0% found this document useful (0 votes)
11 views24 pages

Differences in Thematic Map Reading by S

This study investigates the differences in thematic map reading between grammar school students and their geography teacher using eye-tracking methods. Results indicate that while students can learn effectively from thematic maps, they struggle with certain cartographic visualization methods, particularly with poorly legible symbols and estimating values from legends. The teacher employed a different reading strategy, relying on prior knowledge rather than the map legend, highlighting a significant divergence in map interpretation between students and their instructor.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
11 views24 pages

Differences in Thematic Map Reading by S

This study investigates the differences in thematic map reading between grammar school students and their geography teacher using eye-tracking methods. Results indicate that while students can learn effectively from thematic maps, they struggle with certain cartographic visualization methods, particularly with poorly legible symbols and estimating values from legends. The teacher employed a different reading strategy, relying on prior knowledge rather than the map legend, highlighting a significant divergence in map interpretation between students and their instructor.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 24

International Journal of

Geo-Information

Article
Differences in Thematic Map Reading by Students
and Their Geography Teacher
Marketa Beitlova, Stanislav Popelka * and Vit Vozenilek
Department of Geoinformatics, Palacký University Olomouc, 17. listopadu 12, 77900 Olomouc, Czech Republic;
marketa.beitlova@upol.cz (M.B.); vit.vozenilek@upol.cz (V.V.)
* Correspondence: stanislav.popelka@upol.cz

Received: 29 June 2020; Accepted: 18 August 2020; Published: 19 August 2020 

Abstract: A school world atlas is likely the first systematic cartographic product which students
encounter in their lives. However, only a few empirical studies have analysed school atlases in
the context of map reading and learning geographical curricula. The present paper describes an
eye-tracking study conducted on 30 grammar school students and their geography teacher. The study
explored ten tasks using thematic world maps contained in the Czech school world atlas. Three
research questions were posed: (i) Are students able to learn using these particular types of maps?
(ii) Have the cartographic visualization methods in the school atlas been adequately selected? (iii) Does
the teacher read the maps in the same manner as students? The results proved that the students were
sufficiently able to learn using thematic maps. The average correctness of their answers exceeded
70%. However, the results highlighted several types of cartographic visualization methods which
students found difficult to read. Most of the difficulties arose from map symbols being poorly legible.
The most problematic task was estimating the value of the phenomenon from the symbol size legend.
Finally, the difference between the students’ and teacher’s manner of reading maps in each task
was analysed qualitatively and then quantitatively by applying two different scanpath comparison
methods. The study revealed that the geography teacher applied a different method than her students.
She avoided looking at the map legend and solved the task using her knowledge.

Keywords: thematic maps; map reading; school atlas; eye-tracking; scanpath comparison;
geography education

1. Introduction

1.1. Map Reading


According to Pravda [1] and Pravda and Kusendová [2], reading a map (perception and
understanding map content) is an essential indicator of intelligence in modern humans. Reading a
map consists of perceiving the map, using the map’s legend, and understanding the map’s content.
Reading a map is therefore a process of understanding its content through knowledge of the map’s
language and methods of its use. Reading a map would be meaningless if it was not followed by the
use of information acquired from the map, such as standard navigation of the terrain and simple map
measurements or generating information which enhances human knowledge. Map reading images
studied by Vondráková and Voženílek [3] indicated some specific findings which were reflected in
map reader’s preferences. Initially, the preferred image map was very often useless, and users chose
the map that was subjectively evaluated as one of the worst, but was much more suitable for solving
the task. The literature review on map reading found that the vast majority of sources focused on
reading topographic maps and navigation of the terrain. In the present paper, the authors understand
“map reading” differently. It is not wayfinding, but rather how maps are used to obtain desired

ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492; doi:10.3390/ijgi9090492 www.mdpi.com/journal/ijgi


ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 2 of 24

information from one or more maps to understand the surroundings. This definition was adopted from
Muehrcke, et al. [4]. Many cartographers have addressed the question of how to classify and organize
how maps are used. Olson [5], Board [6] and Morrison [7] mention an increasing complexity in each
level of map use. One of the first researchers who attempted to identify task levels in maps was Olson.
Olson [5] identified three tasks levels and organized tasks according to a hierarchical structure
with ascending difficulty. The first task level involves comparing the characteristics of individual map
symbols, such as shape, relative size, importance, et cetera. The second level means recognizing the
properties of symbol groups on the map as a whole, such as spatial patterns, similarity to other map
patterns, et cetera. The tasks at this level are more complex yet still deal with abstract symbols. The third
level is using the map to assist in decision making or creating knowledge content by associating map
symbols with other information.
Olson’s definition of map reading tasks was also adopted by Board [6], who studied map-reading
tasks suitable for the study of cartographic communication. This study was followed by the work of
Morrison [7], who offered an alternative description of map reading tasks. Morrison’s tasks are (1)
pre-map tasks (acquire, develop, orient map), (2) detection, discrimination and recognition (locate,
identify, define, verify), (3) estimating tasks (calculate, compare, measure), (4) attitude towards map
style (positive, preferred).
Muehrcke, Kimerling and Juliana [4] and Ormeling [8] used the terms map reading, map analysis,
and map interpretation instead of levels. Other authors have considered the same classification.
An extensive review of this issue was offered by Havelková and Hanus [9], who mentioned the
authors [10–15] and others.
Map reading: when reading a map, the user translates its elements into mental images of the
environment. The first step is identifying map symbols. The reader must creatively confront the
elements on the map with those of the real world [4]. According to Wiegand [11], map reading is
characterised as the simple extraction of information from a map. Map features are identified and
named, and their attributes noted. Specific skills might demonstrate symbol detection, discrimination
and decoding, legend comprehension, or determining coordinates [9].
Map analysis: analysis aims to describe spatial structure and relationships. When analysing a
map, the user must reduce the information in the map according to a certain classification in order to
understand it and describe it to other people. Analysis provides a description, not an explanation or
interpretation [4]. Map analysis involves processing information, for example, to describe patterns and
relationships or measure the distances between locations [11]. Map skills associated with map analysis
allow users to extract information about the phenomenon at a location and its distribution, determine
spatial relationships between phenomena, compare the spatial distribution of phenomena, and use the
map scale or plan routes and navigation [9].
Map interpretation: when interpreting the map, the user discovers patterns and seeks explanations
for them. This is a complex creative act involving both previous steps of map reading and map analysis.
Interpreting the map requires more than just understanding the map’s language. The more knowledge
and experience the user can draw on, the better the user can interpret spatial relationships and patterns
in the map [4]. Map interpretation moves beyond what is indicated on the map and involves the
application of previously acquired information to solve problems or make decisions [11].

1.2. School World Atlases in the Czech Republic


Atlases are one of the most important auxiliary materials used in teaching social sciences, history
and geography [16].
Three school world atlases from three different publishers are currently available in the Czech
Republic. Kartografie PRAHA, TERRA, and SHOCart, are private cartographic publishing companies
which collaborate with university experts. Atlases and any other teaching material used in schools
in the Czech Republic must undergo a review and approval procedure by the Ministry of Education,
Youth and Sports of the Czech Republic.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 3 of 24

School world atlases are teaching materials used by students from the sixth grade in elementary
school (11 years) onwards to high school students (19 years). School world atlases have many
complementary atlases which focus mainly on the Czech Republic, individual continents, and topics
such as world energy or finance. All three of the above-mentioned publishers provide their school
world atlases in digital versions.

1.3. Evaluation of Thematic Maps and School World Atlases


Only a few studies have focused on the educational aspects of map reading. Brychtova, et al. [17]
indicate that a visually appealing map usually achieves higher preferences and popularity with the
public, especially students. Carswell [18] analysed children’s abilities in topographic map reading,
summarizing the findings that teachers overestimate success in teaching map reading skills while also
underestimating the map-reading abilities of children. van Dijk, et al. [19] and Schee and Dijk [20] tested
the ability of students to use different types of map skills. Their studies revealed that giving students
an opportunity to determine their own sequence of performing map assignments is a recommended
strategy. Hanus and Marada [21] compared the curricular documents of different countries with special
emphasis on map skills. The results showed that the potential of geography in map skills in the Czech
curricula is not sufficiently fulfilled.
Havelková and Hanus [9] conducted research examining the effect of different (thematic) mapping
methods. The results indicated that students experienced problems with maps using quantitative
mapping methods. Students were more successful in tasks where qualitative or both qualitative
and quantitative mapping methods were used. Working with thematic maps was also evaluated
by Reyes Nuñez, et al. [22] on a sample of students from Argentina and Hungary. The evaluation
was supplemented by a questionnaire for teachers. Thematic (political) atlases are used by teachers
in Argentina, but mainly physical atlases are used in Hungary. Reyes Nuñez and Juhász [23] also
analysed the effectiveness of cartograms. The results showed that geometric area cartograms were
more suitable than geographic area cartograms for use in school cartography. The effectiveness of
an area cartogram in visualizing spatial data was also evaluated by Sun and Li [24]. Their analysis
showed that a pseudo-cartogram is the most preferred technique, and a Dorling cartogram is the
least preferred.
Kubíček, et al. [25] measured response times and error rates in map-reading tasks relative to
different variations of linear feature visualizations. The results confirmed that colour hue and size
were more efficient than shape and colour value.
Goł˛ebiowska [26] aimed to understand how the types of legend layout in thematic maps functioned
during map reading. Study participants were asked to perform two sets of tasks using two thematic
maps with different legend layouts. Three types of legend layouts were used in the study: list legend,
grouped legend, and natural legend. The use of a natural legend required the most time, as this
type of legend is not very common, and participants had to concentrate on understanding the legend
principle. The arrangement of symbols in a grouped legend reduced the load on working memory.
Pétera, et al. [27] conducted an empirical study exploring map drawing skills. Their empirical research
showed less developed competence models of map drawing as opposed to map reading.
However, none of the above-mentioned studies tested maps from atlases. Słomska [28] created an
overview of different types of maps used as stimuli in cartographical empirical research. The study
summarized 103 empirical studies from four cartographic journals. The study substantiated that only
one study [26] used maps from atlases. The atlas used in the study [29] was an interactive digital atlas
which displayed a broad range of thematic data for the USA. This type of information is completely
different material than a school world atlas.
One of the most comprehensive studies of school atlases was conducted by Bugdayci and
Bildirici [16], who evaluated 22 atlases used in geography education and social studies. The authors
examined generalization, symbology, fonts, colours, and common map elements. The final chapter
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 4 of 24

of the study described the map legend, geographic location, and map scale. It also contained some
examples and suggestions for improving the cartographic design of maps contained in the atlases.
Voženílek, et al. [30] investigated the awareness of Czech students of the symbol sets used in
11 different world school atlases. The research applied methods for literature search, comparison of
atlases, online surveys, and statistical processing. The results confirmed that Czech students were
able to understand the map symbols and cartographic methods used in European school atlases.
These results were consistent with Michaelidou, et al. [31], who analysed the ability of elementary
school children to analyse the map content of different thematic maps.
Blaha [32] highlighted the importance of aesthetics in the user-friendliness of cartographic products
and proposed evaluation methods for map aesthetics, such as scoring, classification, expert estimations,
and surveys. The scoring system was used in another study [33] on two Czech school world atlases
and explored aesthetics and user-friendliness in maps.
Peresadko and Baltabaeva [34] evaluated the school atlases currently used in Turkmenistan.
They indicated that the atlases were outdated and contained a large number of cartographic inaccuracies.
The authors justified the need to create a new school atlas for Turkmenistan.
Gómez Solórzano, et al. [35] conducted a survey of 50 respondents to compare printed and digital
atlases. Using five tasks, the authors measured correctness, reaction time, satisfaction, perception, and
emotions. The research showed that printed and digital atlases complement each other. Usability
metrics varied slightly; those related to correctness and reaction time were higher for the digital atlas,
while those related to satisfaction and perception were higher for the printed atlas.
Song, et al. [36] analysed the main factors affecting the design of symbols in the National Economic
Atlas of China. Zhang and Chen [37] undertook an evaluation of the structure, content and design of
the Shanxi Province tourist atlas.

1.4. The Use of Eye-Tracking


The first decade of the twenty-first century opened a new stage in perceptual research. This stage
could be described as cognitive-digital since this type of research is based on computer software and
deals with the cognitive aspects of map perception [38]. According to Rohrer [39], one of the most
objective methods in evaluating (cartographic) stimuli is eye-tracking since it shows “what people
do” instead of “what people say”. Popelka and Vozenilek [40] described the common aspects of
eye-tracking and space-time-cube and have encouraged joint studies in cartographic research.
Dong, et al. [41] applied eye-tracking in geographic education to evaluate the impact of geography
courses in students’ abilities to work with maps. However, the map used in the experiment was
not from a school atlas but a terrain visualization. Biland and Çöltekin [42] used a similar type of
stimuli. Havelková and Goł˛ebiowska [43] evaluated thematic maps using eye-tracking. In their study,
the stimuli were created by the authors but selected according to a content analysis of school geography
atlases and textbooks.
Kiik, et al. [44] compared four different designs of area symbols in thematic maps in a study
to determine whether area symbols are suitable in identifying the extent of polygons while not
distracting the map reader. The best results were achieved with hatches. Popelka and Dolezalova [45]
used three-dimensional thematic maps as stimuli in eye-tracking experiments. Brychtova and
Vondrákova [46] evaluated sequential colour schemes used in thematic maps. Göbel, et al. [45] used
eye-tracking to study the adaptation of legend content using gaze-based methods. The study showed
that legend content changed according to gaze. The symbol types which had been fixated on previously
were drawn with full opacity in the map’s legend, while all others were reduced.
The present study compares the map reading strategy of students and their teacher, which focuses
on the comparison between experts and novices. This issue was previously analysed in a topic related
to cartography, for example, in a study by Burian, et al. [46]. The study evaluated the interpretation of
four different urban plans and compared students and experts in urban planning. The results showed
that the experts made a relatively large number of mistakes since they were too self-confident and did
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 5 of 24

not look into the map legend. This might signify a parallel with the teacher’s strategy in this study,
who also avoided using a map legend and answered directly. The difference between the students’ and
teacher’s map-reading strategy might be also studied as the singular value decomposition similarity
between scanpath sets [47].
Anderson and Leinhardt [48] asked participants to draw the shortest distance between two
locations as they would appear on the earth’s surface (using a map with Mercator projection).
The results showed that geography experts performed significantly better than novices and pre-service
teachers. Their results contrast with the results of the present study, but it is necessary to acknowledge
that the tasks were completely different. The participants in the study of Anderson and Leinhardt [48]
were expected to use the rules according to their knowledge. In the present study, the participants
were instructed to use the map to solve the task.
The difference between experts and novices in reading planimetric and contour maps was analysed
by Thorndyke and Stasz [49] and Gilhooly, et al. [50]. More recently, the perception of interactive
and static 3D maps was investigated by Herman, et al. [51]. In contrast to our findings, the authors
uncovered a statistically significant difference from an accuracy point of view when experts were
more correct.
Other cartographic studies have been conducted by Ooms, et al. [52] and Ooms, et al. [53].
The participants in these studies worked with different types of map. Their results indicated that an
expert’s process of interpretation was much quicker than a novice’s. The research confirmed that the
trial duration of the teacher was quicker in some tasks but slower in others.
To the best of our knowledge, no previous eye-tracking study has evaluated students working
with school atlases.

1.5. Motivation and Research Questions


According to cartographic communication models [6,54–56], maps are products which aim to
assist people in understanding the world. Generally, the first systematic cartographic product young
people encounter in their lives is a school world atlas.
The most commonly used school world atlas in the Czech Republic is published by Kartografie
PRAHA. The authors of the present study surveyed 600 Czech geography teachers with an online
questionnaire, discovering that most of these teachers (94%) used this atlas in their geography lessons.
One of the survey questions asked about the role of the school world atlas in teaching. On a 10-point
Likert scale, 10 indicated the most important role; the median value of responses to this question was
9. Most of the teachers worked with the atlas every lesson (57%), while 29% of them worked with
the atlas every second lesson. Only 3% of the teachers used the atlas less than every third lesson.
These findings verify that the school world atlas is crucial material in geography teaching.
The atlas from Kartografie PRAHA contains 162 maps. Of these, 127 are thematic and 35 are
generally geographic. From the 50 world maps contained in the atlas, 9 thematic maps were selected
for the experiment. The selection criteria and detailed characteristics of the experiment’s maps are
described in Section 2.2. (Stimuli and Tasks).
These atlases should help students understand the natural and socio-economic environment of
the Earth. School atlases should therefore be comprehensible, well-arranged, and clear and easy to use
by students and their teachers.
Studies which examine school atlas map reading can reveal whether students are able to retrieve
the information presented on these maps and can also potentially detect problems in map design.
However, the process of understanding maps in school atlases has not yet been fully explored.
As described above, no study from 103 cartographic user studies has focused on school world atlases,
or atlases in general [28]. The objective of the present study is to begin to fill this gap.
The vast majority of cartographic communication models describe the process between the
cartographer and map reader. However, these models do not describe whether readers interpret maps
in the same manner. A comparison of map reading strategies between students and teachers might
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 6 of 24

unveil a source of problems some students have with map reading. If the teacher and students read
maps differently, educational processes might also be affected and disrupted.
The present paper describes an eye-tracking study using thematic maps from a school world atlas as
stimuli. Participants solved several tasks using these maps. According to this distribution of map skills,
the tasks applied in the experiment fell into the categories of map reading (symbol detection, legend
comprehension) and map analysis (extraction of phenomenon location and distribution, comparison of
spatial phenomena distribution) categories.
The main aim of the experiment in the present study was to analyse how students and their
teacher read maps in a school world atlas. The task in the experiment was to locate a particular object
on a thematic map. The present paper addresses three research questions:

Q1: Are students able to learn with thematic maps and legends from a school world atlas by finding
information and searching for specific objects on a map?
Q2: Are the cartographic methods used in the school world atlas comprehensible to students?
Q3: Do students read the thematic maps from the school world atlas in the same manner as
their teacher?

2. Methods

2.1. Experiment Design


At the beginning of the testing session, the purpose of the experiment was explained to participants
and basic information obtained about the principle of eye-tracking technology. The experiment was
designed in the GazePoint Analysis software. A scheme of the study and experiment is given in
Figure 1.

Figure 1. Scheme of the study.

The experiment was calibrated before testing commenced, and the results were then inspected
by the technician responsible for testing. After successful calibration, a task with no stimulus was
given to each respondent. The respondents received an indefinite time to read and remember the task.
A fixation cross was displayed for 600 ms between the task and the map stimuli to calibrate the origin
of eye-movement trajectory to the centre of the screen. The stimulus was displayed for a maximum of
60 s, and respondents were required to find particular objects on the map. Stimuli were presented in a
fixed order, from simplest to more complex (according to the authors’ opinions). In most of the tasks,
the participants responded using a mouse click (clicks) directly on the map. Only task 10 required the
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 7 of 24

participants to search for specific information on the map and say it aloud. The technician registered
these answers.

2.2. Stimuli and Tasks


All stimuli used in the study were obtained from the electronic version of the School Atlas of
the World published by Kartografie PRAHA (4th edition) [57]. All the maps are identical to the print
version of the atlas. Nine thematic world maps with different topics were selected for the experiment.
Because of the monitor’s aspect ratio and resolution (4:3; 1280 × 1024), some maps required
cropping for better legibility. No relevant parts or information that could affect the results of the
experiment were removed. Each map always contained at least a map field and legend to preserve
as much of the map as possible concerning legibility. All maps used as stimuli differ in visualization
methods, data type (qualitative/quantitative) and style of legend. All maps are shown in Figure 2.
Full-resolution previews are included in the Supplementary Materials.

Figure 2. Map stimuli used in the experiment’s tasks.

The three research questions determined strategic selection of map-stimuli and compilation of
tasks. Q1 asks whether students are able to learn with thematic maps in a school world atlas. The atlas
was thoroughly inspected for its coverage of a wide range of geography curriculum topics. Accordingly,
various world maps focusing on different geographical themes (vegetation zones, urbanisation,
geology, economy, etc.) were selected for the eye-tracking experiment. Q2 probes the comprehension
of cartographic methods. Maps which applied different cartographic methods (graduated symbols,
choropleth maps, area symbols, etc.) were therefore selected. Q3 investigates the similarities and
differences in the map-reading strategies of the students and their teacher and builds on concepts of
Q1 and Q2.
The tasks were formulated for each map stimuli according to the type of information displayed,
visualization method, and legend style and related directly to the research question of whether
respondents could read thematic maps and use the legend to search for information and find a specific
object on the map.
The maps used in the experiment fell into several types according to the type of data which
they displayed: qualitative (Map01, Map05, and Map06), quantitative (Map02, Map04, and Map09),
and both qualitative and quantitative (Map03, Map07, Map08, and Map10).
In the maps which displayed qualitative data, the assigned task was straightforward. Respondents
were required to find an object in the legend and then identify it on the map. The task was to identify
all the areas with temperate deciduous forests in Map01, a convergent plane boundary in Map05,
and places where iron ore was mined in Map06.
Quantitative data were visualized using a choropleth map (Map02), graduated symbol map
(Map04), and flow map (Map09). The task in the choropleth map (Map02) was to identify all the
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 8 of 24

countries with less than 20% urban populations. The task in both diagram maps was to find urban
agglomeration and shipping routes with certain properties.
The remaining maps contained both qualitative and quantitative information. All of these maps
included proportional symbols, and areas were displayed as either choropleth maps or area symbols.
The tasks required participants to work with the diagrams and identify the country with the highest
proportion of potatoes in total calorie consumption (Map03), countries with specific GDP (Map07),
and countries with higher imports than exports (Map08). In the task for Map08, the answer could
be discovered from the graduated symbols (showing values for imports and exports) or using area
symbols (chorochromatic map showing trade balance). In the final task (Map10), participants estimated
Brazil’s exports according to a value scale.
Because the atlas is in the Czech language, all of the tasks were also formulated in Czech.
Translations of these tasks are in Table 1.

Table 1. List of the experiment’s tasks (translated from Czech to English).

ID Description of Task
Task01 Identify all areas with temperate deciduous forests.
Task02 Identify all countries with less than 20% urban populations.
Task03 Identify the country with the highest proportion of potatoes in total calorie consumption.
Identify urban agglomerations with more than 20 million inhabitants in North America,
Task04
Central America, and South America.
Task05 Identify a convergent plate boundary.
Task06 Identify a place on every continent where iron ore is mined.
Task07 Identify three countries with a total GDP of approximately USD 2500 billion.
Task08 Identify three countries whose imports exceed exports.
Task09 Identify three shipping routes with an annual capacity under 100 million tonnes.
Task10 Estimate Brazil’s export volume in billions of USD.

2.3. Participants
Forty-one third grade students (~18 years) from a Czech grammar school participated in the
experiment. Testing was conducted in two stages over two weeks at the end of 2018. The students’
geography teacher also attended the testing in the first half of 2019. For all of the participants, the
testing in this experiment was their first experience with eye-tracking technology. Some of them may
have felt nervous, which may have affected the data quality. Eleven of the 41 students were removed
from the dataset because of the inaccuracy of the device or problems with calibration. This data
pre-processing stage is described later. The data recorded for 30 students (8 males and 22 female) and
one geography teacher (female) were eventually included in the analysis.
The teacher who participated in the research has been teaching geography for over 30 years
at grammar school with more than 400 students. She uses the school world atlas from Kartografie
PRAHA (version from 2006) and older atlases (from around 1989) in her classes. Her students use
atlases every lesson, primarily with general geographic maps and less with thematic maps (climate,
hydrology, lithosphere, biosphere, pedosphere, etc.).

2.4. Apparatus
Eye trajectories were measured using three GazePoint 3 eye-trackers operated by three technicians.
The GazePoint eye-tracker is an inexpensive device similar to TheEyeTribe tracker and Tobii EyeX.
The accuracy and precision of all these low-cost eye-trackers have been previously tested in the studies
by Dalmaijer [58], Ooms, et al. [59] and Popelka, et al. [60]. Janthanasub and Meesad [61] tested the
accuracy of the GazePoint 3 eye-tracker in their study. The results showed it was suitable for use in
research. GazePoint 3 has also been used in studies in the field of neurosciences [62], marketing [63],
mathematics [64], physics [65], kinesiology, and sports science [66]. A comprehensive list of publications
concerning the GazePoint tracker is available at https://www.gazept.com/meet-the-team/publications/.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 9 of 24

2.5. Data Pre-Processing


Recorded eye-movement data was pre-processed and validated before data analysis. Recording
was conducted in the classroom. The students had had no previous experience with eye-tracking testing.
Data were recorded using GazePoint Analysis software. However, the application’s capabilities
for data analysis are minimal. The data were therefore converted into a format readable by the
open-source application OGAMA [67] using the online tool at http://eyetracking.upol.cz/gp2ogama.
The OGAMA application allows the ratio of samples with coordinates 0;0 (upper-left corner of the
stimulus) to be calculated. These samples represent data loss caused by eye-blinking and lost signals.
The ratio of samples recorded off-screen was another factor which required checking because of the
GazePoint eye-tracker. In the extreme cases, the ratio exceeded 60%. This data had to be removed from
the dataset.
The values of the ratio of data loss (α) and off-screen samples (β) for each participant and stimuli
α β
are given in Table 1. The instances where α or β ≥ 10 are highlighted in red. In the next step, the
α β≥
sum of these samples was calculated, and 11 students with more than three problematic stimuli were
excluded from further analysis. The remainder of the participants were renamed consecutively S01–S30.
The students’ geography teacher also engaged in the testing. A summary of the quality of recorded
data is depicted in Figure 3. The values in the table represent the ratio of data loss α (left) or off-screen
α
samples β (right) for each
β participant. The TOTAL column contains the number of cases where the
values exceeded 10%.

Figure 3. Overview of the data quality for 30 students and their teacher.

2.6. Methods of Analyses


Fixations and saccades were identified before the analyses. The fixation detection algorithm
(I-DT) thresholds were set to 20 pixels (distance between points) and 5 (minimum number of samples).
The optimal fixation detection algorithm is described by Popelka [68] in more detail.
Q1 (students’ ability to learn with thematic maps) was analysed according to the correctness
of the responses, and trial duration was analysed as a metric indicating the time required for
respondents to give an answer. Participants marked their answers on the stimuli using mouse clicks.
The online tool http://eyetracking.upol.cz/gp2vanalytics/ converted data from GazePoint Analysis
into V-Analytics [69]. V-Analytics was used to visualize mouse clicks and can also be applied to
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 10 of 24

eye-movement data analysis [70]. The Kruskal–Wallis post hoc Nemenyi test was applied to statistically
evaluate the recorded data in RStudio at a significance level of 0.05.
Q2 (comprehension of cartographic methods) was answered based on a visual inspection of
recorded scanpaths and data visualization using sequence charts. The results obtained in Q1 (correctness
of answers and trial duration) were used for pointing to problematic cartographic tasks. In the next
phase, two experimenters analysed eye-movement trajectories (scanpaths) and created sequence charts.
From these visualizations, they tried to reveal the reason for low correctness or high trial duration.
Typically, the distribution of attention between the map and the legend or focusing on specific parts of
the map was analysed.
Sequence charts are displaying the distribution of fixations in predefined areas of interest
(AOI). Participants’ eye-movement data are represented with coloured bars. The colour of each
cell in a bar represents one fixation in the particular AOI. Unfortunately, neither OGAMA nor
GazePoint Analysis offers this type of visualization; the charts were created manually in MS Excel
using the PART function and conditional formatting. Sequence charts for all tasks are available at
http://eyetracking.upol.cz/atlases_thematic/SequenceCharts.pdf.
Q3 (comparison of difference of map-reading strategies of students and their teacher) addressed
an analysis of eye-movement data using two approaches to calculate scanpath similarity. The first
approach is based on the string-edit-distance using the ScanGraph tool [71,72], which is designed
to process data exported from OGAMA directly. ScanGraph analyses the order of visited AOIs as a
sequence of characters and calculates the similarity of these sequences using Levenshtein distance,
the Needleman–Wunsch algorithm or Damerau–Levenshtein distance. Individual participants are
visualized as nodes in a graph, and ScanGraph looks for cliques in this graph. The cliques represent
groups of participants who were similar to each other at least to a specific (user-defined) degree.
The second approach in analysing the scanpath similarity is based on the multimatch method introduced
by Jarodzka, et al. [73] and Dewhurst, et al. [74]. This method represents scanpaths as mathematical
vectors and allows the scanpath to retain a sequence of fixations and saccades and measure similarity
using geometry. Multimatch similarity measurements are sensitive to the differences in shape, position,
length, direction, and duration between two scanpaths [73]. As the authors of multimatch indicate,
the method does have some drawbacks, the most significant being that measurements only compare
two scanpaths.
In the present study, this drawback is addressed by using batch computation in a python-based
multimatch alternative called multimatch-gaze [75]. Batch computing was possible in all similarity
measurements except duration. In this case, the results were normalized according to the length of the
longer scanpath, so it is not possible to compare values for multiple pairs of scanpaths. The results from
multimatch-gaze were transformed into separate matrices for each task and each type of similarity
(vector, direction, length, position). These matrices can be either imported into ScanGraph for visual
analysis or analysed directly (i.e., in MS Excel).

3. Results

3.1. Correctness of Answers—Students’ Ability to Learn with Maps (Q1)


In the majority of tasks, participants marked their answers directly on the map using mouse click
(clicks). The correctness of these answers was then determined. These answers were used to discuss
and resolve the first research question. Participants only estimated the value in Task10 according to the
symbol size legend. Figure 4 contains a summary of the answers. The correct answers are highlighted
in green, incorrect in red, and partially correct answers (i.e., not all correct countries were marked) in
orange. All missing answers were marked as incorrect. Correctness in all the tasks by all participants
was summarized. Each correct answer was allocated 1 point, and partially correct answers 0.5 points.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 11 of 24

Figure 4. Summary of the correctness of answers for all tasks.

The most straightforward tasks were Task02 (“Identify all countries with less than 20% of the urban
population”) and Task05 (“Identify convergent plate boundary”), with a correctness of 92%. The most
difficult task, however, was Task10, where participants estimated the volume of Brazil’s exports
according to symbol size legend. Although the correct answer was USD 250 billion, the responses
from participants varied from 3 to 5000. Because of tolerances, responses indicating a value between
200 and 300 were counted as partially correct. Participants also demonstrated problems with Task07,
which required them to identify countries with a specific GDP value according to symbol size legend.
The average correctness of answers from all students achieved was 71%. It could therefore be said
that generally, students were sufficiently able to read thematic maps and learn from them.
The trial duration of each task was also investigated. The boxplots in Figure 5 chart the data for
30 students. The value for the geography teacher is indicated with a red dot. Statistically significant
differences between the tasks according to the Kruskal–Wallis post hoc Nemenyi test are represented
using blue lines.
The participants required the most time to solve Task07 and Task10. These two tasks also indicated
problems with correctness. A high trial duration value was also observed for Task01. However,
participants needed only around 19 s (median) to solve Task02. The trial duration value for this task
differed significantly from four other tasks (Task01 (p < 0.001), Task04 (p = 0.004), Task07 (p < 0.001)
and Task10 (p < 0.001)).
No clear connection for trial duration between the students and teacher was identified. In some
tasks, the teacher was quicker than the students, but for other tasks, the teacher’s trial duration was
much higher.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 12 of 24

Figure 5. Trial duration for 30 students and the experiment’s tasks. The red dot indicates the geography
teacher’s answers.

3.2. Results of Individual Tasks—Comprehension of Cartographic Methods (Q2)


The next step analysed the participants’ behaviour in solving individual tasks.

3.2.1. Task01
In the experiment’s first task, the participants identified all areas with temperate deciduous forests.
It was assumed this task would be very easy for the students since all that was required was identifying
the correct symbol from the legend and recognizing all the areas indicated by this symbol. However,
the accuracy of the answers was 61%, and only 13 students solved the task correctly and 11 students
partially. The students marked temperate deciduous forests together with taiga or even subtropical
and tropical forests. The reason was probably a poorly distinguishable legend, with all three types of
vegetation being visualized using very similar symbols (see bottom-left section of Figure 6). Figure 6
indicates the fixations of participants in grey and the teacher’s fixation in red. The answers (clicks) are
visualized as blue dots. From the distribution of fixations, it is evident that participants did not focus
their attention on the strip with climate belts at the edge of the map field.
The teacher answered partially by clicking on the temperate deciduous forests in Europe and also
taiga in Canada. She did not focus her attention on the legend.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 13 of 24

Figure 6. Fixations of students (grey) and their teacher (red), together with answers (blue dots). Similar
symbols from the legend are enlarged in the left lower corner.

3.2.2. Task02
In the second task, respondents found and marked all countries on the choropleth map with less
than 20% of the urban population. Participants found this task very easy, demonstrating one of the
highest accuracies in the entire experiment and requiring the least amount of time to solve the task
(19.25 s as can be seen from Figure 5).
The sequence chart in Figure 7 shows the distribution of fixations in the map field (green) and
legend (red). Only two students (S13 and S22) answered incorrectly (Figure 4) since they did not look
at the legend at all (see Figure 7).

Figure 7. Sequence chart for Task02 showing the distribution of participants’ fixations in the map field
(green) and legend (red). Grey represents fixations out of the AOI.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 14 of 24

The correct answer for this task was marking eight countries with the brightest colour. The teacher
marked only three of them. Since it is visible from the sequence chart, the teacher looked at the legend
when she began to view the stimulus, although only for a brief moment. She probably indicated
countries according to her knowledge from urban geography.

3.2.3. Task03
In the third task, the participants identified the country with the highest proportion of potato
consumption according to total calories. The map legend contained three sections, and the participants
were required to discover the information from pie charts, where brown depicted potatoes. Each of the
participants looked at the legend, and four of them answered incorrectly (S5, S13, S21, and S28).
The teacher looked at the legend only briefly compared to students (9 fixations, while the students’
average was more than 21). Her answer was ranked as partially correct since she selected more than
one country. This task took her the most time to solve in the whole experiment.

3.2.4. Task04
Task04 required identifying a particular graduated symbol on the map. The task was to identify
an urban agglomeration with more than 20 million inhabitants in North America, Central America,
and South America. The legend contained three different symbol sizes for urban agglomerations
(see Figure 8). The participants looked for the biggest circle on the map. Ten students indicated
incorrect answers, and four others were only partially correct. The problem was likely in the difficulty
of identifying graduated symbols (Figure 8).

Figure 8. Symbol size legend of the map for Task04 (translated into English).

The teacher encountered the above-mentioned problem. From the recordings of her
eye-movements, it was evident that she had problems in distinguishing the size of the symbols
in North and South America. She spent a considerable time on this task and answered only partially
correctly since she mismatched the size of the symbols.

3.2.5. Task05
Task05 was one of the easiest in the whole experiment with correctness of 90% as can be seen from
Figure 4. Participants were required to identify the convergent plate boundary. The legend contained
four different linear symbols for plate boundary types. Only one student responded incorrectly (S13)
and was the only who recorded no fixation on the correct part of the legend (with linear symbols for
plate boundaries). This student achieved the worst results in the whole experiment.
The teacher recorded the quickest answer for Task05. Her trial duration of 7.7 s was also quicker
than the students (p = 0.06). The teacher omitted the legend and spontaneously focused her attention
on the plate boundaries on the map. Unfortunately, she mismatched the divergent and convergent
boundary, so her answer was incorrect.

3.2.6. Task06
Task06 was also a simple task. The participants identified a place on every continent where iron
ore was mined. Iron ore was indicated with a red “Fe” symbol, and many students needed only a
few fixations to inspect the legend to find the right symbol. Only two students indicated an incorrect
answer. One of them (S17) did not remember the task and searched for a different symbol (oil field).
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 15 of 24

The teacher again responded according to her knowledge, not according to the map. Although
she looked briefly at the legend twice, she did not search for the correct symbol. She marked the
countries where iron ore was mined (Canada, South Africa, Sweden, and Brazil), but her clicks were
not near the “Fe” symbols.

3.2.7. Task07
Task07 was one of the most complicated in the experiment with correctness of only 35% as can be
seen from Figure 4. The participants identified three countries with a total GDP of approximately USD
2500 billion. GDP information was visualized in a proportional pie chart with a logarithmic scale. To
find the correct answer, participants had to imagine how large the symbol depicting the value of USD
2500 billion was. This process is indicated in Figure 9.

Figure 9. Legend for Task07 (translated into English), with the value of USD 2500 billion (blue circle)
indicated in the pie chart according to relative size.

Participants had difficulties in estimating the pie chart size. Only eight indicated the correct
answer. Almost all the pie charts on the map were marked at least once, which may denote that
students misunderstood the legend. As evident from the sequence chart in Figure 10, a majority (55%)
fixated on the legend (red), and yet they did not respond correctly.

Figure 10. Sequence chart for Task07 showing the distribution of participants’ fixations on the map
field (green) and legend (red). Grey represents fixations out of the AOI.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 16 of 24

In this task, the teacher used the legend for the first time in the experiment. It took a long time
until she oriented herself in the map, but despite this, her trial duration was less than the median value
of students. Her answers were recorded as correct.

3.2.8. Task08
Task08 was to identify three countries whose imports exceeded exports. Finding the correct
answer was possible in two manners. The first was to search for the area symbols (chorochromatic
map) where the information for the trade balance was depicted directly. The second was to use the bar
charts (Figure 11) to find the countries where the bar for imports was taller than the one for exports.

Figure 11. Legend for Task08 (translated into English).

Only five participants (S12, S14, S23, S27, and S29) worked with area symbols. The charts were
used by 19 other participants, who also indicated the correct answer. These numbers suggest that the
task was relatively easy for the participants and was also one of those with low trial duration.
After the experience from the previous task, the teacher looked directly at the legend, and she
spent a relatively long time there. She focused on the bar charts in the legend and selected countries
accordingly. Her trial duration was slightly less than the median for students. Her answer was
also correct.

3.2.9. Task09
Task09 was to identify three shipping routes with an annual capacity below 100 million tonnes.
Information about the shipping routes was visualized using graduated linear symbols in blue.
The colour was similar to the colour for parallels and meridians, and some participants mismatched
these objects. Four participants marked the correct symbol in close proximity to the harbours.
Discussion with the students revealed that they marked the lines near the ports to avoid confusion
with symbols depicting parallels and meridians.
In general, the task was relatively easy; only four students marked the answer incorrectly.
Participant S13 did not look at the legend at all. The teacher behaved similarly, and her response was
also incorrect.

3.2.10. Task10
In the final task, participants estimated Brazil’s export volume in billions of USD. The map
was the same as the map used forTask08. To find the correct answer, participants inspected the bar
chart’s legend, where 1 mm corresponded to USD 50 billion (Figure 11). This was similar to Task07.
The participants fixated mostly on the legend (51%), but only one-quarter of participants indicated a
correct answer. Brazil’s export value was approximately USD 250 billion, so values 200 and 300 were
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 17 of 24

considered partially correct. The responses varied from 3 to 5000. This wide range suggested that
participants were completely lost with this task and that estimating the value caused them difficulties.
The teacher looked into the legend but did not fixate on Brazil. Thus, she could not estimate the
size of the bar chart and her answer was incorrect.

3.3. Scanpath Similarity—Difference between Students and Their Teacher (Q3)


The third research question dealt with comparing the strategy used to inspect stimuli between
students and their geography teacher. As was described in the previous part of the text, the teacher
attempted to solve the tasks mainly using her knowledge, not using the map. The quantitative
comparison of the strategies used by the students and the teacher was based on the results of the
multimatch-gaze and ScanGraph tool. The similarity of the scanpaths for each pair of participants
was evaluated according to four multimatch-gaze metrics (vector, direction, length, and position) and
using string-edit-distance (Levenshtein distance) in ScanGraph. The resulting matrices (Figure 12)
show the average mutual similarity between students and the average similarity between the teacher
and her students. The subtracted average values (∆) indicate whether the teacher applied a unique
strategy in inspecting stimuli or used a more conventional Δ
approach (similar to students). The higher
the value, the more unique the teacher’s scanpath, which meant the more dissimilar the map-reading
strategy. Values higher than average + standard deviation are highlighted in red.

Figure 12. Results of the similarity analysis between the scanpaths of students and their geography
teacher. Red values indicate a unique strategy by the teacher.

Figure 13 reveals the teacher’s unique strategy in Task05 from Levenshtein distance and in Task09
from position measurements using multimatch-gaze. In these cases, the similarity between the teacher
and students was clearly lower than between the students.
Figure 13 depicts these two extreme examples together with Task02, where the differences were
minimal. On the left side of the figure, the teacher’s scanpath is highlighted using red. The scanpaths
of the students are displayed in grey. In Task09 and Task05, the teacher used a different strategy than
the students, because she did not look into the legend and focused her attention on different parts
of the stimuli than the students. On the other hand, in Task02, the teacher looked at the legend and
focused her attention on Africa, where the correct answer was located. The same strategy was used by
the students.
The middle part of Figure 13 displays the results of the position measurement calculated in
multimatch-gaze and visualized with the ScanGraph tool. Each dot in the graph represents one
participant. The participants with a similarity of at least 85% are connected. The teacher is visualized
in red. Task09 and Task05 evidently show that the teacher is not connected to the students. By contrast,
in Task02, the teacher used a strategy at least 85% similar to 27 students. The section at the right of
the figure displays the results of string-edit-distance using Levenshtein distance (similarity greater
than 75%) and confirms a similarity in strategy. It was calculated from the sequence of visited areas of
interest. In Task09 and Task05, the teacher did not inspect the legend; therefore, the similarity of her
strategy towards the students was low. In Task02, the teacher looked at the legend as students did and
she had similarity higher than 75% with nine of them.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 18 of 24

Figure 13. Comparison of students’ and teacher’s map-reading strategies. The left column indicates the
scanpaths, the middle column shows the ScanGraph visualization of position measurement, and the
right column depicts the results of Levenshtein distance (also visualized using ScanGraph). The teacher
is in red.

4. Discussion
The present paper describes an empirical study which evaluates student learning with a school
world atlas. The research is one of the first eye-tracking studies using this kind of stimuli.
In designing the present study’s experiment, the authors selected ten thematic maps which
depicted the entire world. The maps were also selected to include different types of cartographic
visualization methods. For use as stimuli, these maps were cropped to preserve the legibility on
computer monitors with an aspect ratio of 4:3. Monitors with this aspect ratio were used to ensure
good quality in the recorded eye-movements. With wide-screen monitors, the pupils of the eyes might
have been obscured by eyelids.
Students had 60 s to solve the task. Sixty seconds was a sufficient amount of time for most of the
participants and we chose this limit to avoid the situation when the student will try to solve the task
for so long. The correctness of the students’ answers was consistent with the work of Havelková and
Hanus [9]. They determined that the students were more successful in tasks with either qualitative or
both qualitative and quantitative cartographic methods.
Students from two third grade grammar school classes (~18 years) were selected as participants.
Data for 41 students were recorded, but 11 were excluded from the experiment because of inaccuracies
in the eye-tracker. All the students shared the same geography teacher, which allowed a comparison to
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 19 of 24

be made between the teacher’s and students’ strategies. This determined the total number of study
participants, which was limited to the total number of students of both class groups.
Several approaches to data analysis were employed to compare the results of the students and
teacher. First, the teacher’s eye-movement data was thoroughly inspected and qualitatively described.
Two other methods of quantitative similarity calculations were applied. One approach involved a
string-edit-distance method which had been previously used in many eye-tracking studies to compare
different participant groups (i.e., [76–79]). Specifically, ScanGraph calculated the similarity of scanpaths
according to Levenshtein distance (i.e., [43,80,81]) and visualized the results calculated using the
multimatch method, which can only indicate similarity between two scanpaths. The present study
used batch calculations to calculate the similarity between all possible pairs of participants, in other
words, 961 calculations (31 × 31) for each of the ten stimuli in the experiment. The only problem
encountered was with metric duration, one of the five metrics used in multimatch. The results were
normalised by the length of the longer of two analysed scanpaths. It was complicated to find a solution
to this problem, and therefore this metric was excluded from the analysis.
Summarizing the differences between the students and the teacher was based on the average
similarity for all students and the average similarity between the teacher and all the students. These two
values were then subtracted. A greater difference suggested the greater uniqueness of the teacher’s
strategy. Although this approach directed the present study to instances when the teacher applied a
very different strategy to the students, the authors were aware that using these methods might not be
an ideal solution. Using any of the clustering methods to calculate the difference between dissimilarity
matrices might be a possible enhancement for future research.
Qualitative analysis of the teacher’s eye-movements especially and an analysis of her answers
revealed that the teacher used a completely different strategy to solve tasks. In the majority of cases,
the teacher did not look at the legend and attempted to solve the tasks directly. Unfortunately, her
answers were very often not correct. In the discussion after the experiment, the teacher explained
that she had a feeling that she should know the correct answers, and therefore she solved the tasks
according to her knowledge, not with the aid of the map. This may have been caused by the tasks
focusing on topics which were part of the geography curricula. A completely different scenario might
occur if the map of an unknown territory or a fictitious map were used. Kulhavy and Stock [82] stated
that people do not learn maps in a conceptual vacuum; their map representations are affected by
information already retained in memory.
The results of the students’ answers showed that they indicated a considerable number of incorrect
answers in several tasks. The problems with Task01, Task04, Task08, and Task10 may have been
caused by poor choice of cartographic visualization methods or barely legible symbols in the legend.
These findings are important, and it may be beneficial to focus on them in future research. School
atlases are used in most schools in the Czech Republic, and more user studies focusing on problematic
maps may be helpful to publishers and improve the cartographic literacy of students.

5. Conclusions
School world atlases are crucial in geography education. However, only a few user studies have
analysed student learning with a school atlas. The present paper aims to contribute in filling this gap.
An eye-tracking experiment with ten tasks using thematic maps from the Czech school world atlas
was designed, and the eye-tracking data of 30 students were recorded using GazePoint eye-tracker.
The eye-movements of the students’ geography teacher and students were recorded and compared.
The paper defined three research questions and explored the results of an experiment designed to
provide answers to these questions.
The results for Q1 show that in general, the students were able to learn with the maps effectively.
In this research question, the accuracy of answers of all participants was analysed together with
the trial duration. The average correctness of answers from all students was 71%. This analysis
pointed to several problematic tasks. Reading values from pie-charts with a logarithmic scale (Task07)
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 20 of 24

posed the greatest difficulties. The topics of logarithmic scales and pie-charts could be addressed in
geography education.
The results for Q2 revealed difficulties in solving tasks due to poor cartographic visualization
methods, for example, some symbols were hard to distinguish (Task01, Task04). The most serious
problems were discovered in students estimating the value of the bar chart (Task10). Students barely
understood the legend scale in which one millimetre of the bar represented USD 50 million in export
volume. These issues should be considered in the next edition of the school word atlas.
Q3 targeted map-reading strategies. The study proved that the geography teacher used a different
approach in solving tasks to her students. The experiment revealed that the teacher had a feeling that
she should know the correct solution to the task, so she answered according to her knowledge and did
not read the map at all. This performance was observed in most of the tasks. The teacher looked at the
legend in only a few tasks. This strategy, however, resulted in few correct answers. Discovering that a
teacher reads a map and solves tasks differently to her students is very serious. If teachers are not
aware of this difference and select maps and compile tasks according to “their own strategies”, student
learning may not be effective. It is desirable that learning with an atlas is based on consistency in the
compilation of tasks with the maps and the student’s ability to work with these maps. Geography
curricula should focus on issues in map reading.
The present eye-tracking study highlighted several maps with poorly applied cartographic
methods which created difficulties for students. Moreover, the research highlighted that the teacher
used a different approach in map reading. She rather relied on her knowledge than reading the map,
answering directly instead of using the map legend.
The results can assist cartographers and map publishers in improving their maps to be more
comprehensible to readers. Geography teachers can also use the results to understand how their
students read the maps and how to teach geography more attractively and effectively.

Supplementary Materials: Map stimuli in higher resolution can be accessed via www.eyetracking.upol.cz/atlases_
thematic.
Author Contributions: Marketa Beitlova was responsible for the research topic, experiment design, literature review
and interpretation of the results. Stanislav Popelka participated in data recording, conducted data pre-processing,
and analysed the possibilities in the scanpath comparison. Both authors contributed to the analyses of the recorded
data. Vit Vozenilek participated in formulation of the experiment’s concept, interpreted the results and reviewed the
manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research was funded by The Czech Science Foundation grant number 18-05432S and the Internal
Grant Agency of Palacký University Olomouc grant number IGA_PrF_2020_027.
Acknowledgments: This work was supported by The Czech Science Foundation (project No. 18-05432S) and the
Internal Grant Agency of Palacký University Olomouc (IGA_PrF_2020_027). Authors would like to thank to all
participants of the study.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References
1. Pravda, J. Kartografická gramotnost, čítanie máp a generovanie poznatkov z máp. Geod. A Kartogr. Obz.
2001, 47, 8–9.
2. Pravda, J.; Kusendová, D. Počítačová Tvorba Tematických Máp; Univerzita Komenského Bratislava: Bratislava,
Slovakia, 2004.
3. Vondráková, A.; Voženílek, V. User preferences in image map using. Int. Arch. Photogramm. Remote Sens.
Spat. Inf. Sci. 2016, XLI-B4, 599–603. [CrossRef]
4. Muehrcke, P.; Kimerling, A.; Juliana, O. Muehrcke. Map Use: Reading, Analysis, and Interpretation; Revised 4th
Edition; JP Publications: Madison, WI, USA, 2001.
5. Olson, J.M. A coordinated approach to map communication improvement. Am. Cartogr. 1976, 3, 151–160.
[CrossRef]
6. Board, C. Map reading tasks appropriate in experimental studies in cartographic communication. Cartogr. Int.
J. Geogr. Inf. Geovisualization 1978, 15, 1–12. [CrossRef]
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 21 of 24

7. Morrison, J.L. Towards a functional definition of the science of cartography with emphasis on map reading.
Am. Cartogr. 1978, 5, 97–110. [CrossRef]
8. Ormeling, F. Teaching map use concepts to children. In Proceedings of the Seminar on Cognitive Map,
Children and Education in Cartography, Gifu, Japan, 8–10 November 1996.
9. Havelková, L.; Hanus, M. The impact of map type on the level of student map skills. Cartogr. Int. J. Geogr.
Inf. Geovisualization 2018, 53, 149–170. [CrossRef]
10. Hanus, M.; Marada, M. What does a map-skills-test tell us about Czech pupils? Geografie 2016, 121, 279–299.
[CrossRef]
11. Wiegand, P. Learning and Teaching with Maps; Routledge: London, UK, 2006.
12. Herrmann, D.; Pickle, L.W. A cognitive subtask model of statistical map reading. Vis. Cogn. 1996, 3, 165–190.
[CrossRef]
13. Keates, J.S. Understanding Maps; Routledge: London, UK, 2014.
14. Kimerling, A.J.; Muehrcke, P.; Muehrcke, J.O.; Muehrcke, P. Map Use: Reading, Analysis, Interpretation; ESRI
Press Academic: Redlands, CA, USA, 2016.
15. Robinson, A.H.; Kimerling, A. Elements of Cartography; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1995.
16. Bugdayci, I.; Bildirici, I.O. Evaluation of Educational Atlas Maps in Terms of Cartographic Design.
In Proceedings of the IOP Conference Series: Earth and Environmental Science, Prague, Czech Republic, 5–9
September 2016; pp. 1–6.
17. Brychtova, A.; Popelka, S.; Vozenilek, V.; Svobodova, H. The analysis of eye movement as a tool for evaluation
of maps and graphical outputs from GIS. In Proceedings of the 19th International Conference on Geography
and Geoinformatics: Challenge For Practise and Education, Brno, Czech Republic, 8–9 September 2011;
pp. 154–162.
18. Carswell, R.J. Children’s Abilities in Topographic Map Reading; Cartographica: Toronto, ON, Canada, 1970.
19. Van Dijk, H.; van der Schee, J.; Trimp, H.; van der Zijpp, T. Map skills and geographical knowledge. Int. Res.
Geogr. Environ. Educ. 1994, 3, 68–80. [CrossRef]
20. Schee, J.V.D.; Dijk, H.V. The effect of student freedom of choice in learning map skills. Int. Res. Geogr.
Environ. Educ. 1999, 8, 256–267. [CrossRef]
21. Hanus, M.; Marada, M. Mapové dovednosti v českých a zahraničních kurikulárních dokumentech: Srovnávací
studie. Geografie 2013, 118, 158–178. [CrossRef]
22. Reyes Nuñez, J.J.; Juliarena de Moretti, C.E.; Gallé, E.; Garra, A.M.; Rey, C.A.; Alves de Castro, M.V.;
Dibiase, A.S. Reading Thematic Maps in Argentine and Hungarian Schools. In Proceedings of the Read at
Proceedings of the XXII International Cartographic Conference, A Coruña, Spain, 11–16 July 2005; pp. 1–10.
23. Reyes Nuñez, J.J.; Juhász, B. Hungarian survey on the use of cartograms in school cartography. Int. J. Cartogr.
2015, 1, 5–17. [CrossRef]
24. Sun, H.; Li, Z. Effectiveness of cartogram for the representation of spatial data. Cartogr. J. 2010, 47, 12–21.
[CrossRef]
25. Kubíček, P.; Šašinka, Č.; Stachoň, Z.; Štěrba, Z.; Apeltauer, J.; Urbánek, T. Cartographic Design and Usability
of Visual Variables for Linear Features. Cartogr. J. 2017, 54, 91–102. [CrossRef]
26. Goł˛ebiowska, I. Legend layouts for thematic maps: A case study integrating usability metrics with the
thinking aloud method. Cartogr. J. 2015, 52, 28–40. [CrossRef]
27. Pétera, B.-S.; Dulamăb, M.E.; Ilovanc, O.-R.; Kosinszkid, S.-A.; Răcăs, ane, B.S. Exploring Map Drawing Skills
of Geography Teacher Training Students. Eur. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 2016, 12, 41–47.
28. Słomska, K. Types of maps used as a stimuli in cartographical empirical research. Misc. Geogr. 2018, 22,
157–171. [CrossRef]
29. Bishop, B.W.; Haggerty, K.C.; Richardson, B.E. Usability of E-government mapping applications: Lessons
learned from the US National Atlas. Int. J. Cartogr. 2015, 1, 134–150. [CrossRef]
30. Voženílek, V.; Morkesová, P.; Vondráková, A. Cognitive aspects of map symbology in the world school
atlases. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2014, 112, 1121–1136.
31. Michaelidou, E.; Nakos, B.; Filippakopoulou, V. The ability of elementary school children to analyse general
reference and thematic maps. Cartogr. Int. J. Geogr. Inf. Geovisualization 2004, 39, 65–88. [CrossRef]
32. Blaha, J.D. Hodnocení kartografických děl z hlediska estetiky a uživatelské vstřícnosti. Kartogr. Listy 2005,
13, 14–24.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 22 of 24

33. Bláha, M.J.D. Hodnocení současných českých školních atlasů světa z hlediska estetiky a uživatelské vstřícnosti.
In Proceedings of the 1st International Trade Fair of Geodesy, Cartography, Navigation and Geoinformatics,
Prague, Czech, 16–18 March 2006.
34. Peresadko, V.; Baltabaeva, Z. Local lore school atlas of Turkmenistan as the result of foreign students’ scientific
research. Probl. Bezperervnoï Geogr. Osvìti Ì Kartogr. 2017, 2017, 55–61.
35. Gómez Solórzano, L.S.; Sancho Comíns, J.; Bosque Sendra, J. Atlas Design: A Usability Approach for the
Development and Evaluation of Cartographic Products. Cartogr. J. 2017, 54, 343–357. [CrossRef]
36. Song, Y.; Li, Q.; Yu, Y.; Xu, G.; Liu, Y.; Liu, Y. Adaptability of atlas symbol sizes under multivariate conditions.
Cartogr. Geogr. Inf. Sci. 2020, 47, 1–13. [CrossRef]
37. Zhang, H.; Chen, Y. Research on Design and Compilation of “The Tourist Atlas of Shanxi Province”.
In Proceedings of the 2008 International Workshop on Education Technology and Training & 2008 International
Workshop on Geoscience and Remote Sensing, Shanghai, China, 21–22 December 2008; pp. 263–266.
38. Żyszkowska, W. Map perception: Theories and research in the second half of the twentieth century.
Pol. Cartogr. Rev. 2015, 47, 179–190. [CrossRef]
39. Rohrer, C. When to Use Which User-Experience Research Methods. Available online: https://www.nngroup.
com/articles/which-ux-research-methods/ (accessed on 19 August 2020).
40. Popelka, S.; Vozenilek, V. Specifying of Requirements for Spatio-Temporal Data in Map by Eye-Tracking
and Space-Time-Cube. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Graphic and Image Processing,
Singapore, 5–7 October 2012; Volume 8768.
41. Dong, W.; Ying, Q.; Yang, Y.; Tang, S.; Zhan, Z.; Liu, B.; Meng, L. Using Eye Tracking to Explore the Impacts
of Geography Courses on Map-based Spatial Ability. Sustainability 2019, 11, 76. [CrossRef]
42. Biland, J.; Çöltekin, A. An empirical assessment of the impact of the light direction on the relief inversion
effect in shaded relief maps: NNW is better than NW. Cartogr. Geogr. Inf. Sci. 2017, 44, 1–15. [CrossRef]
43. Havelková, L.; Goł˛ebiowska, I.M. What Went Wrong for Bad Solvers during Thematic Map Analysis? Lessons
Learned from an Eye-Tracking Study. ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 9.
44. Kiik, A.; Nyström, M.; Harrie, L. Cartographic Design Matters–A Comparison of Thematic Polygon Design.
Cartogr. J. 2017, 54, 24–35. [CrossRef]
45. Göbel, F.; Kiefer, P.; Giannopoulos, I.; Duchowski, A.T.; Raubal, M. Improving map reading with gaze-adaptive
legends. In Proceedings of Proceedings of the 2018 ACM Symposium on Eye Tracking Research &
Applications, Warsaw, Poland, 14–17 June 2018; pp. 1–9.
46. Burian, J.; Popelka, S.; Beitlova, M. Evaluation of the Cartographical Quality of Urban Plans by Eye-Tracking.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2018, 7, 192. [CrossRef]
47. Dvorský, J.; Snášel, V.; Voženílek, V. Map similarity testing using matrix decomposition. In Proceedings of
the 2009 International Conference on Intelligent Networking and Collaborative Systems, Barcelona, Spain,
4–6 November 2009; pp. 290–294.
48. Anderson, K.C.; Leinhardt, G. Maps as representations: Expert novice comparison of projection
understanding. Cogn. Instr. 2002, 20, 283–321. [CrossRef]
49. Thorndyke, P.W.; Stasz, C. Individual differences in procedures for knowledge acquisition from maps.
Cogn. Psychol. 1980, 12, 137–175. [CrossRef]
50. Gilhooly, K.J.; Wood, M.; Kinnear, P.R.; Green, C. Skill in map reading and memory for maps. Q. J. Exp.
Psychol. Sect. A 1988, 40, 87–107. [CrossRef]
51. Herman, L.; Juřík, V.; Stachoň, Z.; Vrbík, D.; Russnák, J.; Řezník, T. Evaluation of User Performance in
Interactive and Static 3D Maps. ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2018, 7, 415. [CrossRef]
52. Ooms, K.; De Maeyer, P.; Fack, V.; Van Assche, E.; Witlox, F. Interpreting maps through the eyes of expert
and novice users. Int. J. Geogr. Inf. Sci. 2012, 26, 1773–1788. [CrossRef]
53. Ooms, K.; De Maeyer, P.; Fack, V. Study of the attentive behavior of novice and expert map users using eye
tracking. Cartogr. Geogr. Inf. Sci. 2014, 41, 37–54. [CrossRef]
54. Koláčný, A. Cartographic information—A fundamental concept and term in modern cartography. Cartogr. J.
1969, 6, 47–49. [CrossRef]
55. Morrison, J.L. The science of cartography and its essential processes. Cartogr. Int. J. Geogr. Inf. Geovisualization
1977, 14, 58–71. [CrossRef]
56. MacEachren, A.M. How Maps Work: Representation, Visualization, and Design; Guilford Press: New York, NY,
USA, 2004.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 23 of 24

57. Klimová, E. Školní Atlas Světa, 4th ed.; Kartografie Praha: Praha, Czech Republic, 2017.
58. Dalmaijer, E. Is the low-cost EyeTribe eye tracker any good for research? PeerJ Prepr. 2014, 1–35. [CrossRef]
59. Ooms, K.; Dupont, L.; Lapon, L.; Popelka, S. Accuracy and precision of fixation locations recorded with the
low-cost Eye Tribe tracker in different experimental setups. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2015, 8, 1–24. [CrossRef]
60. Popelka, S.; Stachoň, Z.; Šašinka, Č.; Doležalová, J. Eyetribe Tracker Data Accuracy Evaluation and Its
Interconnection with Hypothesis Software for Cartographic Purposes. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2016, 2016.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
61. Janthanasub, V.; Meesad, P. Evaluation of a low-cost eye tracking system for computer input. King Mongkut’s
Univ. Technol. North Bangk. Int. J. Appl. Sci. Technol. 2015, 8, 185–196. [CrossRef]
62. Lewis, G.; Bidelman, G.M. Autonomic Nervous System Correlates of Speech Categorization Revealed
through Pupillometry. Front. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 1418. [CrossRef]
63. Dos Santos, J.P.M.; Ferreira, H.; Reis, J.; Prata, D.; Simões, S.P.; Borges, I.D. The Use of Consumer Neuroscience
Knowledge in Improving Real Promotional Media: The Case of Worten. In Marketing and Smart Technologies;
Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2020; pp. 202–218.
64. Kim, S.; Pollanen, M.; Reynolds, M.G.; Burr, W.S. Problem Solving as a Path to Comprehension.
Math. Comput. Sci. 2020, 1–15. [CrossRef]
65. Skrabankova, J.; Popelka, S.; Beitlova, M. Students’ ability to work with graphs in physics studies related to
three typical student groups. J. Balt. Sci. Educ. 2020, 19, 298–316. [CrossRef]
66. Larson, D.J.; Wetherbee, J.C.; Branscum, P. CrossFit Athletic Identity’s Relationship to Sponsor Recall,
Recognition, and Purchase Intent. Int. J. Kinesiol. Sports Sci. 2019, 7, 6–15. [CrossRef]
67. Voßkühler, A.; Nordmeier, V.; Kuchinke, L.; Jacobs, A.M. OGAMA (Open Gaze and Mouse Analyzer):
Open-source software designed to analyze eye and mouse movements in slideshow study designs.
Behav. Res. Methods 2008, 40, 1150–1162. [CrossRef]
68. Popelka, S. Optimal eye fixation detection settings for cartographic purposes. In Proceedings of the 14th
SGEM GeoConference on Informatics, Geoinformatics and Remote Sensing, Albena, Bulgaria, 17–26 June
2014; pp. 705–712.
69. Andrienko, G.; Andrienko, N.; Wrobel, S. Visual analytics tools for analysis of movement data. ACM SIGKDD
Explor. Newsl. 2007, 9, 38–46. [CrossRef]
70. Andrienko, G.; Andrienko, N.; Burch, M.; Weiskopf, D. Visual Analytics Methodology for Eye Movement
Studies. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 2012, 18, 2889–2898. [CrossRef]
71. Dolezalova, J.; Popelka, S. ScanGraph: A Novel Scanpath Comparison Method Using Visualisation of Graph
Cliques. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2016, 9, 1–13. [CrossRef]
72. Popelka, S.; Dolezalova, J.; Beitlova, M. New features of scangraph: A tool for revealing participants’ strategy
from eye-movement data. In Proceedings of the ACM Symposium on Eye Tracking Research & Applications,
Warsaw, Poland, 14–17 June 2018; p. 103.
73. Jarodzka, H.; Holmqvist, K.; Nyström, M. A vector-based, multidimensional scanpath similarity measure.
In Proceedings of the 2010 Symposium on Eye-Tracking Research & Applications, Austin, TX, USA, 22–24
March 2010; pp. 211–218.
74. Dewhurst, R.; Nyström, M.; Jarodzka, H.; Foulsham, T.; Johansson, R.; Holmqvist, K. It depends on how you
look at it: Scanpath comparison in multiple dimensions with MultiMatch, a vector-based approach. Behav.
Res. Methods 2012, 44, 1079–1100. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
75. Wagner, A.; Halchenko, Y.; Hanke, M. multimatch-gaze: The MultiMatch algorithm for gaze path comparison
in Python. J. Open Source Softw. 2019, 4, 1525. [CrossRef]
76. Coltekin, A.; Fabrikant, S.; Lacayo, M. Exploring the efficiency of users’ visual analytics strategies based on
sequence analysis of eye movement recordings. Int. J. Geogr. Inf. Sci. 2010, 24, 1559–1575. [CrossRef]
77. Castner, N.; Kasneci, E.; Kübler, T.; Scheiter, K.; Richter, J.; Eder, T.; Hüttig, F.; Keutel, C. Scanpath comparison
in medical image reading skills of dental students: Distinguishing stages of expertise development.
In Proceedings of the 2018 ACM Symposium on Eye Tracking Research & Applications, Warsaw, Poland,
14–17 June 2018; pp. 1–9.
78. Day, E.L.; Tang, H.; Kendhammer, L.K.; Pienta, N.J. Sequence Analysis: Use of Scanpath Patterns for
Analysis of Students’ Problem-Solving Strategies. In Eye Tracking for the Chemistry Education Researcher;
ACS Publications: Washington, DC, USA, 2018; pp. 73–97.
ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2020, 9, 492 24 of 24

79. Kang, Z.; Landry, S.J. An eye movement analysis algorithm for a multielement target tracking task: Maximum
transition-based agglomerative hierarchical clustering. IEEE Trans. Hum. -Mach. Syst. 2014, 45, 13–24.
[CrossRef]
80. Popelka, S.; Herman, L.; Řezník, T.; Pařilová, M.; Jedlička, K.; Bouchal, J.; Kepka, M.; Charvát, K.
User Evaluation of Map-Based Visual Analytic Tools. ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2019, 8, 363. [CrossRef]
81. Dolezalova, J.; Popelka, S. Evaluation of user strategy on 2D and 3D city maps based on novel scanpath
comparison method and graph visualization. In Proceedings of the ISPRS 2016, Prague, Czech, 12–19
July 2016.
82. Kulhavy, R.W.; Stock, W.A. How cognitive maps are learned and remembered. Ann. Assoc. Am. Geogr. 1996,
86, 123–145. [CrossRef]

© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy