Colour Vision Requirements of Fire Fighters
Colour Vision Requirements of Fire Fighters
114-124, 1996
Copyright © 1996 Rapid Science Publishers for SOM
Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved
0962-7480/96
To perform their job safely firefighters must be able to identify colours on industrial
Figure 1. The CIE chromacity diagram and a sample of Figure 2. Isochromatic lines of dichromats. Each line represents
discrimination ellipses. Any colour may be specified by referencing the longer axis of the dichromats discrimination ellipse. Colours
its x, y co-ordinates. Colours that fall within an ellipse (shown represented by points along each line cannot be distinguished
enlarged approximately 10 times) appear to be the same to by dichromats, providing the luminance of each colour is the
people with normal colour vision same, (a) Isochromatic lines of protanopes; (b) Isochromatic
lines of deuteranopes.
0.9 -
520 nm
600 nm
<u
Table 1. Prevalence of inherited colour deficiency in men and in the paint, textile, food and printing industries.
women Mistakes made in the correct identification of precious
Type of deficiency % of men % of women stones, ripe fruit, the ink used in printing or in electrical
components may be costly. In other occupations the
Protanopia 1% 0.01%
Protanomalous trichromatism 0.03%
incorrect identification of colour may have human as
1%
Deuteranopia 1% 0.01%
well as financial consequences. This is particularly so
Deuteranomalous trichromatism 5% 0.35%
in transport industries. A more extensive list of
Total 8% 0.40% occupations that require good colour vision is provided
byVolk."
Obviously, it is important to exclude colour deficient
people from some occupations. However, individuals
dichromat. Dichromats confuse a wide range of colours
with defective colour vision are not a homogenous
including spectral hues; reds are confused with greens
group, whereas some colour deficient people have a
and purples are confused with blue-greens. Anomalous
very limited ability to distinguish colours others are
trichromats may only confuse desaturated, pale or dark
only slightly handicapped. There is a strong case that,
colours. People with red/green colour deficiency always
tests, i.e. examiner variability, lack of standardization diagram. Failure of the test occurs when colours from
and the use of colour naming. As we shall show, fire- opposite sides of the hue circle are placed together in
fighters must be able to distinguish between a plethora the arrangement. These errors represent isochromatic
of different colours in a number of different conditions. colour confusions equal to the separation of the colours
It is not possible to recreate the conditions encountered across the hue circle
on the fireground and therefore a trade test would be We concur with the views of others11'13 that clinical
an inappropriate and unjustifiable means of selecting tests alone provide a suitable means of selecting personnel.
firefighter recruits.12
Clinical tests have been designed to perform a variety
of tasks. These include: identifying people with defec- METHODS
tive colour vision, grading the severity of the defect
and/or classifying the defect as protan, deutan, or tritan. To determine suitable colour vision standards for the
Clinical tests that use surface colours are unable to fire service we used a novel objective technique. Briefly,
differentiate between people who are dichromatic and this involved the identification of all colour coded
those who have severe anomalous trichromatism. Al- information that a firefighter may reasonably expect
RESULTS
Portable fire extinguishers are colour coded according such as reflectance, shape and symbols may be suffi-
to their contents, e.g. a cream extinguisher contains cient for correct identification.
foam. Mistakes in colour identification may result in
the use of an inappropriate extinguisher, e.g. the use Object identification by use of non-colour clues
of a water fire extinguisher on an electrical fire. The
colours to be used in the code are described by Safety signs use colour redundantly, i.e. identification
BS5423. of colour is not necessary for the correct interpretation
Colour is used as the main means of identifying high of the sign. For example a 'no smoking' sign is
and low pressure airline hoses. Power, produced by comprised of a stylized picture of a smoking cigarette
compressor, is conveyed to a variety of cutting, spreading on a white background upon which is superimposed
and lifting equipment by these hoses. A representative a red circular band and cross bar. Although the red
sample of hoses have their colours quantified by the colour indicates that the activity is prohibited, identi-
colour matching technique described earlier. fication of the colour is not essential for correct
interpretation of the sign.
The red and amber lights used by road traffic signals
Table 2. Colours that firefighters encounter at work that appear the same to protanopes and deuteranopes
Table 3. Gas cylinder colours confused by dichromats. Although dichromats confuse several colours used to code the contents of
gas cylinders only a small proportion of gas cylinders are likely to be confused by dichromats. This is because they are able to use
non colour clues such as shape and reflectance to distinguish between cylinders whose colour appearance is the same. The first
column describes the colours that appear the same and the second column identifies cylinders that use these confusion colours.
The third column indicates which cylinders are likely to be confused having considered non colour clues.
Several colours used to code the contents of pipelines Figure 4. Shows that non colour clues can be used to
differentiate gas cylinders. Cylinders vary in shape (c, d
are confused by dichromats. However,firefightersusually and e) some have a valve protecting shroud (b) and others
identify such pipelines by the text and symbols that have different reflectance (a and c).
are also displayed on the pipeline (Personal commu-
nication: G. Pearson, Fire Experimental Unit, Morton
in Marsh, UK). Therefore dichromatic firefighters are
no more likely to confuse pipeline contents than their 0
colour normal colleagues.
Gas cylinders, both medical and industrial, primarily
have the colour coding displayed on the cylinder to
enable the identification of cylinder contents. Although
dichromats make numerous colour confusions, correct
identification of the cylinder may still be possible by
interpretation of non colour clues: text, shape and the
coloured rings found at the neck of some cylinders.
In a firefighting situation it is unlikely that a firefighter
would be able to get close enough to a cylinder to
read the text on the cylinder neck, therefore in this
analysis we disregarded text as an appropriate means
of identification. Each cylinder whose body colour may
be confused by dichromats was examined in order to
determine if correct identification could be made by
identifying non-colour clues such as the presence of a
valve protecting shroud or cylinder shape. Figure 4 shows
that gas cylinders may be identified by non-colour protanopes it is unlikely that they will be confused.
clues. This is because much more light is reflected from the
Colour confusion data, presented in Table 2, was cream extinguisher than the red one. In this example
used to identify potential cylinder confusions. Table 3 the cream extinguisher has a reflectance of 0.626 and
shows which confusions were considered in this analysis. the red one 0.144 therefore the cream colour appears
Dichromatic firefighters are unable to distinguish relatively light while the red one appears relatively
between cylinders containing propane (red) and methyl dark. Protanopes may also confuse green (halon) with
chloride (green), neither are they able to distinguish cream (foam) fire extinguishers. However, the chance
between argon (blue) and oxygen (black). Protanopes of such a confusion arising is again minimized by
may also confuse acetylene (maroon) with oxygen differences in reflectance (green 0.173 and cream 0.626).
(black). Although acetylene and oxygen cylinders are Table 2 shows that dichromats are also unable to
a different shape, the difference is not pronounced. distinguish between the colours of various high
Therefore, in some situations, their identity may be pressure hoses from different manufacturers. Although
confused, e.g. if the cylinders have fallen over. correct assembly of this equipment is ensured by the
Despite the fact that cream (foam) and red (water) use of special end connectors, which makes the colour
fire extinguishers have the same colour appearance to coding in many instances redundant, correct operation
T. H. Margrain et a/.: Colour vision requirements of firefighters 121
of some equipment relies upon recognition of hose mats. Hence many of the object confusions made by
colour e.g. hoses used with lift bags. These are dichromats will not be made by anomalous trichromats.
frequently used in pairs to move heavy objects, such In addition, many of the objects that dichromats identify
as vehicles, in a controlled manner so that trapped by non-colour clues appear to be of different colour
occupants may be rescued. Discussion with firefighters to anomalous trichromats. This additional information
reveals that each bag is identified by naming the colour may reduce the length of time it takes to correctly
of the appropriate hose, i.e. a firefighter calls the colour identify objects and possibly decrease the chance of
name to a colleague who controls hose pressure. incorrect identification. However, even with their
Even after non-colour clues have been taken into relatively superior colour vision, deuteranomalous and
consideration it appears that dichromats are still liable protanomalous people who pass the D15 test may still
to confuse several colour coded objects. be unable to distinguish between the blue-green and
black hoses supplied by Amkus. More seriously,
protanomalous individuals who pass the D15 test may
Identification of colours that will be confused confuse the maroon colour used on acetylene gas
by anomalous trichromats cylinders with the black used on oxygen ones. Unlike
Figure 5. Flow diagram showing recommended colour vision allow people who are unable to distinguish the colours
testing procedure of basic colour codes to become firefighters.
Firefighter
recruits ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Unsuitable Suitable
Prot- Deuter- for
for
anomalous firefighting REFERENCES
firefighting anomalous
work work
1. Fire Services: The Fire Services (Appointments and Promo-
tion) (Amendment) (No.3) Regulations. HMSO. SI. No. 2235,
ISBN 0-11-088235-0. 1988.
2. Godber G. Report of the committee to review the medical
fire service and should be rejected without further standards for the fire service. March, 1968.
testing. People who pass the D15 test have mildly 3. Sheedy JE. Fire fighter vision standards. JAmerican Optometric
defective colour vision and may be either protanomalous Association 1984; 55.5: 365-374.
4. Cole BL. Does defective colour vision really matter? In: Drum
or deuteranomalous. The Nagel anomaloscope should B, ed. Colour vision deficiencies XI. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic
be used to make this classification. Protanomalous Publishers, 1993: 67-86.
people are unsuitable for work as firefighters. The 5. Gledhill N, Jamnik VK. Development and validation of a fitness
recommended testing procedure is summarized as a screening protocol for firefighter applicants. Can J Spt Sci
flow diagram in Figure 5. 1992; 17.3: 99-206.
6. Birch J. Diagnosis of defective colour vision. New York: Oxford
University Press, 1993.
Previous investigations of the colour vision 7. Pitt FGH. Characteristics of Dichromatic Vision. Spec Rep XIV
Committee on Physiology of Vision. London: HMSO, 1935.
requirements of firefighters 8. Margrain TH, Owen CG, Woodward EG. Prevalence of spec-
Previous studies of the colour vision requirements of tacle and contact lens wear in the UK fire service. Ophthal and
firefighters concur with our findings that 'perfect' colour Physiol Opt 1995; at press.
9. Cole BL. The handicap of abnormal colour vision. Austr J
vision is unnecessary for the task of firefighting.3"5 In Optom 1972; 65: 135-140.
particular, Cole4 recommends that only people who 10. Christ RE. Review and analysis of colour coding research for
fail the D15 test and those who are diagnosed as being visual displays. Human Factors 1975; 17: 542-570.
protans should be excluded from the fire service. He 11. Voke J. Colour vision testing in specific industries and professions.
suggests that this standard is appropriate because it London: Keeler, 1980.
ensures that firefighters can see various colour codes, 12. Margrain T, Birch J. A suitable study to evaluate colour vision
requirements for firefighters? Occup Med 1994; 44: 257-258.
the colour of flames and are able correctly identify 13. Lakowski R. A critical evaluation of colour vision tests. Br J
road traffic signals. Physiol Opt 1966; 23: 186-209.
Although standards proposed by Sheedy3 and Glendill 14. Vingrys AJ, Cole BL. Are colour vision standards justified for
and Jamnik5 recognize that some colour deficient the transport industry? Ophthal and Physiol Opt 1988; 8:257-274.
people are suitable for firefighting duties we do not 15. Wyszecki G, Stiles WS. Colour Science. New York: John Wiley,
1967.
agree with the standards they suggest. Neither of these 16. The loss prevention council, Library offire safety Vol 2:Fire and
investigations adequately assesses the colour vision hazardous substances. Ian Jerome, ed. London: Fire Protection
demands of the job. Glendill and Jamnik5 suggest that Association, ISBN 0902167 61-8. 1994.
only those who fail the City University test should be 17. Cole BL, Vingrys AJ. Do protanomals have difficulty seeing
barred entry to the fire service. This standard does red lights ? In: Proceedings of the 20th session of the CIE. Paris:
not prevent protanomalous people from becoming fire- CIE Publication No. 56, E04, 1983: 1-3.
18. Verriest G, Neubauer O, Marre M, Uvijls A. New investigations
fighters. Standards proposed by Sheedy3 are particularly concerning the relationships between congenital colour vision
inappropriate: he suggests that only dichromats are defects and road traffic security. Int Ophthalmol 1980; 2:87-99.
unsuitable for firefighting work. Such a standard would
I H. Margrain ef a/.: Colour vision requirements of firefighters 123
CIE chromatlcityco-ordinates
Name of gas Description of cyclinder colour X y
Acetylene maroon 0.353 0.325
Air french grey 0.323 0.356
Ammonia ' black 0.310 0.316
Argon peacock blue 0.240 0.302
Carbon dioxide black 0.310 0.316
Carbon monoxide signal red 0.548 0.334
Chlorine golden yellow 0.485 0.454
Coal gas signal red 0.548 0.334
Ethyl chloride French grey 0.323 0.356
Ethylene dark violet 0.304 0.244
Ethylene oxide dark violet 0.304 0.244
Helium middle brown 0.413 0.380
Hydrogen signal red 0.548 0.334
Hydrogen cyanide peacock blue 0.240 0.302