Memory-Decoding
Memory-Decoding
SECTION 3
READING PASSAGE 3
You should spend about 20 minutes on Question 27-40 which are based on
Reading Passage 3 below.
Memory Decoding
Try this memory test: Study each face and compose a vivid image for the person’s
first and last name. Rose Leo, for example, could be a rosebud and a lion. Fill in
the blanks on the next page. The Examinations School at Oxford University is an
austere building of oak-paneled rooms, large Gothic windows, and looming
portraits of eminent dukes and earls. It is where generations of Oxford students
have tested their memory on final exams, and it is where, last August, 34
contestants gathered at the World Memory Championships to be examined in an
entirely different manner.
In timed trials, contestants were challenged to look at and then recite a two-page
poem, memorize rows of 40-digit numbers, recall the names of 110 people after
looking at their photographs, and perform seven other feats of extraordinary
retention. Some tests took just a few minutes; others lasted hours. In the 14 years
since the World Memory Championships was founded, no one has memorized the
order of a shuffled deck of playing cards in less than 30 seconds. That nice round
number has become the four-minute mile of competitive memory, a benchmark that
the world’s best “mental athletes,” as some of them like to be called, is closing in
on. Most contestants claim to have just average memories, and scientific testing
confirms that they’re not just being modest. Their feats are based on tricks that
capitalize on how the human brain encodes information. Anyone can learn them.
B
It might seem odd that the memory contestants would use visual imagery and
spatial navigation to remember numbers, but the activity makes sense when their
techniques are revealed. Cooke, a 23-year-old cognitive-science graduate student
with a shoulder length mop of curly hair, is a grand master of brain storage. He can
memorize the order of 10 decks of playing cards in less than an hour or one deck
of cards in less than a minute. He is closing in on the 30-second deck. In the Lamb
and Flag, Cooke pulled out a deck of cards and shuffled it. He held up three cards
– the 7 of spades, the queen of clubs, and the 10 of spades. He pointed at a
fireplace and said, “Destiny’s Child is whacking Franz Schubert with handbags.”
The next three cards were the king of hearts, the king of spades, and the jack of
clubs.
How did he do it? Cooke has already memorized a specific person, verb, and object
that he associates with each card in the deck. For example, for the 7 of spades, the
person (or, in this case, persons) is always the singing group Destiny’s Child, the
action is surviving a storm, and the image is a dinghy. The queen of clubs is always
his friend Henrietta, the action is thwacking with a handbag, and the image is of
wardrobes filled with designer clothes. When Cooke commits a deck to memory, he
does it three cards at a time. Every three-card group forms a single image of a
person doing something to an object. The first card in the triplet becomes the
person, the second the verb, the third the object. He then places those images
along a specific familiar route, such as the one he took through the Lamb and Flag.
In competitions, he uses an imaginary route that he has designed to be as smooth
and downhill as possible. When it comes time to recall, Cooke takes a mental walk
along his route and translates the images into cards. That’s why the MRIs of the
memory contestants showed activation in the brain areas associated with visual
imagery and spatial navigation.
The more resonant the images are, the more difficult they are to forget. But even
meaningful information is hard to remember when there’s a lot of it. That’s why
competitive memorizers place their images along an imaginary route. That
technique, known as the loci method, reportedly originated in 477 B.C. with the
Greek poet Simonides of Ceos. Simonides was the sole survivor of a roof collapse
that killed all the other guests at a royal banquet. The bodies were mangled
beyond recognition, but Simonides was able to reconstruct the guest list by closing
his eyes and recalling each individual around the dinner table. What he had
discovered was that our brains are exceptionally good at remembering images and
spatial information. Evolutionary psychologists have offered an explanation:
Presumably, our ancestors found it important to recall where they found their last
meal or the way back to the cave. After Simonides’ discovery, the loci method
became popular across ancient Greece as a trick for memorizing speeches and
texts. Aristotle wrote about it, and later a number of treatises on the art of memory
were published in Rome. Before printed books, the art of memory was considered
a staple of classical education, on a par with grammar, logic, and rhetoric.
The most famous of the naturals was the Russian journalist S.V. Shereshevski, who
could recall long lists of numbers memorized decades earlier, as well as poems,
strings of nonsense syllables, and just about anything else he was asked to
remember. “The capacity of his memory had no distinct limits,” wrote Alexander
Luria, the Russian psychologist who studies Shereshevski also had synesthesia, a
rare condition in which the senses become intertwined. For example, every number
may be associated with a color or every word with a taste. Synesthetic reactions
evoke a response in more areas of the brain, making memory easier.
31 A depiction of the rare ability which assists the extraordinary memory reactions
Questions 32-36
A the champions’ brains are different in some way from common people
B difference in the brain of champions’ scan image to control subjects are shown
when memorizing sequences of three-digit numbers
C champions did much worse when they are asked to remember photographs
E there is some part in the brain coping with visual and spatial memory
27. E
28. A
29. C
30. G
31. F
32. specific person
33. three cards/ 3 cards
34. mental walk
35. loci method
36. education
37. A
38. D
39. B
40. E