifetz, 1998; Kets de Vries & Carlock, 2007).
Psychodynamic constructs seem, on the whole, obsolete. Yet some of the assumptions that
undergird the psychodynamic perspective are receiving new validation. Scholars increasingly
recognize the power of emotional dynamics and implicit processes in shaping group behavior
(Clark & Sline, 2003; McLeod & Kettner-Polley, 2005). Moreover, research on intergroup
perceptions using the Implicit Association Test (Nosek, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2007) suggests
that forces of which group members are wholly unaware can nonetheless profoundly affect what
happens in groups (Dovidio, Kawakami, & Gaertner, 2002).

The Network Approach


The network approach to understanding group behavior, which charts and analyzes the
relationships among group members, both complements and contrasts the work of
psychodynamic scholars. It is complementary in that it also focuses on emotions and the
affective bonds among people. It departs from the psychodynamic tradition, however, by
attending mainly to members‘ explicit, conscious feelings about one another. Jacob Levy
Moreno, the founder of this approach, captures that difference in describing first meeting, as a
young man, with the much-older Sigmund Freud: ―I said [to Freud], I start where you leave off.
You meet people in the artificial setting of your office. I meet them on the street and in their
homes, in their natural surroundings. You analyze [people‘s] dreams. I give them the courage to
dream again‖ (Marineau, 1989, p. 30).

Moreno invented the term, the concept, and the methodology of sociometry, which
captures what he referred to as ―the flow of feeling and sentiment‖ among group members.
Moreno‘s methodology consisted of first surveying all the members of a group to determine their
feelings about one another. This would show whether the affect between each member and
every other member was positive or negative, and whether that sentiment was one-way or
mutual. He then would create a diagram of the group. Each group member would be a node in
the diagram, and the lines connecting nodes would represent the ties—positive or negative, uni-
or bi-directional—between members. The resultant sociogram revealed what he called the
group‘s depth structure as opposed to its formal structure.

The sociogram was a useful tool for operationalizing group-level constructs (Moreno,
1934, 1943). For example, when the sociogram revealed many mutual positive ties among group
members, the group showed high ―cohesion.‖ When the network ties formed complex structures,
such as chains, triangles, or squares, the group showed high ―integration.‖ When there were
many mutual repulsions and one-way attractions, the group exhibited ―disorganization and
disharmony.‖ Moreno applied this technique not only to relationships between individuals in a
group, but to relationships between groups, and to relationships between individuals and groups.

By tracking the changing pattern of network ties in a group or a society (which he called
sociodynamics), Moreno believed he could predict many important phenomena, including the
type of leadership that would emerge (democratic versus autocratic), the level of morale, the
emergence of cliques, the alienation of minority groups, the spread of public opinion, and the
distribution of wealth and power. Moreover, he asserted that the extent of the discrepancy
between a group‘s formal social system and its sociogram determined the level of instability and
conflict in the group (Moreno, 1934).
- 20 -

Moreno‘s writings convey an exuberant sense of possibility for what sociometry could
contribute to understanding and improving group behavior. He dreamed of a future society in
which every group‘s formal structure would mirror its depth structure, with all people free to join
and leave groups as they wished. Under those conditions, every group would be a setting where
creativity and spontaneity were unleashed (Nehnevajsa, 1955). Sociometry thereby could provide
―the cornerstone of a still undeveloped science of democracy‖ (Moreno, 1941, p. 35), which
turned out to be a prescient aspiration given the increasing use of electronic technologies to
create and deploy social networks in pursuit of democratic ideals (Noveck, 2009; Shirky, 2008).

The sociometric approach found a highly receptive audience in Kurt Lewin, and the
journal Moreno founded, Sociometry, became the venue for a number of classic studies by Lewin
and his colleagues on the social psychology group behavior (e.g. Lewin & Lippitt, 1938; Lewin,
Lippitt, & White, 1939). As early as 1935, Lewin had concluded that the study of groups had
reached an impasse and would not realize its value if it continued to focus on purely descriptive
analysis. Because Lewin viewed the rigorous, systematic nature of sociometry as a way to
illuminate causal relationships through experimental methods, he adopted sociometric techniques
for use in his own research.

In 1945, Lewin established the Research Center for Group Dynamics at MIT. The center
was devoted to conducting controlled experiments in both laboratory and field settings (Lewin,
1945). The center became an intellectual incubator where Lewin and his mentees designed
imaginative studies of group communication (e.g. Bavelas, 1950; Guetzkow & Gyr, 1954;
Guetzkow & Simon, 1955; Leavitt, 1951; Leavitt & Mueller, 1951) that generated hundreds of
published articles (Monge & Contractor, 2003).

The early method for studying social networks at the Research Center for Group
Dynamics was as follows: study participants were randomly assigned to seats at adjoining
cubicles. Participants could not see one another but could slip written messages to select others
through slots in the walls. The researchers constrained who could pass notes to whom, thereby
manipulating the pattern of ties and flow of information. Researchers compared the impact of
various network configurations (e.g. circle, chain, y, wheel, and all-channel) on group
functioning and performance.

After a flurry of early studies, the network approach to studying small groups became
dormant from the 1960s through the 1980s. In the last fifteen years, however, this approach has
resurfaced with renewed vigor, in part because of the availability of sophisticated software that
quantifies key network features and creates visual displays of network patterns. Here is a
sampling of findings that illustrate the kinds of things that can be learned from network studies.
Groups with more internal ties outperform groups with fewer ties (Baldwin, Bedell, & Johnson,
1997; Reagans & Zuckerman, 2001). Teams with complex tasks perform better if their internal
network is relatively decentralized (Sparrowe, Liden, Wayne, & Kraimer, 2001). The internal
and external friendship ties of team leaders predict team performance (Mehra, Dixon, Brass, &
Robertson, 2006).

Perhaps the greatest strength of the network approach is that it enables researchers to
bridge the divide between a group‘s internal processes and its external context (Katz, Lazer,
- 21 -

Arrow, & Contractor 2004, 2005). With newly available technologies and analytic methods, the
approach is now so powerful that network methods may soon become indispensible in social
research. As Lazer et al. (2009) note, people now leave an enormous number of ―digital traces‖
in their wake every day. By applying network methods to this vast aggregate of data, researchers
now can study social processes, including the dynamics of widely distributed groups, at a level
of detail never before possible.

The Action Approach


The action approach involves the development and use of group methods specifically to
achieve positively valued outcomes. It seeks simultaneously to solve practical problems and to
discover general laws of group life (Peters & Robinson, 1984). Typically, the approach involves
a spiral process of data collection, feedback of results, and then more data collection—involving
throughout close collaboration with the people being studied.

Action research is most closely identified with Kurt Lewin who, in an early issue of the
Journal of Social Issues, called on his colleagues to commit themselves to research in which ―the
diagnosis has to be complemented by comparative studies of the effectiveness of various
techniques of change... Research that produces nothing but books will not suffice‖ (Lewin,
1946, p. 203). Lewin‘s advocacy of action research was not merely an intellectual interest; it
was driven by the profound anti-Semitism he witnessed and experienced while growing up
Jewish in Europe. According to his social psychologist daughter, after learning that his mother,
aunt, and cousins all had been killed in Nazi concentration camps he resolved to devote the rest
of his career to conducting research that would directly combat bigotry and discrimination
(Mariam Lewin, 1992).

Lewin not only coined the term action research but also was a pioneer in conducting it.
In explaining why he relied on group methods in his action research projects, Lewin (1947a)
wrote:
"Experience in leadership training, in changing of food habits, work production,
criminality, alcoholism, prejudices, all seem to indicate that it is usually easier to
change individuals formed into a group than to change any one separately. As long
as group values are unchanged the individual will resist changes more strongly the
farther he is to depart from group standards. If the group standard itself is changed,
the resistance which is due to the relation between individual and group standard is
eliminated" (p. 34).

One of Lewin‘s first action research projects was a two-week workshop, conducted in the
summer of 1946 with his colleagues from the MIT Group Dynamics Research Laboratory, on
behalf of the Advisory Committee on Race Relations of the State of Connecticut. The goal was
to enhance the skills of teachers and social workers in dealing with interracial issues, and the
primary methodology was small group discussion and role playing. Participants described
problems in race relations that they were facing back home and then practiced alternative ways
of understanding and dealing with those problems.

The experimental manipulation was based on Lewin‘s three-step model of change. Step
1 is ―unfreezing‖: confronted with a dilemma, or a disconfirmation of existing beliefs, an
- 22 -

individual or group becomes aware of a need for change. Step 2 is ―changing‖: participants
experiment with new ways of behaving. Step 3 is ―freezing‖: the new behavior is evaluated, and
if positively reinforced, incorporated into the individual or group‘s repertoire. Lewin predicted
that those participants who came to the workshop in intact groups from their communities would
show greater and more permanent attitudinal and behavioral change than those who came as
individuals, thereby affirming the power of the group as a tool for social change.

At the end of the workshop‘s first day, the research team met to review members‘ notes
and listen to audiotapes of the sessions. To the dismay of the rest of the research team, Lewin
invited participants also to sit in on the debriefing. What transpired as participants discussed
their behavior and its consequences galvanized everyone present. Here is how Benne (1984, p.
82) subsequently described the process:

―A research observer might report [from his notes]: At 10:00 a.m. Mrs. X. attacked
the group leader. Mr. Y came to the defense of the leader, and he and Mrs. X
became involved in a heated exchange. Some other members were drawn into
taking sides. Other members seemed frightened and tried to make peace. But they
were ignored by the combatants. At 10:10 a.m., the leader came in to redirect
attention back to the problem, which had been forgotten in the exchange. Mrs. X
and Mr. Y continued to contradict each other in the discussion that followed.

Immediately, Mrs. X denied and Mr. Y defended the accuracy of the observation.
Other members reinforced or qualified the data furnished by the observer. In brief,
participants began to join observers and training leaders in trying to analyze and
interpret behavioral events…. Participants reported that they were deriving
important understandings of their own behavior and of the behavior of their groups.
To the training staff it seemed that a potentially powerful medium and process of
re-education had been, somewhat inadvertently, hit upon.‖

This experiment—in particular what happened during the informal debriefing sessions—
led to a follow-up series of group workshops at the National Training Laboratory in Bethel,
Maine. The Bethel groups were called ―basic training groups‖ (―T-groups‖ for short). T-groups
had multiple purposes: (1) to deepen participants‘ understanding of group dynamics and
development; (2) to develop participants‘ skill in facilitating group effectiveness; (3) to increase
participants‘ insight into themselves and their impact on others; (4) to provide an opportunity to
experiment with new behaviors; (5) to enhance participants‘ ability to give and receive feedback,
and (6) to make constructive use of conflict.

To achieve these aspirations, groups focused on members‘ thoughts, feelings, and


behaviors as they occurred and were experienced in real time. Moreover, they were expected to
learn how to engage fully in the group emotionally while simultaneously cultivating an analytic
detachment that would allow them to learn from their feelings (Yalom, 1995). T-groups became
extremely popular in the 1960s and 1970s as a form of management training and development,
and provided the foundation for the field of organization development. Offshoots of T-groups,
such as sensitivity training and encounter groups, spread beyond industry to the wider culture.
- 23 -

As the years passed, interest in T-Groups began to wane, in part because the lessons
participants learned on the ―cultural island‖ that the groups provided tended not to persist in their
back-home environments. This development was especially troublesome to social psychologist
Chris Argyris, who had been a leader in the T-Group movement and who, like Lewin, had great
faith in the power of small groups as a crucible for individual change (Argyris, 1984).

Argyris eventually concluded that participants‘ default assumptions and values about
social behavior were too strong to yield even to well-designed and well-led T-Group training.
Specifically, he came to believe that behavior in groups is governed by the overriding goals of
controlling the task, maximizing winning and minimizing losing, and avoiding embarrassment
and threat. Rather than share valid data, Argyris concluded, people keep their true thoughts and
feelings to themselves. Rather than test their assumptions and attributions, people discourage
honest feedback from others. Disagreement is covered over, and a façade of pleasantness and
politeness obscures what is really going on. Moreover, the cover-over is itself covered-over. As
a result, no one learns anything that is personally meaningful or useful in the wider world.

The only escape from this dead end, Argyris argued, was action research that departs
radically from conventional social science. Whereas normal science strives to be objective and
value-neutral, he concluded, action research must be normative and prescriptive (Argyris,
Putnam, & Smith, 1985). The ultimate goal of action research, then, would be to develop a
group‘s capacity for what Argyris calls double loop learning—that is, the capability not just to
regulate behavior to achieve one‘s goals but to explore the validity of the goals themselves.

In recent years, scholars continue to explore how best to achieve ambitious objectives
such as those set forth by Lewin and Argyris, even as they draw on the methods and values of
action research to pursue other scholarly objectives—from ways to use very large groups to
create systemic change in organizations or communities in real time (Bunker & Alban, 2006) to
the development of group methods for carrying out planned organizational change programs
(Hackman & Edmondson, 2008).

Perhaps the greatest overall strength of the action approach is the value it places on
collaboration between those who conduct the research and those who participate in it.
Researcher-participant collaboration helps keep action research focused on issues that have
practical as well as scholarly significance. The risk, however, is that findings from such studies
will be relegated to organization development journals rather than mainline academic journals—
in which case the action research paradigm could become even more intellectually marginalized
than it is now, lose its academic credibility, and fail to have the level of impact to which its
founders aspired.

The Process-Focused Approach

―In the early days of ‗group dynamics‘, when therapists such as Wilfred Bion
reported addressing their groups with interpretations that lumped all participants
together, by saying ‗the group is‘ (doing or feeling so and so, such as ‗pairing‘ or
‗in flight‘), the hair on the back of my neck always bristled in indignation as I read
Bion‘s interpretation. In…groups I was always attuned to individual differences
- 24 -

and interaction between different individuals and different value positions. I


always made a point of looking for dissidents whenever a majority seemed to be
forming. And I nearly always found them, even if there was only one of them,
back in a corner or looking longingly toward the door…‖

Thus wrote R. Freed Bales (1999, p. 115), whose system for analyzing group interaction
launched the process-focused approach to understanding group behavior and performance. In his
early work, Bales showed little interest in, or sympathy for, psychodynamic interpretations, or
group members‘ self-reports about who they did and did not like, or action research that sought
to alter how members thought, felt, or behaved. Instead, he set out to chart—concretely, reliably,
and in detail—what actually transpires in group interaction (Bales, 1950). It is that insistent
focus on group interaction that is the essence of the process-focused approach to the study of
groups.

Bales‘ particular methodology for assessing group interaction, Interaction Process


Analysis (IPA) is a detailed coding system that breaks group interaction into its smallest
constituent units, which he called communication acts. For each act, trained observers record
who performed the act, to whom it was directed, when it occurred, and the function it serves.
With those data are in hand, researchers can then document how group members gradually
develop distinctive roles, how alliances crystallize into subgroups, how the character of
interaction changes over time, and more. Although labor intensive, IPA was so informative
about group interaction that it became the standard method for coding group interaction
throughout the last half of the 20th century (Forsyth & Burnette, 2005).

The IPA framework grew out of the work of Bales‘ mentor, sociologist Talcott Parsons.
Parsons was perhaps best known for his theory of functionalism, which explains the evolution of
societal structures such as economic, legal, and educational institutions in terms of the human
needs they fulfill. Bales extended Parsons‘ theory of functionalism from the level of society to
the level of the small group. Specifically, Bales posited that there are certain fundamental issues
that every group must resolve. Some of those issues are in the task domain, and some are in the
socioemotional domain. In the task domain are the challenges of orientation (developing a
shared understanding of the task), evaluation (identifying which ideas to accept and which to
reject), and control (keeping the group moving forward). In the socioemotional domain are
challenges of decision (how members show agreement and disagreement), tension reduction (for
example, by using humor) and reintegration (showing solidarity and support).

Bales found that the task and socioemotional needs of a group are in conflict. Because
making progress on the task necessarily produces relational strain, a group needs to establish
equilibrium through a cyclic pattern of interaction, with forward movement on the task followed
by socioemotional communication that restores interpersonal harmony. He also documented the
problems that arise when acts in one category are not balanced by acts in a complementary
category. For example, conflict is likely to emerge when ―giving opinions‖ is not balanced by
―asking for opinions.‖

The list of substantive findings obtained using interaction process analysis is long. For
example, two leaders typically emerge in a group, one who focuses on the task domain and the
- 25 -

other on the socioemotional domain. The larger the group, the more likely one person will
dominate. Most acts in small groups are task-oriented rather than socioemotional. In order to
successfully accomplish its task, a group needs more positive than negative communication acts.
Newly formed groups progress through a predictable sequence of phases in problem solving:
evaluation, orientation, and control (Bales & Strodtbeck, 1951; Seeger, 1983).

As informative as interaction process analysis has been about what actually happens in
groups, it generated relatively few findings about what might be done to improve group
functioning. Eventually, Bales developed a new model, based on the over five decades of work
using IPA, that did have a more normative character. This model, known as SYMLOG
(―systematic multiple level observation of groups‖) draws on multiple sources of data to generate
a plot that situates each group member in three dimensional space: dominant vs. submissive,
friendly vs. unfriendly, and instrumentally controlled vs. emotionally expressive (Bales &
Cohen, 1979). That plot, which also depicts subgroups and their location in the three-
dimensional space, provides an empirical basis for interventions intended to improve group
interaction and performance.

In recent years, researchers in the process-focused tradition have been giving increasing
attention to the ways patterns of interaction eventually generate stable group structures—a
process known as structuration (Poole, Seibold, & McPhee, 1985). Structuration explains why
groups that have an identical objective situation often carry out their work quite differently
(Orlikowski, 2000; Perlow, Gittell, & Katz, 2004). To illustrate, consider the structure of a
group's task. Members' work on that task is shaped not only by the objective task structure but
also by their own preferences and values about working together. As Wageman and Gordon
(2005) have shown, when members share egalitarian values, they are more likely to approach a
task as a highly interdependent endeavor, but when they share meritocratic values, they are more
likely to approach the same task as a low interdependence activity. Over time, these emergent
behavioral patterns become normative and solidify into an aspect of the group's structure.

Overall, the major strength of the processed-focused approach is its capacity to capture
nuance and meaning, and to produce a rich and data-based understanding of how group members
interact. Its major limitation is that generating process-focused descriptions of group interaction
is an extremely labor-intensive undertaking. As hardware and software are developed to
automate the coding of interaction processes, it is at least possible that the field of small group
research will see a resurgence of interest in Bales-type process analyses.

The Decision-Analytic Approach


The decision-analytic approach to studying groups has two distinct research streams. The
first stream has its origins in qualitative, case-based methods. The second stream is highly
quantitative and largely laboratory-based. Despite these differences in methods, the streams
share two key assumptions. One is that the purpose of using a group rather than individuals for
decision making is primarily to enhance decision quality. The second is that variations in the
quality of group decisions generally can be attributed to the quality of group interaction
(Wittenbaum, Hollingshead, Paulus, Hirokawa, Ancona, Peterson, Jehn, & Yoon, 2004).
- 26 -

The qualitative stream of research originated with the work of Irving Janis with the
publication in 1972 of his book, Victims of Groupthink. Though Janis was trained as a social
psychologist, his method for studying groupthink, comparing case studies drawn from American
history, was atypical of most of the social psychological research being conducted at that time. In
the introduction to the second edition of the book (Janis, 1982), he explains how he found
himself in that territory:

―The main theme of this book occurred to me while reading [historian] Arthur M.
Schlesinger‘s chapters on the Bay of Pigs in A Thousand Days. At first, I was
puzzled: How could bright, shrewd men like John F. Kennedy and his advisers be
taken in by the CIA‘s stupid, patchwork plan? I began to wonder whether some
kind of psychological contagion, similar to social conformity phenomena observed
in studies of small groups, had interfered with their mental alertness. I kept
thinking about the implications of this notion until one day I found myself talking
about it, in a seminar of mine on group psychology at Yale University... (p. vii)

Janis presented seven detailed case studies in his book (Janis, 1982), ranging from the
decision in 1941 by Admiral Kimmel and his advisors to focus on training rather than on the
defense of Pearl Harbor despite signs of an impending attack by Japan, to the well-known
decision in 1960 by President Kennedy and his advisors to invade Cuba at the Bay of Pigs.
Especially well known is the contrast between the Kennedy group‘s Bay of Pigs fiasco and
essentially the same group‘s successful management of the Cuban missile crisis two years later.

Janis constructed the case studies mainly from secondary sources: minutes of group
meetings, diaries, memoirs, letters, prepared statements given to investigating committees, and
published documents. These data provided the basis for his model of groupthink, which he
specifically defined as ―a mode of thinking that people engage in when they are deeply involved
in a cohesive in-group, when the members‘ strivings for unanimity override their motivation to
realistically appraise alternative courses of action‖ (1982, p. 9).

Janis took pains to make clear that he believed in the potential of groups as effective
decision making units. ―I do not mean to imply that…group decisions are typically inefficient or
harmful. On the contrary, a group whose members have properly defined roles, with traditions
and standard operating procedures that facilitate critical inquiry, is probably capable of making
better decisions than any individual in the group who works on the problem alone. And yet the
advantages of having decisions made by groups are often lost because of psychological pressures
that arise when the members work closely together, share the same values, and above all face a
crisis situation in which everyone is subjected to stresses that generate a strong need for
affiliation‖ (1982, p. 12).

The primary strengths of the groupthink model are its clarity, richness, and
persuasiveness. Moreover, as Baron (2005) notes, the model offers a broad array of testable
hypotheses since Janis carefully delineated the antecedents of groupthink, the symptoms
exhibited by a team that is in its throes, and the consequences that follow. For these reasons, a
great deal of empirical work has been carried out to test the model (e.g. Esser & Ahlfinger, 2001;
Neck & Moorhead, 1992; Tetlock, Peterson, McGuire, Chang, & Feld, 1992) as well as to assess
- 27 -

alternative models of groupthink-like phenomena (e.g. Baron, 2005; Kramer, 1998; McCauley,
1998; Raven, 1998; Turner & Pratkanis, 1998; Whyte, 1998). Baron (2005) offers this summary
appraisal: ―A review of the research and debate about Janis‘s model leads to the conclusion that
after some 30 years of investigation, the evidence has largely failed to support the formulation‘s
more ambitious and controversial predictions. . . ‖ (p. 219).

Despite the decidedly mixed evidence about the validity of the model, it survives and, for
the most part, prospers. Textbooks continue discuss the groupthink model in detail, and it
commonly is invoked to explain contemporary group decision-making fiascoes, ranging from the
launch of the space shuttle Challenger to the Reagan administration‘s Iran-Contra scandal
(Kramer, 1998, p. 238). What accounts for the resilience of groupthink? Clearly, the quality of
Janis‘s writing and his skilled use of case studies play a role—in part because the cases invoke
the availability heuristic (Aldag & Fuller, 1993). But perhaps most significant is the fact that his
hypothesis is counterintuitive for most readers—that is, something people usually think of as
good, group cohesiveness, can actually be quite dangerous. Together, these factors have
rendered the groupthink hypothesis relatively immune to empirical correction, as badly needed
as such corrections have turned out to be.

The second stream of research in the decision-analytic tradition is largely laboratory-


based. In the 1960‘s and 1970‘s, this stream analyzed the ways group members combine their
initial preferences to achieve a consensus position—for example, in jury decision-making. More
recently, this stream has emphasized the combination of members‘ information or expertise
rather than their simple preferences (Kerr & Tindale, 2004).

Research on social combination models (e.g. Davis 1969, 1973; Kerr, 1981; Laughlin,
1980; Penrod & Hastie, 1981; Shiflett, 1979; Stasser & Davis, 1981) seeks to predict how group
members will combine their diverse individual preferences into a single group response. The
basic research paradigm requires a group to reach consensus regarding two or more pre-specified
alternatives (e.g. guilty versus not guilty). The inputs to the group decision are each group
member‘s preference prior to group discussion, with group process simulated through alternative
social combination models. Each way of resolving disagreement is represented probabilistically
by a different decision rule for reaching consensus, such as voting, turn taking, demonstration,
random selection, and the generation of a new alternative. According to Laughlin and
Hollingshead (1995), voting can be represented as a majority wins model. Turn taking can be
represented as a proportionality process. Demonstration can be represented as a truth wins or
truth-supported wins model. Random assignment can be represented by a model that assigns
equal probability to any alternative advocated by at least one group member. Researchers
compare the actual proportion of groups selecting each decision rule to the proportion expected
based on the initial distribution of individuals‘ preferences.

The type of model that best explains the data turns out to depend heavily on the type of
group task being performed. For judgment tasks that have no demonstrably correct answer,
majority models best predict the probability of the group choice, for example. For intellective
tasks for which a demonstrably correct response exists, truth wins or truth-supported wins
models best predict the probability that a group will select the correct answer. But there is a
twist: unless at least one member advocates an alternative prior to the group discussion, that
- 28 -

alternative is rarely even considered by the group and has almost no chance of being selected
(Hollingshead, 1996; Laughlin, 1999).

Another line of research in the decision-analytic tradition focuses on how group members
share information as they come to a decision or generate the solution to a problem. One benefit
of group decision making is that the pooling of information held by different members can result
in a more informed decision than otherwise would be the case. But, as demonstrated in a classic
paper by Stasser and Titus (1985), groups actually tend to discuss only information that is shared
by all members, rather than pieces of information that are uniquely held by single individuals.
This phenomenon can compromise the group product both when the information most needed is
held by a single individual and when many individuals have separate pieces of information that
must be surfaced and integrated for the group to come up with the optimal course of action. As
will be discussed later in this chapter, numerous scholars have explored the conditions that
increase the likelihood that a group actually will elicit and use appropriately information that is
uniquely held by individual members.

The quantitative, laboratory-based stream of research on group decision making is


distinguished both by its formal elegance and its predictive power, especially for well-defined
tasks such as jury decision-making. Relative to qualitative research on group decision-making,
however, this line of research does not capture the substantive richness of what goes on in groups
as members wrestle with difficult decisions—and, therefore, it can come across as relatively dry.
Current developments in neuroscience offer new possibilities for exploring some intriguing
parallels between groups and brains (Hinsz, Tindale, & Vollrath, 1997), as well as for the
conduct of cross-level research on the ways that individual neural processes come to be
assembled, through group interaction, into collective decisions.

The Complex Systems Approach


More than five decades ago, Kurt Lewin conceptualized groups as holistic and dynamic
systems, but at that time the tools needed to conceptually and empirically explore that view were
not available. By the mid-1990s complexity theory seemed to provide both a language and a
methodology for bringing that approach to bear on group behavior (Wheelan, 1996).

Joesph McGrath and his colleagues at the University of Illinois took on the challenge of
doing so. They had grown increasingly dissatisfied with established methods for studying and
conceptualizing group process, for several reasons (McGrath, 1997; McGrath, Arrow, &
Berdahl, 2000). First, groups typically were treated as simple systems, composed of chain-like,
unidirectional cause-effect relationships. Second, the context in which groups were embedded
often was ignored. Third, groups were studied as static entities, without a past or future. And
fourth, groups were treated as generic entities, composed of generic people. The Illinois group
sought an alternative approach, one that would be less mechanistic and more true to their own
experience of group process as a sometimes baffling but always rich mix of routine and surprise,
predictability and randomness, the obvious and the obscure. They turned to complex systems
theory for that alternative approach.

Complex systems theory is an increasingly popular perspective emerging from work in


biology, physics, mathematics, and computer science (Mathews, White, & Long, 1999). As
- 29 -

McKelvey (1999) asserts, ―The study of complex adaptive systems has become the ultimate in
interdisciplinary science, focusing its modeling activities on how microstate events, whether
particles, molecules, genes, neurons, human agents, or firms, self-organize into emergent
aggregate structures.‖ The basic premise of complexity theory, according to McClure (1998), is
as follows: Even though the behaviors of natural systems are not predictable, there are patterns to
their randomness or irregularity. Those patterns emerge over time. The aspiration is to track the
patterns, both linear and non-linear; to identify the contingencies that shape them; and to predict
the exceptions or deviations from them.

Drawing on these few ideas, McGrath, Arrow, and Berdahl (2000) articulated a new
approach to thinking about groups:

―We see groups as complex, adaptive, dynamic systems. Rather than simple,
groups are complex entities embedded in a hierarchy of levels and characterized by
multiple, bidirectional, and nonlinear causal relations. Rather than isolated, groups
are intricately embedded within, and have continual mutual adaptation with, a
number of embedding contexts. Rather than static, groups are inherently dynamic
systems, operating via processes that unfold over time, with those processes
dependent both on the group‘s past history and on its anticipated future‖ (p. 98).

A group is best understood, then, as a complex system made up of individuals who are
themselves complex systems, each guided by goals and perceptions that change over time. Each
individual belongs to multiple groups at the same time, and each of these groups is embedded in
the physical, temporal, sociocultural, and organizational context.

The distinctions among local, global, and contextual dynamics is central to the complex
systems approach to understanding group behavior. Local dynamics involve the activity of
group members and their links to tools and resources to accomplish tasks. Local elements
interact with one another in a recursive, nonlinear way, and they cannot be meaningfully
decomposed into standard independent and dependent variables (McGrath, Arrow, & Berdahl,
2000, p. 99). The pattern of interaction among local variables generates global variables—
emergent aspects of the system, not mere aggregates of local variables—such as norms,
cooperation, conflict, and leadership. Moreover, the higher level order that gradually emerges
from the chaotic activity of lower level components cannot be fully specified from a detailed
understanding of isolated system components. Thus, as Arrow (2005) notes, this approach
requires ―an appreciation for the paradox of coherent patterns arising out of group behavior that
remains unpredictable in its particulars‖ (p. 202).

Contextual dynamics refer to the impact of system-level parameters whose values are
determined in part by the group‘s embedding context, such as the level of organizational support,
the supply of potential members, and the demand for group outputs. The interplay between
micro and macro system levels operates in both directions: global variables emerge from local
actions but then guide and constrain those same actions. Conventional methods of studying
groups are not of much help in sorting this all out. When the elements of a system are strongly
and complexly interconnected, for example, it is not useful to decompose that system into its
basic elements and then attempt to vary them one at a time to assess their independent effects. In
- 30 -

complex systems, the links are as important as the elements, and there are more of them. The
focus of research, then, should be on identifying the rules that guide the interactions among
variables rather than trying to predict the specific future values of particular variables (Arrow,
McGrath, & Berdahl, 2000, p. 46).

A number of researchers have attempted to carry out empirical analyses of small groups
as complex systems, including Arrow (1997), Arrow and Burns (2004), Arrow and Crosson
(2003), Fuhriman and Burlingame (1994), Guastello (2007), Guastello and Guastello (1998), and
Wheelan (1996), sometimes using computational modeling to analyze the dynamics of such
systems (Berdahl, 1998). Yet research on group behavior using a complex systems approach has
diminished somewhat in recent years, perhaps because the approach poses difficult conceptual
challenges and requires use of methodologies that are unfamiliar to most small group
researchers. But it also may be that the promise of the approach initially was oversold to some
extent. As Cohen (1999) has pointed out:

―… we need to begin sharpening our appraisal of the promise and limitations of


complex systems theories…. To have real value, such new ideas cannot for very
long be characterized as the potential answer to almost every question. A period of
testing their applicability across a spectrum of issues is needed. This will help us to
determine on which problems the ideas work best, and which are best attacked with
other tools.‖

The period of testing is not yet over. But findings thus far suggest that the main
contribution of the complex systems approach may lie in its potential for generating
sophisticated answers to the question: What are the factors and conditions that influence group
change and development over time? Other questions about small groups, perhaps, will continue
to be most appropriately addressed using other tools. In Arrow‘s (2005, p. 202) words, ―Work in
this perspective is in an early stage of development. We are, in the terminology of the field,
working far from equilibrium at the edge of chaos, a state in which creativity and disorganization
are both readily available…‖

PART THREE:
CURRENT FRONTIERS OF GROUP RESEARCH

The current flowering of research on purposive groups is generating a number of shoots


that extend off into some new empirical and conceptual directions—some of which, as will be
seen, have their roots in the intellectual history of the approaches just reviewed. This section
explores several specific issues for which contemporary group research appears to be especially
productive, promising, or problematic.

Assembly of Member Attributes

In an early review of research on group performance, Collins and Guetzkow (1964, chap.
3) coined the term ―assembly effect‖ to refer to cases in which a group achieves collectively
- 31 -

something beyond what could have been achieved by any of its members or by a simple
combination of what members bring to the task. An example would be a group decision that is
neither the average of individuals‘ preferences nor the adoption of the prior position of certain
members. Although Collins and Guetzkow focused mainly on group performance, the idea of
the assembly effect can be applied to any circumstance in which the attributes of individuals
come together to create a genuinely collective reality. To illustrate, researchers have studied the
assembly of members‘ emotions (Barsade, 2002), motivation (Chen & Kanfer, 2006), self-
efficacy (Tasa, Taggar, & Seijtz, 2007), cognitions and concepts (Laughlin, 1999), skills and
capabilities (Kozlowsiki, Gully, Nason, & Smith (1999), social values (Wageman & Gordon,
2005), and personal dispositions and styles (Moynihan & Peterson, 2001).

Group polarization—that is, the tendency to settle on a riskier or more conservative


position after group discussion—often is described as if it were an assembly effect (Brown,
1986, Isenberg, 1986). But the great majority of studies of polarization processes actually assess
only changes in the level of risk that individuals are willing to accept after having discussed their
choice with others. Research on group polarization has been more about the impact of group
discussion on privately-expressed individual preferences than about a genuinely group-level
shift.

The attempt by numerous group researchers to generate and study the group-level
manifestations of individual-level states, attributes, and processes raises two questions. One,
how do assembly processes unfold and what do they generate? And two, which aspects of
individuals can be assembled into group-level constructs, and which cannot or should not be?

A number of scholars have addressed these questions. Moreland, Levine and Wingert
(1996) provide a general model of group composition effects, with special attention to how
―chemistry‖ develops in a group. Felps, Mitchell, and Byington (2006) present a model of how
the ripples set in motion by troublesome individual members (i.e., ―a bad apple‖) can alter the
dynamic of the group as a whole. Kozlowski and his colleagues (Kozlowski, Gully, Nason, &
Smith, 1999; Kozlowski, Watola, Nowakowski, Kim, & Botero, 2009) describe the way in which
performance processes compile over time and the role of team leaders in facilitating that process.
Goldstone, Roberts, and Guerckis (2008) draw a parallel between how neurons interconnect in
the brain and how members‘ interactions result in emergent group processes such as bandwagon
effects and population waves. Some researchers have gone even further, adopting the concept of
a ―group brain‖ quite literally, asserting that cognition happens not just at the individual level but
also at the level of the group as a whole (Larson & Christensen, 1993).

Contemporary Models of Collective Cognition

Collective-level cognitive constructs have achieved considerable currency in


contemporary group research and theory (Forbes & Miliken, 1999; Hinsz, Tindale, & Vollrath,
1997). The most prominent of these is the concept of transactive memory, developed by Daniel
Wegner (Wegner, 1986, 1995; for a review of research on the construct, see Peltokorpi, 2008).
Wegner posited that groups, like individuals, have memory systems that collect, encode, store,
and retrieve information. Over time, different members of a group come to keep track of
- 32 -

different matters, which results in the group as a whole having access to more information than
any single individual—transactive memory.

Group members who spend time working together also can develop a shared mental
model, a related concept that also brings what is known about individual cognition to the group
level of analysis (Cannon-Bowers, Salas, & Converse, 1993; Klimoski & Mohammed, 1994;
Mathieu, Heffner, Goodwin, Salas, & Cannon-Bowers, 2000). A mental model is a cognitive
representation of how some system operates. When group members share the same mental
model, coordination is enhanced and the likelihood of miscues is lessened (Ensley & Pearce,
2001; Waller, Gupta, & Giambatista, 2004).

A third collective construct is collective mind which, as set forth by Weick and Roberts
(1993), focuses more on cognitive activities than on the mind as an entity: ―Our focus is at once
on individuals and the collective, since only individuals can contribute to a collective mind, but a
collective mind is distinct from an individual mind because it inheres in the pattern of
interrelated activities among many people‖ (p. 360). Collective mind is explicitly social in
character because it derives from what Weick and Roberts call ―heedful interrelating‖ among
members. The more attentively members interact, the more robust their collective mind will be.

Interpretive Cautions

The developments discussed above are not without interpretive risks, and the present day
is not the first time they have been encountered. Early in the 20th century, William McDougall
published The Group Mind, which laid out the principles of what he called ―collective
psychology.‖ The book prompted a blistering response from Floyd Allport (McDougall, 1920;
Allport, 1924). Allport flatly rejected any notion of social cognition, emotion, or behavior, and
asserted that McDougall had fallen victim to the group fallacy: ―…the individual in the crowd
behaves just as he would have alone, only more so…. There is no psychology of groups that is
not essentially and entirely a psychology of individuals‖ (p. 4).

Although both McDougall and Allport may have stated their cases more vigorously than
absolutely necessary, the study of assembly effects is in fact a risky and challenging undertaking.
There is of course no problem with group-level properties that exist only at the collective level—
for example, compositional features such as group size or the demographic diversity of members,
and structural features such as group norms (whose conceptualization, following Jackson, 1966,
centrally involves the variance among members, and variance is meaningful only at the
collective level). Group properties that are obtained by aggregating the attributes or behaviors of
individual members, however, require interpretive caution. To illustrate, the aggregated property
of ―group height,‖ obtained by averaging the heights of all members, would seem to make no
sense at the group level since only individuals can be tall or short. But in the context of
basketball the concept becomes meaningful—some teams are indeed "taller" than others.

Aggregated properties often are established when a group is formed (e.g., the overall
level of member skills), but they also emerge as a product of members' interactions (e.g., in the
enhancement of collective talent as members learn from one another, or in the development of a
collective point of view about some matter). In either case, group scholars are obligated to
- 33 -

establish that the aggregated property has both conceptual meaning and empirical integrity at the
group level of analysis (Hackman, 2003; Walsh, 1995).

Concepts that describe group processes are more theoretically challenging than those that
describe group properties. Descriptors of group decision-making, task-performance activities, or
learning processes pose no special problems because those processes generate outcomes that can
be unambiguously attributed to the group as a collective—that is, a decision, a product, or
alterations in how members work together. Difficulties arise, however, when collective
processes are described using concepts whose actual referents are the biological, cognitive, or
affective functioning of individual persons. It is hard to know exactly what is meant when a
group is described as perceiving, thinking, or feeling. Because such invoked processes have no
real collective referents, they may be more appropriately viewed as metaphors than as actual
group-level realities.

Use of Member Information and Expertise

It is well established, both in research and in practice, that groups rarely draw on the full
complement of members‘ information, knowledge, and expertise in pursuing collective purposes
(Argote, Ingram, Levine, & Moreland, 2000; Dahlin, Weingart, & Hinds, 2005; Thomas-Hunt,
Ogden, & Neale, 2003), and that group performance suffers as a consequence (Faraj & Sproull,
2000; Gruenfeld, Mannix, Williams, & Neale, 1996). A great deal of research has been
conducted over the last decade to document the reasons why groups under-exploit the
informational resources and expertise of their members, and to identify interventions that might
improve that state of affairs.

Vulnerabilities

Decision-making groups generally focus collective attention on information that is shared


among all members, rather than on information that is uniquely held by different individuals.
Studies of this phenomenon typically employ a hidden profile task, in which information that
would identify the best alternative is distributed among members and therefore is not initially
known to everyone in the group (Stasser & Titus, 1985, 2003). Groups performing hidden
profile tasks (for example, making a decision about who to hire or where to locate a business)
rely so heavily on shared information that they rarely come up with the best answer unless
individual members are somehow prompted to share with the group the information that they
uniquely hold (Schulz-Hardt, Brodbeck, Mojzisch, Kerschreiter, & Frey, 2006; van Ginkel &
van Knippenberg, 2008).

Not only do groups have difficulty finding ways to get all the relevant information onto
the collective table, they also tend to weight the knowledge and expertise of their members sub-
optimally—for example, by misapprehending who actually knows what is needed for the group
to make the right decision or turn in a good performance (Littlepage, Robison, & Reddington,
1997). The credence given to a member‘s contributions often depends much more on that
person‘s demographic attributes (e.g., gender, age, or ethnicity), position (e.g., rank, role, or
- 34 -

office), or behavioral style (e.g., talkativeness or verbal dominance) than on the person‘s actual
expertise (Caruso & Woolley, 2008; Hackman & Morris, 1975).

Both over-reliance on shared information and flawed weighting processes are generic
problems for groups whose purposes require use of member knowledge and expertise. Some
groups are considerably more vulnerable to these difficulties than others, however. The new
group forms that are now emerging—larger groups with a greater diversity of membership,
groups whose composition shifts continuously over time, and groups whose geographically
dispersed members rely mainly on electronic technologies for communication—are likely to find
managing member information and expertise to be especially challenging. It has been well
established, for example, that the larger the group, the greater the chances that worthy individual
ideas and insights will be overlooked (Steiner, 1972, chap. 4). The greater the diversity of group
membership, the more likely that inter-group stereotypes and conflicts will compromise the full
utilization of member resources (Caruso & Woolley, 2008). The more frequently group
composition changes, the harder it is for members to keep track of which members have what
task-relevant information or expertise (Gruenfeld, Martorana, & Fan, 2000; Lewis, Belliveau,
Herndon, & Keller, 2007). And the more that a dispersed group relies on electronic technologies
for communication, the more challenging it will be for members to get a good ―take‖ on who
knows what (Nemiro, M. M. Beyerlein, Bradley, & S. Beyerlein, 2008).

Overcoming the Vulnerabilities

To use member information and expertise fully and well requires overcoming three
hurdles, each of which has been the subject of considerable research. One, the group must
recognize that certain members do have special information or expertise relevant to the group‘s
work (Bauman & Bonner, 2004; Bunderson, 2003; Littlepage, Robison, & Reddington, 1997).
Two, the group must value what those individuals have to contribute (Caruso & Woolley, 2008;
Scholten, van Knippenberg, Nijstad, & De Dreu, 2007). And three, the group must be
sufficiently motivated and coordinated to actually use members‘ knowledge resources (Faraj &
Sproull, 2000; Quigley, Tesluk, Locke, & Bartol, 2007).

A number of conceptual models and empirical studies have sought to identify both the
conditions and the leadership interventions that can increase the likelihood that a group actually
will recognize, value, and use the information and expertise of its members. Among the
structural and contextual factors that have been shown to facilitate use of the full complement of
member resources on knowledge tasks are clear and challenging group goals coupled with
incentives for achieving them (Quigley, Tesluk, Locke, & Bartol, 2007), a division of cognitive
labor that publicly identifies members‘ areas of special expertise (Stasser, Stewart, &
Wittenbaum, 1995), collective accountability for the process by which the team generates its
output (Scholten, van Knippenberg, Nijstad, & De Dreu, 2007), an organizational culture that
emphasizes shared collective interests rather than individual distinctiveness (Chatman, Polzer,
Barsade, & Neale, 1998), and group norms that place a higher value on collaboration and critical
thinking than on achieving consensus (Okhuysen & Eisenhardt, 2002; Postmes, Spears, &
Cihangir, 2001).
- 35 -

Among the leader interventions that have been found to be helpful are those that elicit
and legitimize dissenting views (Schulz-Hardt, Brodbeck, Mojzisch, Kerschreiter, & Frey,
2006); those that ask members to reflect on the group and its work, thereby prompting task
representations that encourage them to draw upon one another‘s knowledge (van Ginkel & van
Knippenberg, 2009); and those that invite members to explicitly plan the performance strategy
the group will use in carrying out its work (Woolley, Gerbasi, Chabris, Kosslyn, & Hackman,
2008). Indeed, this last study suggests that merely having the right group composition (that is,
having in the group people who have the expertise required for task success) can even impair
group performance unless the group also is helped to use that expertise well. Successful
performance of the intelligence analysis task used in the Woolley et al. study required use of
certain task-relevant cognitive capabilities. Teams either did or did not include members who
had scored at the highest levels on a previously administered test of those capabilities, and either
did or did not receive an intervention that fostered collaborative planning about the use of those
capabilities. Performance was enhanced only when groups were composed of members who had
task-appropriate capabilities and when groups received the collaborative planning intervention.
The presence of high expertise in the absence of the social intervention actually decreased team
performance relative to teams that neither had the needed expertise nor received the intervention.

Research on the use of member information and expertise is one of the most vigorous and
productive areas in the field of small group research. Additional evidence about the conditions
and interventions that promote better utilization of the members‘ information and capabilities
should yield not just advances in small group theory but also empirical findings that can
constructively guide the practice of using groups to make decisions and perform work.

Group Learning

Groups are situated at the nexus of individuals and organizations. For that reason, they
have considerable potential for fostering not just their own capabilities but also those of their
members and of the organization as a whole (Edmondson, 2002; Senge, 1990).

The importance of group learning is widely recognized by scholars, and considerable


research has been conducted on the topic (Argote, Gruenfeld, & Naquin, 2001; Kozlowski &
Bell, 2008; Sessa & London, 2008). Yet there remains considerable disagreement about exactly
what group learning is. Ellis and his colleagues, for example, define it as a change in a group‘s
level of knowledge and skill, produced by shared experiences (Ellis, Hollenbeck, Ilgen, Porter, &
West, 2003). Zellmer-Bruhn and Gibson (2006) view learning as the creation of new collective
routines. Gibson and Vermeulen (2003) define it as ―a cycle of experimentation, reflective
communication, and knowledge codification‖ (p. 206). Wilson, Goodman, and Cronin (2007)
say that group learning is ―a change in the group‘s repertoire of potential behavior…whether or
not it is manifested in externally observable behavior‖ (p. 1044).

Despite these definitional disagreements, scholars generally concur with Wilson,


Goodman, and Cronin (2007) that group learning involves three sequential processes: sharing,
storage, and retrieval. According to these authors, the first process, sharing, begins when an
individual member gains some new knowledge, learns a new routine, or invents a new behavior.
Over time, as members become aware of what their fellow members know or know how to do,
- 36 -

collective understanding of the group‘s overall store of knowledge and capabilities emerges.
Eventually, what has been learned by various members becomes a property of the group as a
whole—an emergent repertoire that exceeds what is known by any one of them.

Researchers have made considerable progress in identifying the conditions that increase
the likelihood that learning-oriented sharing will occur in a group. A key condition for sharing is
the level of required interdependence built into the team task, since that gives members both
occasion and incentive to learn from one another (Wageman, 1995). Beyond the task itself, a
broader array of structures, including specialization, formalization, and hierarchy, also can
promote sharing and learning, especially in self-managing teams that may be more in need of
structural supports than are manager-led teams (Bunderson & Boumgarden, in press).

Additional factors that have been shown to foster learning-oriented sharing include a
climate of psychological safety (Edmondson, 1996, 1999b; Van den Bossche, Gijselaers, Segers,
& Kirschner, 2006), a collective sense of group potency or efficacy (Van den Bossche et al.,
2006), and strong identification with the group (Van der Vegt & Bunderson, 2005), all of which
can be fostered by learning-oriented team leadership. In addition, research has identified a
number of specific behaviors by group members that facilitate sharing and that are more likely to
be exhibited when the conditions identified above are in place. These include speaking up about
errors or lapses (Edmondson, 1996; 2003), active listening and communicating (Van den
Bossche et al., 2006), and overt reflection on the group‘s objectives, strategies, and processes
(Schippers, den Hartog, & Koopman, 2008).

Research on storage, the second of the three processes posited by Wilson, Goodman, and
Cronin (2007), involves use of the group‘s transactive memory—that is, members‘ shared
awareness of who in a group knows what. Transactive memory, discussed earlier in this chapter,
has been shown to facilitate group learning as well as group performance (Austin, 2003; Faraj &
Sproull, 2000; Lewis, 2004). Other kinds of repositories also can be useful in storing what is
being collectively learned, including shared databases, bulletin boards, and expert systems, as
well as standard roles, rules, procedures developed by the group itself (Argote, 1999). Research
is now exploring the differential appropriateness of various kinds of storage systems for different
types of group knowledge (Goodman & Darr, 1996).

The third process involved in group learning is the retrieval of what has been learned. As
Wilson, Goodman, and Cronin (2007) note, ―It is not unusual for members of a group to think
that they have stored new learning, only to discover that the group does not access it when the
next opportunity to apply the learning presents itself‖ (p. 1050). Although some research has
been done on the retrieval process (e.g., Cohen & Bacdayan, 1994; Hollingshead, 1998), much
remains unknown about what is required for retrieval to occur smoothly and efficiently.

Group learning is a quite active field of research, but a good number of studies address its
core concept only inferentially. That is, some manipulation or intervention is found to improve
group performance—and then the inference is made that group learning must have occurred to
have generated that improvement. Additional attention to measuring learning processes
themselves would be helpful in deepening knowledge about group learning, especially since
learning and group performance are not isomorphic (Druskat & Kayes, 2000). That is,
- 37 -

performance can improve in the absence of learning (perhaps merely because of increased effort
for some kinds of tasks), and learning does not necessarily generate performance improvements
(and, in some circumstances, can even result in performance decrements, as was found by
Bunderson and Sutcliffe, 2003, and Wong, 2004). It would be good to know more about the
circumstances under which group learning does and does not contribute positively to group
performance as well as to the personal learning of individual members and to the collective
capabilities of the broader organization in which the group operates.

Group Stability

Conventional wisdom about group stability is pessimistic about the viability and
performance of groups whose members stay together for a long time. Although teams may
become better at working together in the early phases of their lives, the argument goes, the
improvements soon plateau and then, at some point, members become too comfortable with one
another, too lax in enforcing standards of behavior, and too willing to forgive teammates‘ lapses.
It is better, therefore, to have a continuous flow-through of members to keep teams fresh and
sharp.

Conventional wisdom is wrong. Research findings, from both laboratory and field
studies, overwhelmingly support the proposition that teams with stable membership have
healthier dynamics and perform better than those that constantly have to deal with the arrival of
new members and the departure of veterans. An analysis of National Transportation Safety
Board (NTSB) records, for example, revealed that 73 percent of the incidents in the NTSB
database occurred on a crew‘s first day of flying together, and 44 percent of those took place on
a crew‘s very first flight (National Transportation Safety Board, 1994). That there is a liability
of newness in flight crew operations was confirmed in an experimental simulation by Foushee,
Lauber, Baetge, and Acomb (1986), in which fatigued crews who had flown together for several
days caught and corrected more errors than did well-rested crews who were just starting their
work together. Similar findings have been obtained for teams as varied as coal miners
(Goodman & Shah, 1992) and construction crews (Hapgood, 1994), and these field-study
findings documenting the benefits of team stability and longevity are affirmed by a number of
laboratory experiments (Argote, Insko, Yovetich, & Romero, 1995; Gruenfeld & Hollingshead,
1993; Lewis, Bellieveau, Herndon, & Keller, 2007; Watson, Michaelsen, & Sharp, 1991).

There are, however, two exceptions to the main body of evidence summarized above,
both of which have to do with the risk that a group whose members remain together for an
extended period eventually will lose touch with critical aspects of the external environment. The
first exception is research and development teams, for which there is some evidence in support of
the conventional view that the relationship between time together and group performance is
curvilinear. In a study of 50 research and development project teams, Katz (1982) found that
team performance improved over the first two years that members worked together, remained
high until around their fourth year, and then declined. Even if these teams had become
increasingly competent in working together over a longer term, their rate of improvement would
have decreased over time. And without the addition of new members with new perspectives,
their gains surely would have been reversed by the loss of fresh input from external sources—a
matter of some consequence for research teams.
- 38 -

The second exception is the risk that stable, long-tenure groups may become increasingly
insular and rely excessively on habitual routines for performing their standard tasks (Gersick &
Hackman, 1990). When these routines are executed inattentively, which is not uncommon for
long-tenure groups, the accrued benefits of team longevity can be more than negated by
unexpected and unnoticed contextual changes that render standard performance strategies
irrelevant or inappropriate.

Mechanisms

Robust understanding of the main research findings about team stability and longevity
(i.e., that more is better), as well as the two exceptions just discussed (i.e., research teams and the
risk of over-reliance on habitual routines), requires consideration of the mechanisms that
generate these effects (Arrow & McGrath, 1995). Conventional wisdom is right that the longer
group members work together, the more they are able to predict one another‘s behaviors, the
more knowledgeable they are about other members‘ quirks, and the more mutual acceptance they
are likely to develop. But rather than posing problems for the group, these developments make it
possible for group members to work together smoothly and, potentially, well.

How does this happen? Research and theory to date have focused mainly on the
cognitive explanations previously described—specifically, the development of transactive
memory (Wegner, 1986) and the creation of shared mental models. For example, Moreland and
his colleagues have shown that groups whose members train together develop transactive
memory systems that can significantly enhance group performance (Moreland, 1999; Moreland
& Myaskovsky, 2000). Moreover, they are likely to develop a robust shared mental model of
their performance situation, which can both smooth group interaction and facilitate performance
effectiveness (Moreland & Argote, 2003).

The benefits of team stability are not merely cognitive, however; affective and emotional
processes also evolve as group members gain experience working together. Socialization
processes, although generally viewed as applying exclusively to new members, actually continue
to operate throughout members‘ time working together—and they shape the affective tone of a
group as well as members‘ cognitive states (Levine, Moreland, & Choi, 2001). Specifically,
affective socialization can reduce the degree to which members, both individually and
collectively, experience ambiguity and anxiety and thereby can increase team learning
capabilities (Goodman & Shah, 1992; also see the section on group learning in this chapter).
Moreover, as group members spend more time together—especially if they come to feel that they
are doing reasonably well in achieving the group‘s purposes—they are likely to develop a sense
of collective efficacy (Bandura, 2000) and, perhaps, become a more cohesive social unit.

Group cohesiveness, although commonly invoked in distinguishing effective from failing


groups, is something of a fraught concept (Beal, Cohen, Burke, & McLendon, 2003; Hogg, 1993;
Mullen & Cooper, 1994). On the one hand, it has long been known that cohesive groups
generate greater pressures for member conformity (Festinger, 1950) and, moreover, that
members of such groups are disposed to accede to those pressures (Lott & Lott, 1965), which can
inhibit both team learning and the correction of errors. On the other hand, cohesiveness often is
- 39 -

viewed as a desirable state of affairs that helps groups succeed in achieving their purposes
(Chang & Bordia, 2001). For lay persons, the latter view is not unreasonable. When one
experiences the co-incidence of high cohesiveness and group effectiveness it can be hard to resist
the logical error of assuming that former is responsible for the latter. In fact, cohesiveness is
neither as pernicious as the groupthink model posits nor as generally advantageous as lay persons
and some scholars occasionally have suggested.

Knowledge about the antecedents and consequences of group cohesiveness has remained
unsettled throughout more than fifty years of research on the topic, which may reflect problems
with the construct validity of the concept itself, particularly regarding the basis of a group‘s
cohesiveness. It has been suggested, for example, that cohesiveness based on a shared
commitment to the work of the group is more functional for groups and their members than that
based mainly on highly valued social relationships, since a focus on intragroup relationships can
be at the expense of attention to changing external realities (Hackman, 1992; Chang & Bordia,
2001; Zaccarro & Lowe, 1988). Until such matters are resolved, any conclusion that the positive
features of longer-tenure groups are due mainly to their greater cohesiveness should be drawn
tentatively and with great caution.

Overcoming the Liabilities of Newness

The liabilities of newness discussed above appear to be of a kind that could be remedied
by leadership or consultative interventions that foster altertness to the risks of mindlessly
executed habitual routines and that help groups develop strategies for staying closely in touch
with developments in their external contexts. It is more challenging to identify ways of dealing
with the uncertain and shifting membership of new group forms (e.g., groups with constantly
changing composition whose members interact mainly using electronic technologies) as well as
those whose primary purpose has to do with the development of the team or its members rather
than execution of a specific, finite piece of work (e.g., academic seminars or research teams
whose purposes include the development of junior members into autonomous scientists).

One managerial strategy for dealing with these problems would be to emphasize the
training of individual members rather than the team as a whole, in hopes that people who have
been well trained can be swapped in and out of teams much like parts of a standardized
mechanical system. Such a strategy has the potential of minimizing both the inefficiencies
involved in starting up a new team and the performance decrements that are encountered as
members learn how to work together. This approach has been used with success with military
aviators (Leedom & Simon, 1995), but the generality of its applicability remains to be assessed.

A second approach would be to establish team stability and continuity one level higher in
the social system where the team operates. That is, specific teams would form and re-form in
response to changing circumstances, but the social system from which members are drawn would
remain relatively stable. Overall purposes, norms, and contextual supports are properties of the
parent system, and would persist over time—only the composition of particular teams would
change. Because members could import into those teams what they already know and are ready
to execute, some of the normal costs and potential liabilities of team start-up would be
circumvented. These kinds of teams, which were characterized earlier in this chapter as ―sand
- 40 -

dune‖ teams, may be especially appropriate for the new, technology-intensive group and
organizational forms that are emerging. It would be good to have more empirical data about
them, especially about the conditions that enable them to operate efficiently and productively.

Compositional Diversity

The study of diversity in groups is an extraordinarily active area of research on a topic of


great scholarly and societal importance. Moreover, two superb reviews of the vast and rapidly
expanding literature on diversity in groups recently have appeared: an Annual Review of
Psychology chapter on ―Work group diversity‖ by Daan van Knippenberg and Michaela
Schippers, and an analysis of ―What differences make a difference?‖ in Psychological Science in
the Public Interest by Elizabeth Mannix and Margaret Neale (Mannix & Neale, 2005; van
Knippenberg & Schippers, 2007). These reviews, along with an edited volume on diversity and
groups in the Research on Managing Groups and Teams series (Phillips, 2008) provide
comprehensive coverage of developments in research and theory on compositional diversity
since the assessment of the state of the field published a decade ago by Williams and O‘Reilly
(1998).

Rather than undertake yet another review and analysis of the same material, therefore,
this section raises some specific issues that appear to have special relevance for understanding
diversity in purposive groups that operate within social systems, relying heavily on the insights
and analyses provided in the reviews cited above.

What is Compositional Diversity?

It would seem simple to assess how diverse a given group is—just see if the members are
different from one another or if they are pretty much the same. But as the reviewers cited above
have noted and explained in considerable detail, there actually are many different ways of
conceptualizing the differences among members. The simplest definition of a ―difference‖ is
provided in the Williams and O‘Reilly review: ―…for our purposes, the effects of diversity can
result from any attribute people use to tell themselves that another person is different‖ (1998, p.
81). But for scholarly purposes, are some differences of greater interest or consequence than
others? Which ones, and how should they be conceptualized and measured? Although there are
no straightforward answers to those questions, both Mannix-Neale and van Knippenberg-
Schippers find the concept of ―faultlines‖ to be a useful device for dealing with differences
among members.

A faultline, as formulated by Lau and Murnighan (1998, 2005) is a constructed category


used to divide a group into subgroups using multiple attributes simultaneously—for example,
young Hispanic females vs. older Asian males. The concept, which has much in common with
Alderfer‘s (1997) idea of ―embedded intergroups,‖ provides a way of partitioning a group that
maximizes between-subgroup variation relative to within-subgroup variation. Faultlines have
been shown to be an efficient and powerful concept for the analysis of compositional diversity
and its effects (Homan, van Knippenberg, Van Kleef, & De Dreu, 2007; Polzer, Crisp,
Jarvenpaa, & Kim, 2006).
- 41 -

What are the Effects of Compositional Diversity?

Both the Mannix-Neale and van Kinppenberg-Schippers reviews conclude that there are
no substantial main effects of diversity on group outcomes—it neither helps nor impairs groups
reliably. Both reviewers then proceed to do what scholars almost always do when a presumably
powerful factor does not generate strong, consistent main effects: they search for moderators,
and they find some good possibilities. That is the slippery slope that has tempted scholars in a
wide variety of substantive areas, from the study of psychic phenomena to the search for the
traits of effective leaders (to be discussed later in this chapter). As research progresses, so many
contingencies are identified and documented that conceptual models become inelegant and
practical advice impossible.

Moderators also invite conservatism in thought and action. If it should be, for example,
that the effects of compositional diversity depend on certain enduring features of the social
system context (as they do: Mannix-Neale, p. 43), what is to be done with that knowledge? Or
how might one alter members‘ deeply seated diversity beliefs and perspectives if they turned out
to moderate composition-outcome relationships (as they also do)? The implication would seem
to be that homogeneous groups should be created in certain contexts, and diverse groups created
in others. But that strategy implicitly overlooks an important reality about compositional
diversity—namely, that diverse groups always have a greater pool of knowledge, skill,
experience, and perspectives than do homogeneous groups. The question is how to capture and
utilize those resources.

Beyond Mechanisms and Moderators

Both the Mannix-Neale and the van Knippenberg-Schippers reviews delve deeply and
informatively into the mechanisms that are responsible for the dynamics of compositionally
diverse groups. Multiple social and psychological processes operate simultaneously when group
members deal with others who are distinctively different from themselves and their own groups.
For one thing, members are more likely to like, affiliate with, and relate harmoniously to similar
others. The other side of the same coin is the human tendency to categorize others based on their
most salient or distinctive attributes, which can lay the groundwork for stereotyping other-group
members relative to members of one‘s own group. Together, these two mechanisms increase the
likelihood that, at least initially, the dynamics of diverse groups will be more halting than
harmonious.

Harmonious interaction is indeed pleasant, but it does not facilitate group performance or
individual learning. Indeed, working through task-based conflicts and disagreements can be more
helpful for performance and learning than either smoothing them over or basking in the pleasure
of interacting with similar, like-minded colleagues. Once group members come to realize, if
they do, that the knowledge, skills, perspectives and experiences of their diverse teammates are
valuable collective resources, group dynamics can become both more interesting and more
productive than is typically the case for homogeneous groups.
- 42 -

Although research on the mechanisms that shape the dynamics of diverse groups
generates interesting and informative findings, knowing why something happens is not the same
thing as knowing how to create it or to repair it when it goes bad. As medical researchers know
well, one does not really understand a malady until one can both create and cure it at will. The
implications of this view for research on compositional diversity are clear. Rigorous scholarship
on mechanisms and moderators should continue unabated. But it also may be of value to
supplement those studies with research that is framed quite differently—specifically, to
experiment with creating conditions that could increase the chances, although not guarantee, that
diverse groups will become increasingly able to identify, value, and actually use the full
complement of what their members bring.

Almost certainly such interventions would involve the simultaneous introduction of


multiple conditions that are themselves intercorrelated and that may even be redundant in some
ways. Introducing multiple, redundant, correlated interventions (read: independent variables) is
of course taboo for scholars who aspire to tease out specific causes that are tightly linked to
specific effects. But that is exactly what those who seek to make constructive differences in
social systems do. And, interestingly, such interventions also would have a nice resonance with
the concept of faultlines, itself a concept with multiple attributes that are intercorrelated within
the ecologies of social systems.

Research on compositional diversity has come a long way in the decade since the
Williams-O‘Reilly review, as has been impressively demonstrated by Mannix-Neale and van
Knippenberg-Schippers. It may now be time to return to the Lewinian roots of group psychology
and devise action research projects that have the potential to further advance knowledge even as
they help groups find new ways to cultivate and use well their members‘ diverse resources.

The Context of Group Behavior and Performance

There are no truly freestanding groups. All groups operate within some broader social
system whose properties and processes affect group behavior. Although contextual influences
are more frequently discussed in reports of field than laboratory research, they also are present
and powerful in the experimental laboratory. The difference is that in the laboratory they are
established by the experimenter rather than by organizational authority figures. It is the
experimenter who selects and arranges the physical space where the research will be conducted,
specifies group purposes, decides what rewards will be available and administers them, provides
the resources that teams require for their work, and establishes the basic norms of conduct that
guide behavior in the setting. In effect, the experimenter creates and manages a social system—
albeit a small and temporary one—that serves as the context of the group. Precisely because
contexts are so powerful, expert experimenters go to great lengths to ensure that contextual
features are standardized for all groups in a given study.

Group contexts rarely are discussed in research reports, however. When context is
mentioned at all, it usually is as a constraint on external validity: ―Since the findings were
obtained in a laboratory setting, generalizations to organizational life should be made with
caution.‖ Or: ―Because the groups in this study all operated in a bureaucratic organization the
findings may not apply to network-type enterprises.‖ Or: ―These findings may not generalize to
- 43 -

Asian cultures where collective outcomes are more highly valued than they are in the United
States.‖ Although contextual features can indeed moderate the relationships among other
variables, transactions between groups and their contexts also are an integral part of everyday
group behavior (Ancona & Caldwell, 1992; Haas, 2006). As group researchers are gradually
coming to recognize, the context is part of the phenomenon of group dynamics, not merely a
factor that constrains the generalizability of empirical findings about them (Frey, 2003; Ilgen,
1999; Lacey & Gruenfeld, 1999; Putnam & Stohl, 1990; Rousseau & Fried, 2001; Sundstrom,
1999; Wageman, 1999).

Contexts also shape both what is studied and is found. Certain phenomena are highly
constrained in some contexts. If one is interested in studying the effects of task design on group
performance, for example, prospects for success are dim if the research is carried out in a setting
where a large number of groups perform essentially the same task, thereby limiting the
variability of the very phenomenon under study. By contrast, Sutton and Hargadon (1996) chose
to study group brainstorming in a product development firm, where the phenomenon was both
vivid and ubiquitous. The accessibility of certain contexts, moreover, can subtly shape
researchers choices about what to study. The ready availability to academically based
researchers of laboratory experimental groups, student project teams, and classroom
demonstrations such as ―survival‖ and negotiation exercises, for example, can result in bodies of
knowledge that may be more influenced than either scholars or research consumers realize by the
special features of academic contexts. The following sections identify some specific contextual
features that may deserve more attention by group researchers than they typically receive.

Cues and Contingencies

Groups in different contexts routinely deal with different kinds of content—the materials,
ideas, or issues with which they work. Top management teams, for example, deal constantly
with power and influence; task forces with ideas and plans; human service teams with emotions
and relationships; production teams with technology; and so on. As is shown in collections of
research studies on different types of teams, these differences in content strongly affect the
character of group interaction (Frey, 2003; Hackman, 1990).

It has long been known that context-supplied cues activate the motivational and
emotional states of individuals, and the mechanisms that drive these effects are well established
(Atkinson, 1954; Schachter, 1964; Schultheiss & Brunstein, 1999). Since all members of any
given group experience many of the same cues in the group‘s context, these states can diffuse
across members and spawn a common emotional interpretation of an arousing situation, or a
heightened (or depressed) level of collective motivation (Brown, 1965, chap. 14; Edmondson,
1999a).

Contexts also differ in the contingencies that link group behaviors with outcomes. In
some contexts, for example, achieving group purposes depends mainly on exerting sufficient
collective effort; in others, it is more a matter of applying the right knowledge and skill; in still
others, it depends heavily on how members approach and execute the work—the group‘s
performance strategy (Hackman & Wageman, 2005a,b). A robust understanding of group
behavior and performance, therefore, requires not just knowledge about the cues that are present
- 44 -

in its context but also about the behavior-outcome contingences that characterize the social
system within which the group operates. A great deal presently is understood about these
matters at the individual level of analysis, but much remains to be learned about how they are
assembled into processes and properties of the group-as-a-whole.

Purposes, Places, and Resources

With the exception of self-governing groups, the purposes that group pursue, as well as
the places where they operate and the resources and supports that are available for their work, are
contextually supplied. Considerable empirical research has been conducted on certain aspects of
the group context, such as reward contingencies (Beersma, Hollenbeck, Humphrey, Moon,
Conlon, & Ilgen, 2003; Lawler, 2003; Wageman & Baker, 1997; Zenger & Marshall, 2000), the
amount and kinds of information groups can obtain (Haas, 2006; Haas & Hansen, 2007;
Hargadon, 1999), and the availability of educational resources to foster team learning (Salas,
Nichols, & Driskell, 2007). Relatively little systematic evidence has accumulated about the
appropriateness of groups‘ spatial arrangements or the munificence of the material resources that
are available for carrying out group work.

Research on physical space would be especially welcome. Many years ago, Hall (1969)
and Barker (1968) explored how settings shape social behavior. Hall used the terms ―fixed
feature space‖ and ―semi-fixed‖ feature space to refer to the permanence vs. moveability of the
physical surround. Steele (1973) subsequently added the term ―pseudo-fixed feature space‖ to
designate physical features that are readily changeable but that are treated as if they were fixed—
for example, when members of a small discussion group meet in a room where all chairs are
aligned in forward-facing rows but do not rearrange them before beginning their discussion.
More recent commentators also have discussed how the properties of physical spaces shape and
constrain group behavior (Burgoon, 1983; Gladwell, 2000), but the leads they have generated
await systematic empirical investigation.

Authorities, Clients, and Other Groups

The contextual influences discussed above are for the most part subtle, things one might
not notice if one were not looking for them. By contrast, authority figures and other groups are
among the most pervasive and salient features of a group‘s context. Few groups can accomplish
their purposes without coordinating with external groups and authorities, obtaining information
from them, receiving their feedback, or relying on them for assistance of some kind (Ancona &
Caldwell, 1992; Haas, 2006).

The relationship between groups and the authority figures in their contexts has long been
of central concern in psychodynamic analyses of group behavior, discussed earlier in this
chapter. These days, the field also is seeing substantially increased attention to groups‘
relationships with other groups—those a group serves, those with which it must coordinate to
achieve its purposes, and those with which it is in competition (for details, see the chapters in
this Handbook on inter-group relationships by De Dreu and by Yzerbyt & Demoulin). Indeed,
the active stream of research in the communications literature on what are called ―bona fide‖
groups gives extensive attention to the inter-group context of group behavior (Stohl & Walker,
- 45 -

2002), including transactions that extend even beyond groups‘ own organizational boundaries
into the external environment (Lacey & Gruenfeld, 1999; Schopler, 1987). As inter-
organizational relationships become more extensive and more global in reach, it would not be
surprising to see research on inter-group coordination become as extensive in future years as
research on intra-group interaction has been heretofore.

Collective Values

Perhaps the most powerful contextual feature of all in shaping group behavior and
performance is the value that the social system within which a group operates places on
collaborative work. Some years ago, Walton (1985) identified two general constellations of
organizational values, which he refers to as ―control‖ vs. ―commitment‖ orientations. In the
former, authorities impose control over all aspects of the work in the pursuit of efficiency; in the
latter, they seek to generate and utilize member commitment to achieve collective purposes. As
Walton subsequently demonstrated, group life was strikingly different under these two sets of
values (Walton & Hackman, 1986). In control-type organizations, groups generally were
spontaneously formed by organization members themselves to provide social satisfaction and
some level of protection for members. Management, at best, ignored such groups and often
attempted to kill them off. In commitment-type organizations, by contrast, management actively
created and fostered work groups of various kinds and came to depend heavily upon them for
accomplishing organizational objectives.

As Walton‘s research shows, group life is profoundly affected by the values that pervade
the broader social system. Further research on the value context of groups, including those that
differ across national cultures, could be highly informative in furthering understanding of the
other features of organizational contexts discussed above, as well as the dynamics of group-
context relationships more generally.

Temporal Issues

Over the last two decades, temporal issues have become an increasingly prominent theme
in research on group behavior and performance. Among the most influential early studies on
time were Gersick‘s (1988, 1989) empirical studies of the temporal pacing of group
development, research on the temporal entrainment of group activities by Kelly and McGrath
(1985; McGrath & Kelly, 1986), and the analysis by Ancona and Chong (1996) of the ways
pace, cycles, and rhythm shape group behavior. There followed numerous books and articles
expanding on those topics and exploring other temporal phenomena (e.g., Ancona, Okhuysen, &
Perlow, 2001; Blount, 2004; McGrath & Tschan (2004, 2007).

The flow of research on the temporal issues shows no sign of abating. As will be seen in
the next section of this chapter, the impact of leader coaching strongly depends upon the time in
the group life cycle when they are provided. But temporal dynamics also are consequential for
groups in several additional ways, described below.
- 46 -

Beginnings

Most frameworks that describe the process of group development specify a number of
stages through which groups pass, with movement into each successive stage contingent on
successful completion of the previous one. The ―forming-storming-norming-performing‖
sequence described by Tuckman (1965) is perhaps the best known of the stage models. As
plausible as the Tuckman model is, research on temporal dynamics in groups have raised
questions about its generality and validity. In a field study of the entire life histories of a number
of project teams, for example, Gersick (1988) found that each group she tracked developed a
distinctive approach to its task immediately after commencing work, and stayed with that
approach until almost exactly halfway between its first meeting and the project deadline. At that
point, all teams experienced a ―midpoint transition‖ that involved dropping previous patterns of
behavior and adopting new roles and performance strategies. This new approach remained in
place until very near the project deadline, at which time the group focused on a new set of issues
having to do with termination.

The most widely discussed of these findings, subsequently replicated in the experimental
laboratory (Gersick, 1989), has been the midpoint transition, perhaps because it most clearly
signaled that group development is shaped more by temporal factors than by progressing through
a fixed series of stages (Okhuysen & Waller, 2002; Waller, Zellmer-Bruhn, & Giambatista,
2002). But equally as important in understanding group development is what happened at the
beginning of the lives of the groups she studied—namely, that their initial pattern of interaction,
which they typically fell into immediately and without deliberation, established the track on
which they stayed for the entire first half of their life cycle. The same thing was found by
Ginnett (1993) for an entirely different kind of group—aircraft flightdeck crews. Specifically,
what happened during a crew‘s pre-flight briefing shaped members‘ behavior for a considerable
time thereafter. Moreover, getting off to a good, fast start may be a prerequisite for success: In a
study of high- and low-performing project teams, Ericksen and Dyer (2004) found that the high-
performing teams mobilized themselves more quickly and participatively than did low-
performing teams. The very beginning of a group‘s life or task cycle, it appears, is highly
consequential both for the life of the group and for its eventual performance.

Entrainment, Pacing, and Rhythm

The pace and rhythm that will characterize member interaction gets established very early
in a group‘s life, as groups adjust their activities to synchronize with the phase, periodicity, or
other temporal parameter of their work (Ancona & Chong, 1999, p. 36). A specific deadline, for
example, can drive the group‘s pace, and that pace will be maintained on subsequent tasks—even
when the amount of time available for the subsequent work changes (Kelly & McGrath, 1985).
When there is no deadline or it is ambiguous, groups do not establish a pace for their work and
tend to flounder (Davis-Sacks, 1990).

Although a temporal anchor may be required for a group to be able to pace itself on a
given type of task, entrainment apparently does not persist across different task types. In a study
by Harrison, Mohammed, McGrath, Florey, and Vanderstoep (2003), groups that initially were
entrained on a certain task changed their pace of work when on a second trial they were given a
- 47 -

task of a different kind. But they returned to the original pacing when on a third trial they once
again worked on a task of the first type.

Regular cycles, like time limits, can pace a group and generate a characteristic rhythm for
work activities. Semesters in academia have this quality, as do quarters for sales teams in
businesses (Gladstein, 1984). And even when there is no natural rhythm, as was the case for
teams at a continuously operating semiconductor plant studied by Abramis (1990), arbitrary
temporal markers almost always are put in place. At the semiconductor plant, these were six-
week production periods. Although these periods had no external referent whatever, they served
as the pivot around which all team planning and pacing took place. When natural temporal
markers do not exist, we apparently are driven to create them—and then to use them to give
rhythm to our work.

Constructions of Time

People differ in both their perspectives on time and their perceptions of temporal
markers. Those differences can have profound effects, especially when groups are composed of
members from different cultures (Ancona, Okhuysen, & Perlow, 2001; Gibson, Waller,
Carpenter, & Conte, 2007). They affect matters of substantive interest, of course, such as how
teams experience and deal with deadlines and the duration of time itself. But they also have
implications for the methodologies used by group researchers. How, for example, should the
temporal aspects of group behavior be studied when there is reason to believe that members may
differ substantially in time perspectives or perceptions? What interpretive and external validity
issues arise when research is done on groups whose members have a highly restricted time
horizon—that is, when members know that they will meet only once for one hour to perform one
task? To what extent do the temporal dynamics in such groups differ from those that have an
extended or indefinite time horizon, such as the groups tracked by McGrath and his colleagues as
their tasks and membership changed over time (McGrath, 1993)? As Ballard, Tschan, and
Waller (2008) suggest, temporal issues such as these merit considerably more thought and
attention by group researchers than they heretofore have received.

Team Leadership

The paradox about team leadership is this. On the one hand, observers so pervasively
attribute to leaders responsibility for team successes and failures that the phenomenon has been
characterized as the ―leader attribution error‖ by Hackman and Wageman (2005b) and as part of
the ―romance of leadership‖ by Meindl (1990). On the other hand, researchers over the years
have been unable to identify the particular traits or behavioral styles that reliably distinguish
great from so-so team leaders. Many different attributes of individuals have been found to be
modestly associated with rated leader effectiveness and (especially) with who is chosen to
occupy leadership positions (for an early review on traits and group behavior, see Mann, 1959;
for a contemporary and more trait-sympathetic review, see Zaccaro, 2007). But the usefulness of
these findings for guiding either theory development or leadership practice is limited because the
size of the empirical relationships typically has been so small.
- 48 -

Alternatives to Traits

If traits are not controlling, then perhaps anyone could be an effective group leader if he
or she learned the right ways to behave. But no one leadership style has been found that works
well across situations—a style that is effective in one situation may not work so well in another
(for a review, see Bass, 1990). Research on leader styles therefore evolved from a search for the
one best style to contingency models that specify which leader behaviors work best in which
circumstances (Fiedler & Garcia, 1987; Vroom & Jago, 2007). Such models identify those
attributes of the situation and of the group being led that determine what leader behaviors are
likely to be effective, thereby providing research-based guidance about how leaders ought to
behave in various circumstances (for a review, see Yukl, 2002, chap. 8).

As research identifies more and more moderators of the leader behavior-group outcome
relationship, contingency models necessarily become so complex that they can require of leaders
a level of on-line cognitive processing that exceeds human capabilities (Gigerenzer, 1999;
Simon, 1990). Moreover, direction of causality remains an open question. Although both
scholars and lay persons generally view leader behavior as the cause of team behavior, it has
long been known that in some circumstances the causal arrow points in the opposite direction
(Farris & Lim, 1969; Lowin & Craig, 1968). For example, a team that exhibits cooperation and
competence can elicit a considerate, participative leadership style, whereas one that is hostile and
incompetent can prompt a more directive style.

These findings suggest that individual differences among leaders may be more usefully
dealt with as competencies (McClelland, 1973), things the leader knows or knows how to do
rather than as either personality traits or behavioral styles (for a debate on this matter, see
Hollenbeck, McCall, & Silzer, 2006). Identification of the competencies most needed for
effective team leadership is still a work in progress (Gist & McDonald-Mann, 2000; Salas,
Kosarzycki, Tannenbaum, & Carnegie, 2004). Whatever the specifics turn out to be, they clearly
will include competencies both in diagnosing social systems (a cognitive capability that centrally
involves inductive conceptualization) and in taking constructive action to address group or
contextual problems and opportunities (Hackman & Walton, 1986).

Emergent Leadership

Although the main body of leadership research is about individuals who hold formal roles
as leaders, scholars are giving increasing attention to the emergence of leaders within task-
performing teams. This development can be at least partially attributed to the rise of interest in
self-managing work teams, especially those that explicitly are designed without anyone
designated as the formal team leader (Cohen, Ledford, & Spreitzer, 1996). Initially, research on
leadership emergence focused mainly on the personal attributes of those individuals who were
most likely to become a group leaders. General cognitive ability, for example, as well as certain
personality traits such as conscientiousness and extraversion have been shown to enhance an
individual‘s standing in the informal group structure (Neubert, 1999; Smith & Foti, 1998;
Taggar, Hackett, & Saha, 1999).
- 49 -

More recently, scholars have begun to examine the impact of the behavior of emergent
group leaders on group processes and performance. For example, emergent leaders who help a
group set realistic goals have been shown to enhance group efficacy (De Souza & Klein, 1995;
Pescosolido, 2001); and emergent leaders who help groups manage their emotional processes
help foster group identity and solidarity (Pescosolido, 2002). These studies have laid the
groundwork for recasting research on group leadership from its historical focus on individuals to
an emphasis on the accomplishment of those leadership functions that are most critical to group
effectiveness (Day, Gronn, & Salas, 2004; Hackman & Wageman, 2005b; Nye, 2008; Nohria &
Khurana, in press).

Leadership Functions

The functional approach to team leadership was first articulated by McGrath (1962), who
suggested that a team leader‘s main job ―is to do, or get done, whatever is not being adequately
handled for group needs‖ (p. 5). Effective leaders, therefore, do in their own idiosyncratic ways
whatever is necessary to ensure that those functions that are critical to the achievement of team
purposes are accomplished.

Leadership functions can be partitioned into two general classes: designing the group and
providing hands-on group leadership (Hackman & Walton, 1986). The latter class has received
the major portion of research attention, from the classic study of autocratic, democratic, and
laissez-faire leadership of boys‘ clubs by Lewin, Lippitt, and White (1939) to the present day.
But as Wageman (2001) has shown, it is the former function—getting the group set up right—
that turns out to make the most difference. Specifically, she found that leaders‘ design choices
controlled more than four time as much variation in both team self-management and
performance effectiveness as did their hands-on coaching activities. Moreover, well-designed
teams benefitted greatly from competent leader coaching and were not much hurt by bad
coaching. Poorly designed teams, by contrast, were not helped by competent coaching—and
were devastated by bad coaching.

Timing

Group researchers increasingly are recognizing that when a leadership intervention is


made can be as important as the content of that intervention or how skillfully it is delivered.
Work by Kozlowski and his colleagues, for example, shows that the kind of leadership assistance
teams need depends upon the stage of their development and, moreover, that there are specific
times in the group life cycle when teams are more and less open to receiving leadership
interventions (Kozlowski, Gully, McHugh, Salas, & Cannon-Bowers, 1996; Kozlowski, Watola,
Nowakowski, Kim, & Botero, 2009). Because the issues that are most salient for a group evolve
as it develops, Kozlowski and his colleagues suggest that the proper focus of leader-facilitated
―learning sessions‖ is quite different for newly formed vs. mature groups. Similar conclusions
are reached by Hackman and Wageman (2005a), who give special attention to the kinds of
coaching interventions that are most impactful at the beginnings, midpoints, and ends of task
performance cycles. They posit that even competently delivered coaching will not be helpful if it
addresses issues that are not alive for a team at the time it is provided.
- 50 -

Overall, research has identified four times when leader actions can make a large and
constructive difference in team behavior. One, before the group even convenes, when the leader
can structure the group and arrange for resources and contextual supports that facilitate
competent teamwork (e.g., Hackman, 2002). Two, when the group begins work on its task, at
which point the leader can bound the group, help members become oriented to one another and
to their collective work, and foster collective motivation to perform that work well (e.g., Ginnett,
1993). Three, at the midpoint of the task cycle, when the group has logged some actual
experience on the task and the leader can help members reflect on and improve the
appropriateness of its performance strategy (e.g., Woolley, 1998). And four, at the end of the
task cycle, when the leader can help members learn from their collective experiences and thereby
strengthen the group‘s overall complement of knowledge and skill (Smith-Jentsch, Cannon-
Bowers, Tannenbaum, & Salas, 2008).

Shared Leadership

Shared leadership has been discussed, usually hopefully but not always favorably, since
the earliest days of group research (Gibb, 1954). The phenomenon has become increasingly
relevant recently as organizations experiment with co-leaders (Heenan & Bennis, 1999) and with
widely distributed leadership in self-governing groups and organizations (Cheney, 1999). The
research record on the efficacy of shared leadership is decidedly mixed (Pearce & Conger, 2003).
On the one hand, when leadership is shared there are by definition more resources available to
fulfill leadership functions; on the other, coordination difficulties, social loafing, and struggles
for power or dominance can significantly erode the utilization of those resources.

The tension between the risks and benefits of shared leadership are perhaps most vividly
seen in leadership teams—that is, teams composed of individual leaders whose collective task is
to provide leadership to a broader organizational unit or to a whole enterprise (Berg, 2005;
Edmondson, Roberto, & Watkins, 2003; Hambrick, 2007; Wageman, Nunes, Burruss, &
Hackman, 2008). Such teams are composed of powerful, competent people who value being on
the team and who, collectively, can command whatever resources as are needed to help the team
succeed. Yet the teams themselves are under-designed, under-resourced, riddled with
undiscussable authority dynamics, and so inefficient that members despair over the time they
have to spend in them (Wageman & Hackman, in press). Precisely because group processes in
these teams are simultaneously vivid and fraught, the lessons learned from research on them may
be especially useful in identifying what is required for the effective sharing of team leadership
more generally.

Collaboration in Dispersed Groups

As organizations become more global in reach and as communications and information


processing technologies become more powerful and pervasive, increasing numbers of groups are
relying on these technologies for carrying out their work. Technology is evolving so quickly that
social science research on virtual and distributed teams necessarily is lagging somewhat behind
group and organizational practice. Nonetheless, a good deal has been learned about groups
- 51 -

whose members communicate mainly electronically, and the pace of research on such groups is
accelerating.

The earliest research on distributed team processes was carried out in the 1990s, and
generally involved direct comparisons of face-to-face and ―virtual‖ teams in laboratory settings.
These studies treated virtuality as a dichotomous variable: members interacted either face to face
or using electronic technologies exclusively (e.g., Hollingshead, McGrath, & O‘Conner, 1993;
McLeod, Baron, Marti, & Yoon, 1997; Weisband, 1992). These days, even co-located teams
employ one or more forms of computer-mediated interaction, whether email, instant messaging,
desktop videoconferencing, file-and-application sharing, group decision support systems,
electronic bulletin boards, or real-time calendar and scheduling systems. Thus, virtuality now is
more appropriately understood as a matter of degree rather than as identifying a particular type
of team (Bell & Kozlowski, 2002; Martins, Gilson, & Maynard, 2004).

Among the teams that rely most extensively on electronic technologies for
communication and coordination are those whose members are geographically dispersed.
Indeed, a recent analysis by O‘Leary and Cummings (2007) suggests that the nature and extent
of a team‘s dispersion, rather than the number or kind of electronic aids a group uses, may
provide the greatest leverage in understanding its dynamics. Specifically, O‘Leary and
Cummings distinguish among, and provide indices for assessing, three types of dispersion:
spatial, temporal, and configural. Spatial dispersion, the average physical distance among team
members, lessens the possibility of face-to-face communication. Temporal dispersion, the extent
to which group members‘ work or awake hours overlap, lessens the possibility of synchronous
problem-solving. Configural dispersion, the number of different locations where members are
located and the distribution of members across those locations, increases the complexity of
coordination and subgroup relationships.

Dispersed groups can be just as ―real‖ as face-to-face groups—that is, they can be as
bounded, interdependent, and stable over time as those whose members do their work around a
common table. The difference is their greater reliance on communication and information
processing technologies for planning and executing the team‘s work. The body of research
findings about what commonly are called virtual teams, therefore, can perhaps more
appropriately be understood as providing insight into the dynamics of real-but-dispersed teams
that necessarily use electronic technologies for communication and coordination.

What Is Known

It is reasonably well established that use of electronic technologies reduces both the
overall amount of communication in a group and members‘ sense of a shared group identity
(Bhappu, Griffith, & Northcraft, 1997; Hiltz, Johnson, & Turoff, 1986; Hollingshead, 1996;
Straus, 1996), although to a lesser extent for groups whose members have experience working
together (Alge, Wiethoff, & Klein, 2003; Bouas & Arrow, 1996). Use of electronic technologies
does tend to equalize member participation (Kiesler, Siegel, & McGuire, 1984; Straus, 1996;
Zigurs, Poole, & DeSanctis, 1988), as well as to increase the likelihood that minority members
will express their views (McLeod, Baron, Marti, & Yoon, 1997), and to reduce members‘
attentiveness to ingroup-outgroup differences (Bhappu, Griffith, & Northcraft, 1997). These
- 52 -

phenomena may occur because electronic technologies mask some social cues and therefore
attenuate the impact of status differences and subgroup memberships on participation dynamics
(Dubrovsky, Kiesler, & Sethna, 1991; Hollingshead, 1996; Sproull & Kiesler, 1986).

The content of group interaction can be problematic in groups that use electronic
technologies, however. Members are more likely to exhibit uninhibited behavior such as insults,
swearing, and name-calling (Siegel, Dubrovsky, Kiesler, & McGuire, 1986; Sproull & Kiesler,
1986). Moreover, they tend to make what Wageman (2003) calls ―sinister‖ attributions about the
commitment of teammates who do not respond quickly enough to team needs. When
communications break down because of a technological failure, for example, the resulting
silence may be wrongly assumed to reflect a member‘s poor work ethic (Cramton & Orvis,
2003).

Although teams that rely on electronic technologies do not apply less effort to their work
than face-to-face teams, reliance on electronic technologies does impair group performance for
tasks that require high levels of coordination in real time (Shepherd, Briggs, Reinig, Yen, &
Nunamaker, 1996; Siegel, Dubrovsky, Kiesler, & McGuire, 1986; Straus & McGrath, 1994).
Such groups generally take longer to get things done (Graetz, Boyle, Kimble, Thompson, &
Garloch, 1998; Hollingshead, 1996; Straus, 1996; Weisband, 1992), and they tend to have
problems staying on schedule and budget (McDonough, Kahn, & Barczak, 2001). Beyond the
reality that typing is slower than talking, members of groups that communicate using computers
also may be tempted to multi-task while at the computer rather than to focus exclusively on the
team‘s work (Lebie, Rhoades, & McGrath, 1996; Malhotra, Majchrzak, Carman, & Lott, 2001).

Research Frontiers

Advanced communication and computational technologies have become commonplace in


collaborative work. Sometimes it is a matter of necessity, such as for groups whose members are
globally dispersed. Or it may be for economic reasons, to lessen the costs in time and travel for
group meetings. Or it may simply be because younger members have grown up with such
technologies and would find it strange not to use them routinely. It may be time, therefore, for
group scholars to move beyond studies that further document the risks and benefits of electronic
technologies, and to focus more on identifying the conditions that increase the likelihood that
groups will use them well. Such a development would parallel what is happening in research on
compositional diversity, discussed earlier—a movement from documentation of the main-effect
risks and benefits of diversity to identification of the conditions that enable groups to recognize,
value, and fully utilize their members‘ differences.

Although it is not yet known what conditions will emerge as most critical in fostering the
effective use of electronic tools by groups, one point of departure for research and theory could
be what already is known about the conditions that foster team effectiveness (Gibson & Cohen,
2003). It is reasonably well established, for example, that performance is facilitated when teams
are small and compositionally stable, with clear but permeable boundaries and interdependence
for some consequential shared purpose (Hackman, 2002). What is not known is whether these
same conditions are critical for teams whose members heavily use electronic means of
communication and coordination. One could argue, for example, that electronic technologies
- 53 -

render team structures less critical than otherwise would be the case, since these technologies
make it possible for members to continuously and autonomously adjust in real time everything
from their work strategies to the composition of the group itself. But one also could argue that
enabling structures are more important for technology-intensive dispersed groups because such
groups run a greater risk of devolving into disorder and disorganization than those whose
members work in the same place at the same time. Whatever the key conditions for
effectiveness for dispersed teams turn out to be, it surely will be a significant team leadership
challenge to create and maintain them when members are scattered across geographies and time
zones—also a matter worthy of attention by group researchers (Cummings, 2007).

Another research challenge is to identify the proper mix of face-to-face and electronically
mediated interaction. Research has shown that it is helpful to temper electronic collaboration in
dispersed teams with periodic face-to-face interaction (Maznevski & Chudoba, 2001). The
emerging question is to determine when in the team life cycle face-to-face contact is most useful.
Studies of the temporal aspects of group leadership, discussed earlier, identified beginnings,
midpoints, and ends as times when groups are most ready to accept and make use of leadership
interventions. Might these three times also be ones when face-to-face interaction is most useful?
The importance of face-to-face engagement at the beginning of a group‘s work has been
empirically demonstrated (DeMeyer, 1991; Robey, Khoo, & Powers, 2000). Midpoints and ends
of task cycles might also be times when it would be helpful for group members to come
physically together to debrief and to reflect on how they might best utilize their electronic
resources in carrying out their collective work.

The number of different technologies available for use by task-performing groups has
increased dramatically in recent years, and the rate of growth shows no sign of abating. Indeed,
a sufficient diversity of electronic resources has accumulated that it may now be possible to
empirically identify those types of technologies that are, and are not, especially useful to groups
that perform different types of tasks—for example, those that involve the generation of ideas vs.
the evaluation of issues vs. the implementation of actions. As knowledge about such task by
technology interactions accumulates, groups will become increasingly able to wisely select the
kinds of electronic resources that are most appropriate for their particular circumstances. And,
perhaps, they may become less inclined than some groups are at present either to resist using
such technologies at all or, alternatively, to adopt relatively mindlessly whatever new
technological resources as become available.

Conclusion

As noted at the outset of this chapter, groups evoke strong feelings—people tend either to
love them or to despair of them. Scholars, through their research, pull in the tails of this
distribution as they identify and document the structures, contexts, and leader behaviors that can
help groups circumvent their nastiest dysfunctions—and, occasionally, capture the synergies of
which the best groups actually are capable. The aspiration of this chapter has been to provide
scholarly readers an enriched understanding of the intellectual history of group research as well
as an overview of the current frontiers of the field.
- 54 -

The 1940s and 1950s were heady days for group research, a time when a variety of
approaches were tried, found to not quite provide what was needed, and then be supplanted by
fresh ideas about how to study group behavior and performance. The lessons learned in those
years have much to teach contemporary group researchers. Specifically, the six approaches
reviewed in this chapter—the psychodynamic, network, action, process-focused, decision-
analytic, and complex systems approaches—offer some tantalizing leads for the future.

The psychodynamic approach reminds us that some of the most powerful influences on
group dynamics are implicit and mostly hidden from view. The network approach invites us to
apply today‘s powerful computational tools to generate new understanding of the evolution over
time of relationships within and among groups. The action approach cautions us not to focus too
much on the search for tight causal links between inputs, processes, and outputs and, instead, to
explore ways to create conditions that foster the achievement of normatively valued outcomes.
The process-focused approach also orients our attention to computational tools, this time to chart
patterns of member interaction—who says what to whom when with what effect—at a level of
detail never before possible. The decision-analytic approach offers a way to go beyond the
search for additional cognitive dysfunctions and to seek instead strategies that help groups draw
on the full complement of members‘ information and expertise in making collective judgments
and decisions. And the complex systems approach reminds us that groups, even if small and
short-lived, really are dynamic social systems that create and redefine the very realities that
shape their own behavior.

In 1974, Ivan Steiner ended his classic Journal of Experimental Social Psychology article
on ―Whatever Happened to the Group in Social Psychology?‖ with these words: ―If the tea
leaves tell me true, social psychology in the late 70‘s is going to look a lot like social psychology
in the late 40‘s, better, of course, but groupy once more (Steiner, 1974, p. 106). It did not happen
back then, but if we combine the lessons from the history of small group research with the
knowledge and research tools that now have become available, it very well could be that Steiner
was absolutely right—if just a few decades early.
- 55 -

References

Abramis, D. J. (1990). Semiconductor manufacturing team. In J. R. Hackman (Ed.), Groups that


work (and those that don’t) (pp. 449-470). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

Alcoholics Anonymous. (2001). Alcoholics anonymous. New York: Alcoholics Anonymous


World Services.

Aldag, R. J. & Fuller, S. R. (1993). Beyond fiasco: A reappraisal of the groupthink phenomenon
and a new model of group decision processes. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 533-552.

Alderfer, C. P. (1977). Group and intergroup relations. In J. R. Hackman & J. L. Suttle (Eds.),
Improving life at work (pp. 227-296). Santa Monica, CA: Goodyear.

Alderfer, C. P. (1987). An intergroup perspective on group dynamics. In J. Lorsch (Ed.),


Handbook of organizational behavior (pp. 190-222). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
Hall.

Alderfer, C. P. (1997). Embedded intergroup relations and racial identity theory. In C. E.


Thompson & R. T. Carter (Eds.), Applications of racial identity development theory:
Individual, group, and organizational interventions (pp. 237-263). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.

Alge, B. J., Wiethoff, C., & Klein, H. J. (2003). When does the medium matter? Knowledge-
building experiences and opportunities in decision-making teams. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91, 26-37.

Allen, N. J., & Hecht, T. D. (2004). The ‗romance of teams‘: Toward an understanding of its
psychological underpinnings and implications. Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology, 77, 439-461.

Allmendinger, J., Hackman, J. R., & Lehman, E. V. (1996). Life and work in symphony
orchestras. The Musical Quarterly, 80, 194-219.

Allport, F. H. (1924). Social psychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Ancona, D., & Bresman, H. (2007). X-teams: How to build teams that lead, innovate, and
succeed. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.

Ancona, D. G., & Caldwell, D. F. (1992). Bridging the boundary: External activity and
performance in organizational teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37, 634-665.

Ancona, D. G., & Caldwell, D. F. (1992). Demography and design: Predictors of new product
team performance. Organization Science, 3, 321-341.
- 56 -

Ancona, D. G., & Chong, C. (1996). Entrainment: Pace, cycle, and rhythm in organizational
behavior. Research in Organizational Behavior, 18, 251-284.

Ancona, D. G., & Nadler, D. A. (1989, Fall). Top hats and executive tales: Designing the senior
team. Sloan Management Review, 19-29.

Ancona, D. G., Okhuysen, G. A., & Perlow, L. A. (2001). Taking time to integrate temporal
research. Academy of Management Review, 26, 512-529.

Andrews, K. T., Ganz, M., Baggetta, M., Han, H., & Lin, C. (in press). Leadership, membership,
and voice: Civic associations that work. American Journal of Sociology.

Argote, L. (1999). Organizational learning: Creating, retaining, and transferring knowledge.


Norwell, MA: Kluwer.

Argote, L., Gruenfeld, D., & Naquin, C. (2001). Group learning in organizations. In M. E.
Turner (Ed.), Groups at work: Theory and research (pp. 369-411). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.

Argote, L., Ingram, P., Levine, J. M., & Moreland, R. L. (2000). Knowledge transfer in
organizations: Learning from the experience of others. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 82, 1-8.

Argote, L., Insko, C. A., Yovetich, N., & Romero, A. A. (1995). Group learning curves: The
effects of turnover and task complexity on group performance. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 25, 512-529.

Argyris, C. (1964). T-Groups for organizational effectiveness. Harvard Business Review, 42(2),
60-74.

Argyris, C. (1969). The incompleteness of social psychological theory: Examples from small
group, cognitive consistency, and attribution research. American Psychologist, 24, 893-
908.

Argyris, C. (1993). Education for leading-learning. Organizational Dynamics, 21(3), 5-17.

Argyris, C., Putnam, R., & Smith, D. M. (1985). Action science. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Aronson, E., & Patnoe, S. (1997). The jigsaw method: Building cooperation in the classroom
(2nd ed.). New York: Longman.

Arrow, H. (1997). Stability, bistability, and instability in small group influence patterns. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 75-85.
- 57 -

Arrow, H. (2005). Chaos, complexity, and catastrophe: The nonlinear dynamics perspective. In
S. A. Wheelan (Ed.), The handbook of group research and practice (pp. 201-220).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Arrow, H., & Burns, K. L. (2004). Self-organizing culture: How norms emerge in small groups.
In M. Schaller & C. S. Crandall (Eds.), The psychological foundations of culture (pp.
171-199). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Arrow, H., & Crosson, S. B. (2003). Musical chairs: Membership dynamics in self-organized
group formation. Small Group Research, 5, 523-556.

Arrow, H., & McGrath, J. E. (1995). Membership dynamics in groups at work: A theoretical
framework. Research in Organizational Behavior, 17, 373-411.

Arrow, H., McGrath, J. E., & Berdahl, J. L. (2000). Small groups as complex systems. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.

Atkinson, J. W. (1954). Explorations using imaginative thought to assess the strength of human
motives. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: 1954 (pp. 56-112).
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Austin, J. R. (2003). Transactive memory in organizational groups: The effects of context,


consensus, specialization, and accuracy on group performance. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 88, 866-878.

Baldwin, T. T., Bedell, M. D., & Johnson, J. L. (1997). The social fabric of a team-based M.B.A.
program: Network effects on student satisfaction and performance. Academy of
Management Journal, 40, 1369-1397.

Bales, R. F. (1950). Interaction process analysis: A method for the study of small groups.
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Bales, R. F. (1999). Social interaction systems: Theory and measurement. New Brunswick, NJ:
Transaction Publishers.

Bales, R. F., & Cohen, S. P. (1979). SYMLOG: A system for the multilevel observation of groups.
New York: Free Press.

Bales, R. F., & Strodtbeck, F. L. (1951). Phases in group problem-solving. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 46, 485-495.

Ballard, D. I., Tschan, F., & Waller, M. J. (2007). All in the timing: Considering time at multiple
stages of group research. Small Group Research, 39, 328-351.

Bandura, A. (2000). Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 9, 75-78.
- 58 -

Barker, J. R. (1993). Tightening the iron cage: Concertive control in self-managing teams.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 408-437.

Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.

Baron, R. S. (2005). So right it‘s wrong: Groupthink and the ubiquitous nature of polarized
group decision making. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (Vol. 37, pp. 219-253). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Baron, R. S., Kerr, N. L., & Miller, N. (1992). Group process, group decision, group action.
Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.

Barsade, S. G. (2002). The ripple effect: Emotional contagion and its influence on group
behavior. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47, 644-675.

Bass, B. M. (Ed.). (1990). Bass and Stogdill’s handbook of leadership (3rd ed.). New York: Free
Press.

Baumann, M. R., & Bonner, B. L. (2004). The effects of variability and expectations on
utilization of member expertise and group performance. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 93, 89-101.

Bavelas, A. (1950). Communication patterns in task-oriented groups. Journal of the Acoustical


Society of America, 22, 725-730.

Beal, D. J., Cohen, R. R., Burke, M. J., & McLendon, C. L. (2003). Cohesion and performance
in groups: A meta-analytic clarification of construct relations. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 88, 989-1004.

Beersma, B., Hollenbeck, J. R., Humphrey, S. E., Moon, H., Conlon, D. E., & Ilgen, D. R.
(2003). Cooperation, competition, and team performance: Toward a contingency
approach. Academy of Management Journal, 46, 572-590.

Behfar, K., Friedman, R. A., & Brett, J. M. (2008, November). The team negotiation challenge:
Defining and managing the internal challenges of negotiating teams. Paper presented at
the 21st Annual Conference of the International Association for Conflict Management,
Chicago.

Bell, B. S., & Kozlowski, S. W. J. (2002). A typology of virtual teams. Group and Organization
Management, 27, 14-49.

Benne, K. D. (1964). History of the T-Group in the laboratory setting. In L. P. Bradford, J. R.


Gibb, & K. D. Benne (Eds.), T-Group theory and laboratory method: Innovation in re-
education (pp. 136-167). New York: John Wiley and Sons.
- 59 -

Berdahl, J. L. (1998). The dynamics of composition and socialization in small groups: Insights
gained from developing a computational model. In D. H. Gruenfeld (Ed.), Research on
managing groups and teams (Vol. 1, pp. 209-227). Stamford, CT: JAI. Press.

Berg, D. N. (2005). Senior executive teams: Not what you think. Consulting Psychology Journal,
57, 107-117.

Bhappu, A. D., Griffith, T. L., & Northcraft, G. B. (1997). Media effects and communication
bias in diverse groups. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70, 19-
205.

Bion, W. R., & Rickman, J. (1943, November). Intra-group tensions in therapy. Lancet, 27, 678-
681.

Bion, W. R. (1952). Group dynamics: A re-view. International Journal of Psycho-analysis, 33,


235-247.

Bion, W. R. (1961). Experiences in groups. London: Tavistock.

Blount, S. (Ed.). (2004). Time in groups. Stamford, CT: JAI Press.

Bouas, K. S., & Arrow, H. (1996). The development of group identity in computer and face-to-
face groups with membership change. Computer Supported Cooperative Work, 4, 153-
178.

Brooks, F. P., Jr. (1995). The mythical man-month (2nd ed.). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Brown, R. (1965). Social psychology. New York: Free Press. (Second edition, 1986).

Bunderson, J. S. (2003). Recognizing and utilizing expertise in work groups: A status


characteristics approach. Administrative Science Quarterly, 48, 557-591.

Bunderson, J. S., & Boumgarden, P. (in press). Structure and learning in self-managed teams:
Why ―bureaucratic‖ teams can be better learners. Organization Science.

Bunderson, J. S., & Sutcliffe, K. M. (2003). Management team learning orientation and business
unit performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 552-560.

Bunker, B. B., & Alban, B. T. (Eds.). (2006). The handbook of large group methods: Creating
systemic change in organizations and communities. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Burgoon, J. K. (1983). Spatial relationships in small groups. In R. S. Cathcart & L. A. Samovar


(Eds.), Small group communication (pp. 276-292). Dubuque, IA: Brown.

Butterworth, T. (1990). Detroit String Quartet. In J. R. Hackman (Ed.), Groups that work (and
those that don’t) (pp. 207-224). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.
- 60 -

Cannon-Bowers, J. A., Salas, E., & Converse, S. (1993). Shared mental models in expert team
decision making. In N. J. Castellan (Ed.), Individual and group decision making (pp. 221-
246). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Cartwright, D., & Zander, A. F. (1953). Group dynamics: Research and theory. New York:
Harper (second edition: 1960; third edition: 1968).

Caruso, H. M., & Woolley, A. W. (2008). Harnessing the power of emergent interdependence to
promote diverse team collaboration. In K. W. Phillips (Ed.), Diversity and groups (pp.
245-266). Stamford, CT: JAI Press.

Chang, A., & Bordia, P. (2001). A multidimensional approach to the group cohesion-group
performance relationships. Small Group Research, 32, 379-405.

Chapman, J. A., Polzer, J. T., Barsade, S. G., & Neale, M. A. (1998). Being different yet feeling
similar: The influence of demographic composition and organizational culture on work
processes and outcomes. Administrative Science Quarterly, 43, 749-780.

Chen, G., & Kanfer, R. (2006). Toward a system theory of motivated behavior in work teams.
Research in Organizational Behavior, 27, 223-267.

Cheney, G. (1999). Values at work. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

Clark, C. C., & Sline, R. W. (2003). Teaming with emotion: The impact of emotionality on
work-team collaboration. In R. Y. Hirokawa, R. S. Cathcart, L. A. Samovar, & L. D.
Henman (Eds.), Small group communication: Theory and practice (8th ed., pp. 158-168).
Los Angeles: Roxbury.

Cline, R. J. W. (1999). Communication in social support groups. In L. R. Frey (Ed.), The


handbook of group communication theory and research (pp. 516-538). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.

Cohen, M. (1999). Commentary on the Organization Science special issue on complexity.


Organization Science, 10, 373-376.

Cohen, M., & Bacdayan, P. (1994). Organizational routines are stored as procedural memory:
Evidence from a laboratory study. Organization Science, 5, 554-568.

Cohen, S. G., Ledford, G. E., & Spreitzer, G. M. (1996). A predictive model of self-managing
work team effectiveness. Human Relations, 49, 643-676.

Collins, B. E., & Guetzkow, H. E. (1964). The social psychology of group processes for
decision-making. New York: Wiley.
- 61 -

Cornes, R., & Sandler, T. (1996). The theory of externalities, public goods, and club goods (2nd
ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Cramton C. D., & Orvis, K. L. (2003). Overcoming barriers to information sharing in virtual
teams. In C. B. Gibson & S. G. Cohen (Eds.), Virtual teams that work: Creating
conditions for virtual team effectiveness (pp. 214-230). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Cummings, J. N. (2007). Leading groups from a distance. In S. P. Weisband (Ed.), Leadership at


a distance: Research in technologically supported work (pp. 33-50). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.

Cummings, J. N., & Kiesler, S. (2005). Collaborative research across disciplinary boundaries.
Social Studies of Science, 35, 703-722.

Cummings, T. G. (1978). Self-regulating work groups: A socio-technical synthesis. Academy of


Management Review, 3, 625-634.

Dahlin, K. B., Weingart, L. R., & Hinds, P. J. (2005). Team diversity and information use.
Academy of Management Journal, 48, 1107-1123.

Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and
interracial interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 62-68.

Davis, J. H. (1969). Group performance. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Davis, J. H. (1973). Group decision and social interaction: A theory of social decision schemes.
Psychological Review, 80, 97-125.

Davis-Sacks, M. L. (1990). Credit analysis team. In J. R. Hackman (Ed.), Groups that work (and
those that don't) (pp. 126-145). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Day, D. V., Gronn, P., & Salas, E. (2004). Leadership capacity in teams. Leadership Quarterly,
15, 857-880.

DeMeyer, A. (1991, Spring). Tech talk: How managers are stimulating global R&D
communication. Sloan Management Review, 32, 49-59.

Denison, D. R., & Sutton, R. I. (1990). Operating room nurses. In J. R. Hackman (Ed.), Groups
that work (and those that don’t) (pp. 293-308). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

De Souza, G., & Klein, H. J. (1995). Emergent leadership in the group goal-setting process.
Small Group Research, 26, 475–496.

Dess, G. G., Rasheed, A. M. A., McLaughlin, K. J., & Priem, R. L. (1995, August). The new
corporate architecture. Academy of Management Executive, 1-11.
- 62 -

Druskat, V. U., & Kayes, D. C. (2000). Learning versus performance in short-term project teams.
Small Group Research, 31, 328-353.

Dubrovsky, V. J., Kiesler, S., & Sethna, B. N. (1991). The equalization phenomenon: Status
effects in computer-mediated and face-to-face decision making groups. Human Computer
Interaction, 6, 119-146.

Dugosh, K. L., & Paulus, P. B. (2005). Cognitive and social comparison processes in
brainstorming. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 313-320.

Edmondson, A. C. (1996). Learning from mistakes is easier said than done: Group and
organizational influences on the detection and correction of human error. Journal of
Applied Behavioral Sciences, 32, 5-32.

Edmondson, A. C. (1999). A safe harbor: Social psychological conditions enabling boundary


spanning in work teams. In R. Wageman (Ed.), Groups in context (pp. 179-199).
Stamford, CT: JAI Press. (a)

Edmondson, A. C. (1999). Psychological safety and learning behavior in work teams.


Administrative Science Quarterly, 44, 350-383. (b)

Edmondson, A. C. (2002). The local and variegated nature of learning in organizations: A group
level perspective. Organization Science, 13, 128-146.

Edmondson, A. C. (2003). Speaking up in the operating room: How team leaders promote
learning in interdisciplinary action teams. Journal of Management Studies, 40, 1419-
1452.

Edmondson, A. C., Roberto, M. A., & Watkins, M. D. (2003). A dynamic model of top
management team effectiveness: Managing unstructured task streams. Leadership
Quarterly, 14, 297-325.

Ellis, A. P., Hollenbeck, J. R., Ilgen, D. R., Porter, C. O., & West, B. (2003). Team learning:
Collectively connecting the dots. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 821-832.

Ensley, M. D., & Pearce, C. L. (2001). Shared cognition in top management teams: Implications
for new venture performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22, 145-160.

Eriksen, J., & Dyer, L. (2004). Right from the start: Exploring the effects of early team events on
subsequent project team development and performance. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 49, 438-471.

Esser, J. K., & Ahlfinger, N. R. (2001). Testing the groupthink model: Effects of promotional
leadership and conformity predisposition. Social Behavior and Personality, 29, 31-42.
- 63 -

Faraj, S., & Sproull, L. (2000). Coordinating expertise in software development teams.
Management Science, 46, 1554-1568.

Farris, G. F., & Lim, F. G., Jr. (1969). Effects of performance on leadership, cohesiveness,
influence, satisfaction, and subsequent performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53,
490-497.

Feinberg, J. M., & Aiello, J. R. (2006). Social facilitation: A test of competing theories. Journal
of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 1087-1109.

Felps, W., Mitchell, T. R., & Byington, E. (2006). How, when, and why bad apples spoil the
barrel: Negative group members and dysfunctional groups. Research in Organizational
Behavior, 27, 175-222.

Festinger, L. (1950). Informal social communication. Psychological Review, 57, 157-166.

Fiedler, F. E., & Garcia, J. E. (1987). New approaches to effective leadership: Cognitive
resources and organizational performance. New York: Wiley.

Forbes, D. P., & Milliken, F. J. (1999). Cognition and corporate governance: Understanding
boards of directors as strategic decision-making groups. Academy of Management
Review, 24, 489-505.

Forsyth, D. R. (2001). Therapeutic groups. In M. A. Hogg & R. S. Tindale (Eds.), Blackwell


handbook of social psychology: Group processes (pp. 628-659). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Forsyth, D. R., & Burnette, J. L. (2005). The history of group research. In S. A. Wheelan (Ed.),
The handbook of group research and practice (pp. 3-18). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Foushee, H. C., Lauber, J. K., Baetge, M. M., & Acomb, D. B. (1986). Crew factors in flight
operations: III. The operational significance of exposure to short-haul air transport
operations (Technical Memorandum No. 88342). Moffett Field, CA: NASA Ames
Research Center.

Freud, S. (1922/1959). Group psychology and the analysis of the ego. New York: Norton.

Frey, L. R. (Ed.). (1999). The handbook of group communication theory and research. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.

Frey, L. R. (Ed.). (2003). Group communication in context: Studies of bona fide groups (2nd ed.).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Fuhriman, A., & Burlingame, G. M. (1994). Measuring small group process: A methodological
application of chaos theory. Small Group Research, 25, 502-519.

Ganz, M., & Wageman, R. (2009). Leadership development in a civic organization: Multi-level
influences on effectiveness. Manuscript submitted for publication.
- 64 -

Gersick, C. J. G. (1988). Time and transition in work teams: Toward a new model of group
development. Academy of Management Journal, 31, 9-41.

Gersick, C. J. G. (1989). Marking time: Predictable transitions in task groups. Academy of


Management Journal, 31, 9-41.

Gersick, C. J. G. (1990). The students. In J. R. Hackman (Ed.), Groups that work (and those that
don’t) (pp. 89-111). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

Gersick, C. J. G., & Hackman, J. R. (1990). Habitual routines in task-performing teams.


Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 47, 65-97.

Gibb, C. A. (1954). Leadership. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp.
877-917). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Gibson, C., & Vermeulen, F. (2003). A healthy divide: Subgroups as a stimulus for team
learning behavior. Administrative Science Quarterly, 48, 202-224.

Gibson, C. B., & Cohen, S. G. (Eds.). (2003). Virtual teams that work: Creating conditions for
virtual team effectiveness. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Gibson, C. B., Waller, M. J., Carpenter, M. A., & Conte, J. M. (2007). Antecedents,
consequences, and moderators of time perspective heterogeneity for knowledge
management in MNO teams. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 28, 1005-1034.

Gigerenzer, G. (1999). Fast and frugal heuristics: The adaptive toolbox. In G. Gigerenzer & P.
M. Todd (Eds.), Simple heuristics that make us smart (pp. 3-34). New York: Oxford
University Press.

Gillette, J., & McCollom, M. (Eds.) (1990). Groups in context: A new perspective on group
dynamics. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Ginnett, R. C. (1993). Crews as groups: Their formation and their leadership. In E. L. Wiener, B.
G. Kanki, & R. L. Helmreich (Eds.), Cockpit resource management (pp. 71-98). Orlando,
FL: Academic Press.

Gist, M. E., & McDonald-Mann, D. (2000). Advances in leadership training and development. In
C. L. Cooper & E. A. Locke (Eds.), Industrial and organizational psychology: Linking
theory with practice (pp. 52-71). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Gladstein, D. L. (1984). Groups in context: A model of task group effectiveness. Administrative


Science Quarterly, 29, 499-518.

Gladwell, M. (2000, Dec. 11). Designs for working. The New Yorker, 60-70.
- 65 -

Goldstone, R. L., Roberts, M. E., & Gureckis, T. M. (2008). Emergent processes in group
behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 10-15.

Goodman, P. S., & Darr, E. D. (1996). Computer-aided systems for organizational learning.
Trends in Organizational Behavior, 3, 81-97.

Goodman, P. S., & Leyden, D. P. (1991). Familiarity and group productivity. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 76, 578-586.

Goodman, P. S., & Shah, S. (1992). Familiarity and work group outcomes. In S. Worchel, W.
Wood, & J. Simpson (Eds.), Group process and productivity (pp. 276-298). London:
Sage.

Graetz, K. A., Boyle, E. S., Kimble, C. E., Thompson, P., & Garloch, J. (1998). Information
sharing in face-to-face, teleconferencing, and electronic chat groups. Small Group
Research, 29, 714-743.

Groysberg, B., Healy, P., & Gui, Y. (2008). Can research committees add value for investors?
Journal of Financial Transformation, 24, 123-130.

Guastello, S. J. (2007). Non-linear dynamics and leadership emergence. Leadership Quarterly,


18, 357-369.

Guastello, S. J., & Guastello, D. D. (1998). Origins of coordination and team effectiveness: A
perspective from game theory and nonlinear dynamics. Journal of Applied Psychology,
83, 423-437.

Gruenfeld, D. H., & Hollingshead, A. B. (1993). Sociocognition in work groups: The evolution
of group integrative complexity and its relation to task performance. Small Group
Research, 24, 383-405.

Gruenfeld, D. H., Mannix, E. A., Williams, K. Y., & Neale, M. A. (1996). Group composition
and decision making: How member familiarity and information distribution affect
process and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67,
1-15.

Gruenfeld, D. H., Martorana, P. V., & Fan, E. T. (2000). What do groups learn from their
worldliest members? Direct and indirect influence in dynamic teams. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 82, 45-59.

Guetzkow, H., & Gyr, J. (1954). An analysis of conflict in decision-making groups. Human
Relations, 7, 367-382.

Guetzkow, H. & Simon, H. A. (1995). The impact of certain communication nets upon
organization and performance in task-oriented groups. Management Science, 1, 233-250.
- 66 -

Haas, M. R. (2006). Knowledge gathering, team capabilities, and project performance in


challenging work environments. Management Science, 52, 1170-1184.

Haas, M. R., & Hansen, M. T. (2007). Different knowledge, different benefits: Toward a
productivity perspective on knowledge sharing in organizations. Strategic Management
Journal, 28, 1133-1153.

Hackman, J. R. (1986). The psychology of self-management in organizations. In M. S. Pallack &


R. O. Perloff (Eds.), Psychology and work: Productivity, change, and employment (pp.
89-136). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Hackman, J. R. (Ed.). (1990). Groups that work (and those that don’t). San Francisco: Jossey-
Bass.

Hackman, J. R. (1992). Group influences on individuals in organizations. In M. D. Dunnette &


L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 3, pp.
1455-1525). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Hackman, J. R. (2002). Leading teams: Setting the stage for great performances. Boston:
Harvard Business School Press.

Hackman, J. R. (2003). Learning more from crossing levels: Evidence from airplanes, orchestras,
and hospitals. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24, 1-18.

Hackman, J. R., & Edmondson, A. C. (2008). Groups as agents of change. In T. Cummings


(Ed.), Handbook of organization development (pp. 167-186). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Hackman, J. R., & Morris, C. G. (1975). Group tasks, group interaction process, and group
performance effectiveness: A review and proposed integration. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 1-55). New York: Academic
Press.

Hackman, J. R., & Wageman, R. (2005). A theory of team coaching. Academy of Management
Review, 30, 269-287. (a)

Hackman, J. R., & Wageman, R. (2005). When and how team leaders matter. Research in
Organizational Behavior, 26, 37-74. (b)

Hackman, J. R., & Walton, R. E. (1986). Leading groups in organizations. In P. S. Goodman


(Ed.), Designing effective work groups (pp. 72-119). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Hackman, J. R., & Woolley, A. W. (in press). Creating and leading analytic teams. In R. L. Rees
& J. W. Harris (Eds.), A handbook of the psychology of intelligence analysis: The human
factor. Burlington, MA: Centra.
- 67 -

Hall, E. T. (1969). The hidden dimension (2nd ed). Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

Hamilton, D. L., Sherman, S. J., & Rodgers, J. S. (2004). Perceiving the groupness of groups:
Entitativity, homogeneity, essentialism, and stereotypes. In V. Yzerbyt, C. M. Judd, & O.
Corneille (Eds.), The psychology of group perception: Perceived variability, entitativity,
and essentialism (pp. 39-60). New York: Psychology Press.

Hambrick, D. C. (2007). Upper echelons theory: An update. Academy of Management Review,


32, 334-343.

Hapgood, F. (1994, August). Notes from the underground. The Atlantic Monthly, 34-38.

Hargadon, A. B. (1999). Group cognition and creativity in organizations. In R. Wageman (Ed.),


Groups in context (pp. 137-155). Stamford, CT: JAI Press.

Hare, A. P. (1992). Groups, teams, and social interaction: Theories and applications. New York:
Praeger.

Harkins, S. G., & Szymanski, K. (1989). Social loafing and group evaluation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 934-941.

Harrison, D. A., Mohammed, S., McGrath, J. E., Florey, A. T., & Vanderstoep, S. W. (2003).
Time matters in team performance: Effects of member familiarity, entrainment, and task
discontinuity on speed and quality. Personnel Psychology, 56, 633-669.

Hastie, R., Penrod, S. D., & Pennington, N. (1983). Inside the jury. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.

Heenan, D. A., & Bennis, W. (1999). Co-leaders: The power of great partnerships. New York:
Wiley.

Heifetz, R. A. (1998). Leadership without easy answers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.

Hiltz, S. R., Johnson, K., & Turoff, M. (1986). Experiments in group decision making:
Communication process and outcome in face-to-face versus computerized conferences.
Human Communication Research, 13, 225-252.

Hinsz, V. B., Tindale, R. S., & Vollrath, D. A. (1997). The emerging conceptualization of groups
as information processors. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 43-64.

Hogg, M. A. (1993). Group cohesiveness: A critical review and some new directions. European
Review of Social Psychology, 4, 85-111.

Hogg, M. A., & Tindale, R. S. (Eds.). (2001). Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Group
processes. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
- 68 -

Hollenbeck, G. P., McCall, M. W., Jr., & Silzer, R. F. (2006). Leadership competency models.
Leadership Quarterly, 17, 398-413.

Hollingshead, A. B. (1996). Information suppression and status persistence in group decision


making. Human Communication Research, 23, 193-219.

Hollingshead, A. B. (1998). Communication, learning and retrieval in transactive memory


systems. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 423-442.

Hollingshead, A. B., McGrath, J. E., & O‘Connor, K. M. (1993). Group task performance and
communication technology: A longitudinal study of computer-mediated versus face-to-
face work groups. Small Group Research, 24, 307-333.

Homan, A. C., van Knippenberg, D., Van Kleef, G. A., & De Dreu, K. W. (2007). Bridging
faultlines by valuing diversity: Diversity beliefs, information elaboration, and
performance in diverse work groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1189-1199.

Howell, S., Brock, B., & Hauser, E. (2003). A multicultural, intergenerational youth program:
Creating and sustaining a youth community group. In L. R. Frey (Ed.), Group
communication in context (2nd ed., pp. 85-107). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hutchins, E. (1995). How a cockpit remembers its speeds. Cognitive Science, 19, 265-288.

Ilgen, D. (1999). Teams imbedded in organizations: Some implications. American Psychologist,


54, 129-139.

Ilgen, D. R., Hollenbeck, J. R., Johnson, M., & Jundt, D. (2005). Teams in organizations: From
input-process-output models to IMOI models Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 517-543.

Isenberg, D. J. (1986). Group polarization: A critical review and meta-analysis. Journal of


Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1141-1151.

Jackson, J. (1966). A conceptual and measurement model for norms and roles. Pacific
Sociological Review, 9, 35-47.

James, L. R., Joyce, W. F., & Slocum, J. W. (1988). Organizations do not cognize. Academy of
Management Review, 13, 129-132.

Janis, I. L. (1972). Victims of groupthink. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Janis, I. L. (1982). Groupthink: Psychological studies of policy decisions and fiascoes (2nd ed.).
Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Jaques, D., & Salmon, G. (2007). Learning in groups: A handbook for face-to-face and online
environments. New York: Routledge.
- 69 -

Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1998). Cooperative learning and social interdependence
theory. In R. S. Tindale (Ed.), Theory and research on small groups (pp. 9-35). New
York: Plenum.

Joyce, A. S., Piper, W. E., & Ogrodniczuk, J. S. (2007). Therapeutic alliance and cohesion
variables as predictors of outcome in short-term group psychotherapy. International
Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 57, 269-297.

Kaplan, R. E. (1979). The utility of maintaining work relationships openly: An experimental


study. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 15, 41-59.

Karau, S. J., & Williams, K. D. (1993). Social loafing: A meta-analytic review and theoretical
integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 681-706.

Katz, N., Lazer, D., Arrow, H., & Contractor, N. (2004). Network theory and small groups.
Small Group Research, 35, 307-332.

Katz, N., Lazer, D., Arrow, H., & Contractor, N. (2005). The network perspective on small
groups: Theory and research. In M. S. Poole & A. B. Hollingshead (Eds.), Theories of
small groups: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 277-312). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Katz, R. (1982). The effects of group longevity on project communication and performance.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 27, 81-104.

Katzenbach, J. R., & Smith, D. K. (1993). The wisdom of teams. Boston: Harvard Business
School Press.

Kelly, J. R., & McGrath, J. E. (1985). Effects of time limits and task types on task performance
and interaction of four-person groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49,
395-407.

Kerr, N. L. (1981). Social transition schemes: Charting the group‘s road to agreement. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 684-702.

Kerr, N. L., MacCoun, R. J., & Kramer, G. P. (1996). Bias in judgment: Comparing individuals
and groups. Psychological Review, 103, 687-719.

Kerr, N. L., Niedermeier, K. E., & Kaplan, M. F. (1999). Bias in jurors vs. bias in juries: New
evidence from the SDS perspective. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 80, 70-86.

Kerr, N. L., & Tindale, R. S. (2004). Group performance and decision making. Annual Review of
Psychology, 55, 623-655.
- 70 -

Ketz de Vries, M. F. R., & Carlock, R. (2007). The family business on the couch: A
psychological perspective. West Sussex, UK: Wiley.

Kiesler, S., Siegel, J., & McGuire, T. W. (1984). Social psychological aspects of computer-
mediated communication. American Psychologist, 39, 1123-1134.

Kirkman, B. L., Rosen, B., Tesluk, P. E., & Gibson, C. B. (2004). The impact of team
empowerment on virtual team performance: The moderating role of face-to-face
interaction. Academy of Management Journal, 47, 1-18.

Kirkman, B. L., & Shapiro, D. L. (1997). The impact of cultural values on employee resistance
to teams: Toward a model of globalized self-managing work team effectiveness.
Academy of Management Review, 22, 730-757.

Klein, K. J., Ziegert, J. C., Knight, A. P., & Xiao, Y. (2006). Dynamic delegation: Shared,
hierarchical and deindividualized leadership in extreme action teams. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 51, 590-621.

Klimoski, R., & Mohammed, S. (1994). Team mental model: Construct or metaphor? Journal of
Management, 20, 403-437.

Kluckhohn, C., & Murray, H. A. (Eds.). (1953). Personality in nature, society, and culture. New
York: Knopf.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Bell, B. S. (2003). Work groups and teams in organizations. In W. C.
Borman, D. R. Ilgen, & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psychology
(Vol. 12): Industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 333-375). New York: Wiley.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Bell, B. S. (2008). Team learning, development, and adaptation. In V. I.
Sessa & M. London (Eds.), Group learning (pp. 15-44). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., Gully, S. M., McHugh, P. P., Salas, E., & Cannon-Bowers, J. A. (1996). A
dynamic theory of leadership and team effectiveness: Developmental and task contingent
leader roles. In G. R. Ferris (Ed.), Research in personnel and human resource
management (Vol. 14, pp. 253-305). Stamford, CT: JAI Press.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., Gully, S. M., Nason, E. R., & Smith, E. M. (1999). Developing adaptive
teams: A theory of compilation and performance across levels and time. In D. R. Ilgen &
E. D. Pulakos (Eds.), The changing nature of work performance: Implications for
staffing, personnel actions, and development (pp. 240-292). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Ilgen, D. R. (2006). Enhancing the effectiveness of work groups and
teams. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7, 77-124.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., Watola, D. J., Nowakowski, J. M., Kim, B. H., & Botero, I. C. (2009).
Developing adaptive teams: A theory of dynamic team leadership. In E. Salas, G. F.
- 71 -

Goodwinh, & C. S. Burke (Eds.), Team effectiveness in complex organizations: Cross-


disciplinary perspectives (pp. 113-156). New York: Psychology Press.

Kramer, R. M. (1998). Revisiting the Bay of Pigs and Vietnam decisions 25 years later: How
well has the groupthink hypothesis stood the test of time? Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 73, 236-271.

Lacey, R., & Gruenfeld, D. (1999). Unwrapping the work group: How extra-organizational
context affects group behavior. In R. Wageman (Ed.), Groups in context (pp. 157-177).
Stamford, CT: JAI Press.

Larson, J. R., & Christensen, C. (1993). Groups as problem-solving units: Toward a new
meaning of social cognition. British Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 5-30.

Latane, B., Williams, K. D., & Harkins, S. G. (1979). Many hands make light the work: The
causes and consequences of social loafing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
37, 823-832.

Lau, D., & Murnighan, J. K. (1998). Demographic diversity and faultlines: The compositional
diversity of organizational groups. Academy of Management Review, 23, 325-340.

Lau, D., & Murnighan, J. K. (2005). Interactions within groups and subgroups: The effects of
demographic faultlines. Academy of Management Journal, 48, 645-659.

Laughlin, P. R. (1980). Social combination processes of cooperative, problem-solving groups on


verbal intellective tasks. In M. Fishbein (Ed.), Progress in social psychology (pp. 127-
155). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Laughlin, P. R. (1999). Collective induction: Twelve postulates. Organizational Behavior and


Human Decision Processes, 80, 50-69.

Laughlin, P. R., Bonner, B. L., & Miner, A. G. (2002). Groups perform better than the best
individuals on Letters-to-Numbers problems. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 88, 605-620.

Laughlin, P. R., & Hollingshead, A. B. (1995). A theory of collective induction. Organizational


Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 61, 94-107.

Laughlin, P. R., VanderStoep, S. W., & Hollingshead, A. B. (1991). Collective versus individual
induction: Recognition of truth, rejection of error, and collective information processing.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 50-67.

Lawler, E. E. (2003). Pay systems for virtual teams. In C. B. Gibson & S. C. Cohen (Eds.),
Virtual teams that work: Creating conditions for virtual team effectiveness (pp. 121-144).
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
- 72 -

Lazer, D., Pentland, A., Adamic, L., Aral, S., Barabasi, A. L., Brewer, D., Christakis, N.,
Contractor, N., Fowler, J., Gutmann, M., Jebara, T., King, G., Macy, M., Roy, D., & Van
Alstyne, M. (2009). Computational social science. Science, 323, 721-723.

Leavitt, H. J. (1951). Some effects of certain communication patterns on group performance.


Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 46, 38-50.

Leavitt, H. J. (1975). Suppose we took groups seriously. . . In E. L. Cass & F. G. Zimmer (Eds.),
Man and work in society (pp. 67-77). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

Leavitt, H. J., & Mueller, R. A. (1951). Some effects of feedback on communication. Human
Relations, 4, 401-410.

Lebie, L., Rhoades, J. A., & McGrath, J. E. (1996). Interaction process in computer-mediated
and face-to-face groups. Computer Supported Cooperative Work, 4, 127-152.

Le Bon, G. (1895/1995). The crowd. Piscataway, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

Leedom, D. K., & Simon, R. (1995). Improving team coordination: A case for behavior-based
training. Military Psychology, 7, 109-122.

Levine, J. M., & Moreland, R. L. (1998). Small groups. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G.
Lindsey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 415-469). New
York: McGraw-Hill.

Levine, J. M., Moreland, R. L., & Choi, H. S. (2001). Group socialization and newcomer
innovation. In M. A. Hogg & R. S. Tindale (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social
psychology: Group processes (pp. 86-106). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Lewin, K. (1945). The Research Center for Group Dynamics at Massachusetts Institute of
Technology. Sociometry, 8, 126-136.

Lewin, K. (1946/1948). Action research and minority problems. In G. W. Lewin (Ed.), Resolving
social conflicts: Selected papers on group dynamics (pp. 201-216). New York: Harper &
Row.

Lewin, K. (1947). Frontiers in group dynamics, I. Human Relations, 1, 2-38. (a)

Lewin, K. (1947). Frontiers in group dynamics, II. Human Relations, 1, 143-153. (b)

Lewin, K., & Lippitt, R. (1938). An experimental approach to the study of autocracy and
democracy: A preliminary note. Sociometry, 1, 292-300.

Lewin, K., Lippitt, R., & White, R. K. (1939). Patterns of aggressive behavior in experimentally
created ‗social climates.‘ Journal of Social Psychology, 10, 271-299.
- 73 -

Lewin, M. A. (1992). The impact of Kurt Lewin‘s life on the place of social issues in his work.
Journal of Social Issues, 48, 15-30.

Lewis, K. (2004). Knowledge and performance in knowledge-worker teams: A longitudinal


study of transactive memory systems. Management Science, 50, 1519-1533.

Lewis, K., Belliveau, M., Herndon, B., & Keller, J. (2007). Group cognition, membership
change, and performance: Investigating the benefits and detriments of collective
knowledge. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103, 159-178.

Lipman-Blumen, J., & Leavitt, H. J. (1999). Hot groups: Seeding them, feeding them, and using
them to ignite your organization. New York: Oxford University Press.

Litchfield, R. C. (2008). Brainstorming reconsidered: A goal-based view. Academy of


Management Review, 33, 649-668.

Littlepage, G., Robison, W., & Kelly, R. (1997). Effects of task experience and group experience
on group performance, member ability, and recognition of expertise. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69, 133-147.

Locke, E. A., Tirnauer, D., Roberson, Q., Goldman, B., Latham, M. E., & Weldon, E. (2001).
The importance of the individual in an age of groupism. In M. E. Turner (Ed.), Groups at
work: Theory and research (pp. 501-528). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Lott, A. J., & Lott, B. E. (1965). Group cohesiveness as interpersonal attraction: A review of
relationships with antecedent and consequent variables. Psychological Bulletin, 64, 259-
309.

Lowin, B., & Craig, J. R. (1968). The influence of level of performance on managerial style: An
experimental object-lesson in the ambiguity of correlational data. Organizational
Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 440-458.

Malhotra, A., Majchrzak, A., Carman, R., & Lott, V. (2001). Radical innovation without co-
location: A case study at Boeing-Rocketdyne. MIS Quarterly, 25, 229-249.

Mann, R. D. (1959). A review of the relationships between personality and performance in small
groups. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 241-270.

Mannix, E., & Neale, M. A. (2005). What difference does a difference make? The promise and
reality of diverse teams in organizations. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 6,
31-55.

Manz, C., & Sims, H. P., Jr. (1987). Leading workers to lead themselves: The external leadership
of self-managing work teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 32, 106-129.
- 74 -

Marineau, R. (1989). Jacob Levy Moreno, 1889-1974: Father of psychodrama, sociometry, and
group psychotherapy. New York: Routledge.

Marquart, D. I. (1955). Group problem solving. Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 103-113.

Mas, A., & Moretti, E. (in press). Peers at work. American Economic Review.

Martins, L. L., Gilson, L. L., & Maynard, M. T. (2004). Virtual teams: What do we know and
where do we go from here? Journal of Management, 30, 805-835.

Mathews, K. M., White, M. C., & Long, R. G. (1999). Why study complexity in the social
sciences? Human Relations, 52, 439-462.

Mathieu, J. E., Heffner, T. S., Goodwin, G. F., Salas, E., & Cannon-Bowers, J. A. (2000). The
influence of shared mental models on team process and performance. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 85, 273-283.

Mathieu, J. E., Maynard, M. T., Rapp, T., & Gilson, L. (2008). Team effectiveness 1997-2007: A
review of recent advancements and a glimpse into the future. Journal of Management, 34,
410-476.

Maznevski, M. L., & Chudoba, K. M. (2001). Bridging space over time: Global virtual team
dynamics and effectiveness. Organization Science, 11, 473-492.

McCauley, C. (1998). Group dynamics in Janis‘s theory of groupthink: Backward and forward.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 73, 142-162.

McClelland, D. C. (1973). Testing for competence rather than intelligence. American


Psychologist, 28, 1-14.

McClure, B. A. (1998). Putting a new spin on groups: The science of chaos. Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.

McDonough, E. F., Kahn, K. B., & Barczak, G. (2001). An investigation of the use of global,
virtual, and co-located new product development teams. Journal of Product Innovation
Management, 18, 110-120.

McDougall, W. (1920). The group mind: A sketch of the principles of collective psychology with
some attempt to apply them to the interpretation of national life and character. New
York: Putnam.

McElvey, B. (1999). Complexity theory in organization science: Seizing the promise or


becoming a fad? Emergence, 1, 5-32.

McGrath, J. E. (1962). Leadership behavior: Some requirements for leadership training.


Washington, DC: U.S. Civil Service Commission.
- 75 -

McGrath, J. E. (1984). Groups: Interaction and performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
Hall.

McGrath, J. E. (1993). The JEMCO Workshop: Description of a longitudinal study. Small Group
Research, 24, 147-174.

McGrath, J. E. (1997). Small group research, that once and future field: An interpretation of the
past with an eye to the future. Group Dynamics, 1, 7-27.

McGrath, J. E., Arrow, H., & Berdahl, J. (2000). The study of groups: Past, present, and future.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 95-105.

McGrath, J. E., & Altman, I. (1966). Small group research. New York: Holt.

McGrath, J. E., & Kelly, J. R. (1986). Time and social interaction. New York: Guilford.

McGrath, J. E., & Tschan, F. (2004). Temporal matters in social psychology: Examining the role
of time in the lives of groups and individuals. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.

McGuire, W. J. (1969). The nature of attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson
(Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 136-314). Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.

McLeod, P. L., Baron, R. S., Marti, M. W., & Yoon, K. (1997). The eyes have it: Minority
influence in face-to-face and computer-mediated groups. Journal of Applied Psychology,
82, 706-718.

McLeod, P. L., & Kettner-Polley, R. (2005). Psychodynamic perspectives on small groups. In


M. S. Poole & A. B. Hollingshead (Eds.), Theories of small groups: Interdisciplinary
perspectives (p. 99-138). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Mehra, A., Dixon, A. L., Brass, D. J., & Robertson, B. (2006). The social network ties of group
leaders: Implications for group performance and leader reputation. Organization Science,
17, 64-79.

Meindl, J. R. (1990). On leadership: An alternative to conventional wisdom. Research in


Organizational Behavior, 12, 159-203.

Mohrman, S. A., Cohen, S. G., & Mohrman, A. M. (1995). Designing team-based organizations:
New forms for knowledge work. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Monge, P. R., & Contractor, N. (2003). Theories of communication networks. New York: Oxford
University Press.
- 76 -

Moreland, R. L. (1999). Transactive memory: Learning who knows what in work groups and
organizations. In L. L. Thompson, J. M. Levine, & D. M. Messick (Eds.), Shared
cognition in organizations: The management of knowledge (pp. 3-31). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.

Moreland, R. L., & Argote, L. (2003). Transactive memory in dynamic organizations. In R. S.


Peterson & E. A. Mannix (Eds.), Leading and managing people in the dynamic
organization (pp. 135-162). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Moreland, R. L., Hogg, M. A., & Hains, S. C. (1994). Back to the future: Social psychological
research on groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 527-555.

Moreland, R. L., Levine, J. M., & Wingert, M. L. (1996). Creating the ideal group: Composition
effects at work. In E. H. Witte & J. H. Davis (Eds.), Understanding group behavior (Vol.
2, pp. 11-35). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Moreland, R. L., & Myaskovsky, L. (2000). Exploring the performance benefits of group
training: Transactive memory or improved communication. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 82, 117-133.

Moreno, J. L. (1934). Who shall survive? A new approach to the problem of human
interrelations. Washington, DC: Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing Company.

Moreno, J. L. (1941). Foundations of sociometry: An introduction. Sociometry, 4, 15-35.

Moreno, J. L. (1943). Sociometry and the cultural order. Sociometry, 6, 299-344.

Mortensen, M. (2009). What is a team? The changing characteristics of teams and their effects
on individual and team outcomes. Working Paper, Sloan School of Management,
Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

Moynihan, L. M., & Peterson, R. S. (2001). A contingent configuration approach to the role of
personality in organizational groups. Research in Organizational Behavior, 23, 327-378.

Mullen, B., & Copper, C. (1994). The relation between group cohesiveness and performance: An
integration. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 210-227.

Murnighan, J. K., & Conlon, D. E. (1991). The dynamics of intense work groups: A study of
British string quartets. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 165-186.

National Transportation Safety Board. (1994). A review of flightcrew-involved major accidents


of U.S. air carriers, 1978 through 1990. Washington, DC: Author.

Neck, C. P., & Moorhead, G. (1992). Jury deliberations in the trial of U. S. v. John DeLorean: A
case analysis of groupthink avoidance and an enhanced framework. Human Relations, 45,
1077-1091.
- 77 -

Nehnevajsa, J. (1955). Sociometry: Decades of growth. Sociometry, 4, 48-95.

Nemiro, J., Beyerlein, M. M., Bradley, L., & Beyerlein, S. (Eds.). (2008). Handbook of high
performance virtual teams: A toolkit for collaborating across boundaries. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.

Neubert, M. J. (1999). Too much of a good thing or the more the merrier? Exploring the
dispersion and gender composition of informal leadership in manufacturing teams. Small
Group Research, 30, 635–646.

Nijstad, B. A., & Stroebe, W. (2006). How the group affects the mind: A cognitive model of idea
generation in groups. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 186-213.

Nohria, N., & Khurana, R. (Eds.). (in press). Advancing leadership. Boston: Harvard Business
School Press.

Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). The Implicit Association Test at age 7:
A methodological and conceptual review. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Automatic processes in
social thinking and behavior (pp. 265–292). New York: Psychology Press.

Noveck, B. S. (2009). Wiki government: How technology can make government better,
democracy stronger, and citizens more powerful. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.

Nye, J. S. (2008). The powers to lead. New York: Oxford University Press.

Offermann, L. R., & Spiros, R. K. (2001). The science and practice of team development.
Academy of Management Journal, 44, 376-392.

Oh, H., Labianca, G., & Chung, M. H. (2006). A multilevel model of group social capital.
Academy of Management Review, 31, 569-582.

Okhuysen, G. A., & Eisenhardt, K. M. (2002). Integrating knowledge in groups: How formal
interventions enable flexibility. Organization Science, 13, 370-386.

Okhuysen, G. A., & Waller, M. J. (2002). Focusing on midpoint transitions: An analysis of


boundary conditions. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 1056-1065.

O‘Leary, M. B., & Cummings, J. N. (2007). The spatial, temporal, and configurational
characteristics of geographic dispersion in teams. MIS Quarterly, 31, 433-452.

Orlikowski, W. J. (2000). Using technology and constituting structures: A practice lens for
studying technology in organizations. Organization Science, 11, 404-428.

Parks, C. D., & Sanna, L. J. (1999). Group performance and interaction. Boulder, CO: Westview
Press.
- 78 -

Paulus, P. B., Dugosh, K. L., Dzindolet, M. T., Coskun, H., & Putman, V. L. (2002). Social and
cognitive influences in group brainstorming: Predicting production gains and losses.
European Review of Social Psychology, 12, 299-325.

Pearce, C. A., & Conger, J. A. (Eds.). (2003). Shared leadership: Reframing the hows and whys
of leadership. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Peltokorpi, V. (2008). Transactive memory systems. Review of General Psychology, 12, 378-
394.

Penrod, S. D., & Hastie, R. (1981). A computer simulation of jury decision making.
Psychological Review, 87, 133-159.

Perlow, L. A., Gittell, J. H., & Katz, N. (2004). Contextualizing patterns of work group
interaction: Toward a nested theory of structuration. Organization Science, 15, 520-536.

Pescosolido, A. T. (2001). Informal leaders and the development of group efficacy. Small Group
Research, 32, 74–93.

Pescosolido, A. T. (2002). Emergent leaders as managers of group emotion. Leadership


Quarterly, 18, 1-18.

Peters, M., & Robinson, V. (1984). The origins and status of action research. Journal of Applied
Behavioral Sciences, 20, 113-124.

Phillips, K. W. (Ed.). (2008). Diversity and groups. Stamford, CT: JAI Press.

Polzer, J. T., Crisp, C. B., Jarvenpaa, S. L., & Kim, J. W. (2006). Extending the faultline model
to geographically dispersed teams: How collocated subgroups can impair group
functioning. Academy of Management Review, 49, 679-692.

Poole, M. S., & Hollingshead, A. B. (2005). Theories of small groups: Interdisciplinary


perspectives. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Poole, M. S., Seibold, D. R., & McPhee, R. D. (1985). Group decision-making as a structuration
process. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 71, 74-102.

Postmes, T., Spears, R., & Cihangir, S. (2001). Quality of decision making and group norms.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 918-930.

Pritchard, R. D., & Watson, M. D. (1992). Understanding and measuring group productivity. In
S. Worchel, W. Wood, & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Group process and productivity (pp. 252-
265). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
- 79 -

Putnam, L. L., & Stohl, C. (1990). Bona fide groups: A reconceptualization of groups in context.
Communication Studies, 41, 248-265.

Quigley, N. R., Tesluk, P. E., Locke, E. A., & Bartol, K. M. (2007). A multilevel investigation of
the motivational mechanisms underlying knowledge sharing and performance.
Organization Science, 18, 71-88.

Raven, B. H. (1998). Groupthink, Bay of Pigs, and Watergate reconsidered. Organizational


Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 73, 352-361.

Reagans, R., & Zuckerman, E. W. (2001). Networks, diversity, and productivity: The social
capital of corporate R&D teams. Organization Science, 12, 502-517.

Robey, D., Khoo, H. M., & Powers, C. (2000). Situated learning in cross-functional virtual
teams. IEEE Transaactions on Professional Communications, 43, 51-66.

Rioch, M. J. (1975). The work of Wilfred Bion on groups. In A. D. Colman & W. H. Bexton
(Eds.), Group relations reader (pp. 21-33). Sausalito, CA: GREX.

Rosseau, D. M., & Fried, Y. (2001). Location, location, location: Contextualizing organizational
research. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22, 1-13.

Salas, E., Bowers, C. A., & Cannon-Bowers, J. A. (1995). Military team research: 10 years of
progress. Military Psychology, 7, 55-75.

Salas, E., Goodwin, G. F., & Burke, C. S. (Eds.). (2008). Team effectiveness in complex
organizations: Cross-disciplinary perspectives and approaches. Boca Raton, FL: CRC
Press.

Salas, E., Kosarzycki, M. P., Tannenbaum, S. I., & Carnegie, D. (2004). Principles and advice
for understanding and promoting effective teamwork in organizations. In R. J. Burke &
C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Leading in turbulent times: Managing in the new world of work (pp.
95-120). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Salas, E., Nichols, D. R., & Driskell, J. E. (2007). Testing three team training strategies in intact
teams: A meta-analysis. Small Group Research, 38, 471-488.

Salas, E., Stagl, K. C., & Burke, C. S. (2004). 25 years of team effectiveness in organizations:
Research themes and emerging needs. Review of Industrial and Organizational
Psychology, 19, 47-91.

Schachter, S. (1964). The interaction of cognitive and physiological determinants of emotional


state. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 49-
80). New York: Academic Press.
- 80 -

Schippers, M. C., den Hartog, D. N., & Koopman, P. L. (2008). Reflexivity in teams: A measure
and correlates. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 56, 189-211.

Scholten, L., van Knippenberg, D., Nijstad, B. A., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2007). Motivated
information processing and group decision-making: Effects of process accountability on
information processing and decision quality. Journal of Experimental Social
Psycyhology, 43, 539-552.

Schopler, J. H. (1987). Interorganizational groups: Origins, structure, and outcomes. Academy of


Management Review, 12, 702-713.

Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Goal imagery: Bridging the gap between implicit
motives and explicit goals. Journal of Personality, 67, 1-34.

Schulz-Hardt, S., Brodbeck, F. C., Mojzisch, A., Kerschreiter, R., & Frey, D. (2006). Group
decision making in hidden profile situations: Dissent as a facilitator for decision quality.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1080-1093.

Seeger, J. A. (1983). No innate phases in group problem solving. Academy of Management


Review, 8, 683-689.

Senge, P. M. (1990). The fifth discipline: The art and practice of the learning organization. New
York: Doubleday.

Sessa, V. I., & London, M. (Eds.). (2008). Group learning. Mahwah, NH: Erlbaum.

Shaw, M. E. (1932). A comparison of individuals and small groups in the rational solution of
complex problems. American Journal of Psychology, 44, 491-504.

Shepherd, M. M., Briggs, R. O., Reinig, B. A., Yen, J., & Nunamaker, J. F. (1996). Invoking
social comparison to improve electronic brainstorming: Beyond anonymity. Journal of
Management Information Systems, 12, 155-170.

Sherblom, J. C. (2003). Influences on the recommendations of international business consulting


teams. In L. R. Frey (Ed.), Group communication in context (2nd ed., pp. 263-290).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Shiflett, S. C. (1979). Toward a general model of small group productivity. Psychological


Bulletin, 86, 67-79.

Shirky, C. (2008). Here comes everybody: The power of organizing without organizations. New
York: Penguin.

Shore, M. (2008). Personal communication (manuscript in preparation).


- 81 -

Siegel, J., Dubrovsky, V., Kiesler, S., & McGuire, T. W. (1986). Group processes in computer-
mediated communication. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 37,
157-187.

Simon, H. A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 1-19.

Slavin, R. E. (1980). Cooperative learning in teams: State of the art. Educational Psychologist,
15, 93-111.

Smith, K. K., & Berg, D. N. (1987). Paradoxes of group life. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Smith, J. A., & Foti, R. J. (1998). A pattern approach to the study of leader emergence.
Leadership Quarterly, 9, 147–160.

Smith-Jentsch, K. A., Cannon-Bowers, J. A., Tannenbaum, S. I., & Salas, E. (2008). Guided
team self-correction: Impacts on team mental models, processes, and effectiveness. Small
Group Research, 39, 303-327.

Sparrowe, R. T., Liden, R. C., Wayne, S. J., & Kraimer, M. L. (2001). Social networks and the
performance of individuals and groups. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 316-325.

Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1986). Reducing social context cues: Electronic mail in organizational
communication. Management Science, 32, 1492-1512.

Stasser, G., & Davis, J. H. (1981). Group decision making and social influence: A social
interaction sequence model. Psychological Review, 88, 523-551.

Stasser, G., & Dietz-Uhler, B. (2001). Collective choice, judgment, and problem solving. In M.
A. Hogg & R. S. Tindale (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Group
processes (pp. 31-55). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Stasser, G., Kerr, N. L., & Davis, J. H. (1989). Influence processes and consensus models in
decision-making groups. In P. B. Paulus (Ed.), Psychology of group influence (pp 431 -
477). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Stasser, G., & Titus, W. (1985). Pooling of unshared information in group decision-making:
Biased information sampling during discussion. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 48, 1467-1478.

Stasser, G., & Titus, W. (2003). Hidden profiles: A brief history. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 304-
313.

Stasser, G., Stewart, D. D., & Wittenbaum, G. M. (1995). Expert roles and information exchange
during discussion: The importance of knowing who knows what. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 31, 244-265.
- 82 -

Steele, F. I. (1973). Physical settings and organizational development. Reading, MA: Addison-
Wesley.

Steiner, I. D. (1966). Models for inferring relationships between group size and potential
productivity. Behavioral Science, 11, 273-283.

Steiner, I. D. (1972). Group process and productivity. New York: Academic Press.

Steiner, I. D. (1974). Whatever happened to the group in social psychology. Journal of


Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 94-108.

Steiner, I. D. (1986). Paradigms and groups. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental


Social Psychology, 19, 251-289.

Stewart, G. L., & Barrick, M. R. (2000). Team structure and performance: Assessing the
mediating role of intrateam process and the moderating role of task type. Academy of
Management Journal, 43, 135-148.

Stohl, C. & Walker, K. (2002). A bona fide perspective for the future of groups: Understanding
collaborating groups. In L. R. Frey (Ed.), New directions for group communication (pp.
237-252). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Straus, S. G. (1996). Getting a clue: The effects of communication media and information
distribution on participation and performance in computer-mediated and face-to-face
groups. Small Group Research, 27, 115-142.

Straus, S. G., & McGrath, J. E. (1994). Does the medium matter? The interaction of task type
and technology on group performance and member reactions. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 79, 87-97.

Straus, S. G., Parker, A. M., Bruce, J. B., & Dembosky, J. W. (2008). The group matters: A
review of the effects of group interaction on processes and outcomes in analytic teams.
Arlington, VA: Rand Corporation (Report WR-580-USG).

Sundstrom, E. D. (Ed.). (1999). Supporting work team effectiveness. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Sutton, R. I., & Hargadon. A. B. (1996). Brainstorming groups in context: Effectiveness in a


product design firm. Administrative Science Quarterly, 41, 685-718.

Taggar, S., Hackett, R., & Saha, S. (1999). Leadership emergence in autonomous work teams:
Antecedents and outcomes. Personnel Psychology, 52, 899–926.

Tasa, K., Taggar, S., & Seijtz, G. H. (2007). The development of collective efficacy in teams: A
multilevel longitudinal perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 17-27.

Taylor, F. W. (1911). The principles of scientific management. New York: Harper.


- 83 -

Tetlock, P. E., Peterson, R. S., McGuire, C., Chang, S., & Feld, P. (1992). Assessing political
group dynamics: A test of the groupthink model. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 63, 403-425.

Thelen, H. A. (1981). The classroom society: The construction of educational experience. New
York: Wiley.

Thomas-Hunt, M. C., Ogden, T. Y., & Neale, M. A. (2003). Who's really sharing? Effects of
social and expert status on knowledge exchange within groups. Management Science, 49,
464-477.

Tindale, R. S., Nadler, J., Krebel, A., & Davis, J. H. (2001). Procedural mechanisms and jury
behavior. In M. A. Hogg & R. S. Tindale (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social
psychology: Group processes (pp. 574-602). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Townsend, A. M., DeMarie, S. M., & Hendrickson, A. R. (1998). Virtual teams: Technology and
the workplace of the future. Academy of Management Executive, 12, 17-29.

Tracy, K., & Standerfer, C. (2003). Selecting a school superintendent: Sensitivities in group
deliberation. In L. R. Frey (Ed.), Group communication in context (2nd ed., pp. 109-134).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Tuckman, B. W. (1965). Developmental sequence in small groups. Psychological Bulletin, 63,


384-399.

Turner, M. E., & Pratkanis, A. R. (1998). Twenty-five years of groupthink theory and research:
Lessons from the evaluation of a theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 73, 105-115.

Van den Bossche, P., Gijselaers, W. H., Segers, M., Kirschner, P. A. (2006). Social and
cognitive factors driving teamwork in collaborative learning environments: Team
learning beliefs and behaviors. Small Group Research, 37, 490-521.

Van der Vegt, G. S., & Bunderson, J. S. (2005). Learning and performance in multidisciplinary
teams: The importance of collective team identification. Academy of Management
Journal, 48, 532-547.

van Ginkel, W. P., & van Knippenberg, D. (2009). Knowledge about the distribution of
information and group decision making: When and why does it work? Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108, 218-229.

van Knippenberg, D., & Schippers, M. C. (2007). Work group diversity. Annual Review of
Psychology, 58, 515-541.
- 84 -

Vroom, V. H., & Jago, A. G. (1988). The new leadership: Managing participation in
organizations. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Wageman, R. (1995). Interdependence and group effectiveness. Administrative Science


Quarterly, 40, 145-180.

Wageman, R. (Ed.). (1999). Groups in context. Stamford, CT: JAI Press.

Wageman, R. (2001). How leaders foster self-managing team effectiveness: Design choices
versus hands-on coaching. Organization Science, 12, 559-577.

Wageman, R. (2003). Virtual process: Implications for coaching the virtual team. In E. A.
Mannix & R. Peterson (Eds.), Understanding the dynamic organization (pp. 65-86).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Wageman, R., & Baker, G. (1997) Incentives and cooperation: The joint effects of task and
reward interdependence on group performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18,
139-158.

Wageman, R., & Gordon, F. (2005). As the twig is bent: How group values shape emergent task
interdependence in groups. Organization Science, 16, 687-700.

Wageman, R., & Hackman, J. R. (in press). What makes teams of leaders leadable? In N. Nohria
& R. Khurana (Eds.), Advancing leadership. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.

Wageman, R., Nunes, D. A., Burruss, J. A., & Hackman, J. R. (2008). Senior leadership teams:
What it takes to make them great. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.

Wall, T. D., Kemp, N. J., Jackson, P. R., & Clegg, C. W. (1986). Outcomes of autonomous
work-groups: A long-term field experiment. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 280-
304.

Waller, M. J., Gupta, N., & Giambatista, R. C. (2004). Effects of adaptive behaviors and shared
mental models on control crew performance. Management Science, 50, 1534-1544.

Waller, M. J., Zellmer-Bruhn, M. E., & Giambatista, R. C. (2002). Watching the clock: Group
pacing behavior under dynamic deadlines. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 1046-
1055.

Walsh, J. P. (1995). Managerial and organizational cognition: Notes from a trip down memory
lane. Organization Science, 6, 280-321.

Walton, R. E. (1985). From control to commitment: Transforming workspace force management


in the United States. In K. B. Clark, R. H. Hayes, & C. Lorenz (Eds.), The uneasy
alliance: Managing the productivity-technology dilemma (pp. 237-265). Boston: Harvard
Business School Press.
- 85 -

Walton, R. E., & Hackman, J. R. (1986). Groups under contrasting management strategies. In P.
S. Goodman (Ed.), Designing effective work groups (pp. 168-201). San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.

Watson, W., Michaelsen, L. K., & Sharp, W. (1991). Member competence, group interaction,
and group decision making: A longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76,
803-809.

Wegner, D. M. (1986). Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. In B.


Mullen & G. R. Goethals (Eds.), Theories of group behavior (pp. 185-208). New York:
Springer-Verlag.

Wegner, D. M. (1995). A computer network model of human transactive memory. Social


Cognition, 13, 319-339.

Weick, K. E., & Roberts, K. H. (1993). Collective mind in organizations: Heedful interrelating
on flight decks. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 357-381.

Weisband, S. P. (1992). Group discussion and first advocacy effects in computer-mediated and
face-to-face decision making groups. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 53, 35-380.

Wheelan, S. A. (1996). An initial exploration of the relevance of complexity theory to group


research and practice. Systems Practice, 9, 47-70.

Wheelan, S. A. (Ed.). (2005). The handbook of group research and practice. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.

Whyte, G. (1998). Recasting Janis‘s groupthink model: The key role of collective efficacy in
decision fiascoes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 73, 185-209.

Williams, K. Y., & O‘Reilly, C. A., III. (1998). Demography and diversity in organizations: A
review of 40 years of research. Research in Organizational Behavior, 20, 77-140.

Wilson, J. M., Goodman, P. S., & Cronin, M. A. (2007). Group learning. Academy of
Management Review, 32, 1041-1059.

Wilson, J. M., O‘Leary M. B., Metiu, A., & Jett, Q. R. (2008). Perceived proximity in virtual
work: Explaining the paradox of far-but-close. Organization Studies, 29, 979-1002.

Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20,
1-5.
- 86 -

Wittenbaum, G. M., Hollingshead, A. B., Paulus, P. B., Hirokawa, R. Y., Ancona, D. G.,
Peterson, R. S., Jehn, K. A., & Yoon, K. (2004). The functional perspective as a lens for
understanding groups. Small Group Research, 35, 17-43.

Wong, S. S. (2004). Distal and local group learning: Performance trade-offs and tensions.
Organization Science, 15, 645-656.

Wood, J. D. (1990). New Haven Nighthawks. In J. R. Hackman (Ed.), Groups that work (and
those that don’t) (pp. 265-279). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

Woolley, A. W. (1998). Effects of intervention content and timing on group task performance.
Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 34, 30-49.

Woolley, A. W., Gerbasi, M. E., Chabris, C. F., Kosslyn, S. M., & Hackman, J. R. (2008).
Bringing in the experts: How team composition and collaborative planning jointly shape
analytic effectiveness. Small Group Research, 39, 352-371.

Yalom, I. D. (1995). The theory and practice of group psychotherapy (4th ed). New York: Basic
Books.

Yalom, I. D., & Leszcz, M. (2005). Theory and practice of group psychotherapy (5th ed.). New
York: Basic Books.

Yukl, G. A. (2002). Leadership in organizations (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Zaccaro, S. J. (2007). Trait-based perspectives of leadership. American Psychologist, 62, 6-16.

Zaccaro, S. J., & Lowe, C. A. (1988). Cohesiveness and performance on an additive task:
Evidence for multidimensionality. Journal of Social Psychology, 128, 547-558.

Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269-274.

Zellmer-Bruhn, M. & Gibson, C. (2006). Multinational organizational context: Implications for


team learning and performance. Academy of Management Journal, 49, 501-518.

Zemore, S. E., Kaskutas, L. A., & Ammon, L. N. (2004). In 12-step groups, helping helps the
helper. Addiction, 99, 1015-1023.

Zenger, T. R., & Marshall, C. R. (2000). Determinants of incentive intensity in group-based


rewards. Academy of Management Journal, 43, 149-163.

Zigurs, I., Poole, M. S., & DeSanctis, G. L. (1988). A study of influence in computer-mediated
group decision making. MIS Quarterly, 12, 625-644.

View publication stats

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy