Maximizing Learning
Maximizing Learning
Our definition of learning makes it clear that people acquire and develop skills
within the learner and in the environment. In this section of the chapter, we
present factors that have been shown to affect learning and discuss their outcomes. We discuss three primary areas as we
emphasize ways to maximize
learning, namely trainee characteristics, training design, and the transfer of training
Trainee Characteristics
new tasks and new information. Three such characteristics are trainability, personality,
and attitudes.
the trainee’s level of ability and motivation with his or her perceptions of
Involved in
Maximizing Learning
Trainee Characteristics
Trainability
Motivation
Ability
Training Design
Conditions of practice
Active practice
Overlearning
Task sequencing
Interference
Transfer of Training
Identical elements
General principles
Stimulus variability
Opportunity to perform
Transfer-of-training climate]
This formula illustrates that a trainee must have both the motivation and the
ability to learn; if either is lacking, learning will not occur. The equation also
shows that a very high level of one cannot completely overcome a very low
level of the other. In addition, if employees perceive little support in the work
environment for learning new knowledge or skills, they will be less likely to
learn and use them. Thus, it is important to note that trainability is not just a
function of the individual trainee, but also of the work environment in which
they are not motivated to attend or are not prepared to do well in wastes time
and resources. Trainees with less ability take longer to learn, which can increase
the length of the training period and the expense involved in conducting training.
In fact, it is possible that such trainees may never learn to the levels desired
by the organization.
company is in a training program designed to teach selling skills for the equipment
relations. If the technician lacks either skill, it is likely that learning to sell
effectively will be difficult. The technician may want to learn and try hard to do
so, but this low level of ability will hinder learning. Similarly, if the technician
does not think learning to sell will help to achieve his or her own personal goals,
no effort may be made to learn the sales skills. A number of studies have shown
the clear links between ability and learning.24 The same is true for motivation,25
as well as for perceptions of the work environment.26
Over the past several years, researchers have studied the notion of pretraining
• The way in which individuals view their own ability (as a fixed entity or an
acquirable skill) affects anxiety level, efficacy perceptions, and the learning
• Experiencing negative events on the job prior to training can increase trainees’ motivation
activities.
• These findings are useful in that they suggest ways in which organizations
can increase the motivation to participate in and learn from HRD interventions.
address a need they have, and as a way to achieve valued outcomes. Further,
trainees must perceive the organization and their immediate work environment
allowing trainees some choice in what training they receive, see the boxed
finds that general cognitive ability (i.e., intelligence) has a direct impact on “the
acquisition of” job knowledge, but prior job knowledge has almost no effect on
the acquisition of subsequent job knowledge.34 This finding suggests that cognitive
ability rather than prior job knowledge should be used to select trainees into
Trainability testing is one approach that can be used to ensure that trainees
have both the motivation and the ability to learn. This approach focuses on measuring
the motivation and relevant abilities of candidates for training and selecting
for training only those who show a sufficient level of trainability. For
of the training program and use their performance on that section as a predictor
of how well they will perform during the remainder of training. For example,
Arthur Siegel described a method called miniature training and evaluation testing,
in which U.S. Navy recruits were trained on a sample of important tasks and
tested on their ability to perform these tasks. Using eleven training and evaluation
modules, the approach yielded better predictions of success for several jobs
BMW took a similar approach when it opened its first U.S. auto manufacturing
facility in Greer, South Carolina. To lure the company to South Carolina, state
government officials offered generous tax incentives. The state also agreed to
recruit and train potential BMW employees. Once trainees completed their
training, BMW selected only those they wanted to hire to be among the initial
1,500 associates in its new plant. BMW stated that it was looking for associates
with a strong commitment to quality and teamwork, and this procedure allowed
the company to select the very best among those who had completed the rigorous
quality and success of the cars made at this plant (including the Z3 roadster), as
well as subsequent expansions of the plant and workforce, indicate that this “train, and then select” strategy was successful.
Similarly, from the research literature, a meta-analysis of research studies examining the use of work sample
tests of trainability concluded that such tests predict success in training and job performance for untrained job applicants.
Trainability testing has also been effective in predicting the training success of older workers.
Personality and Attitudes. Although not explicitly mentioned in the definition
learning (see Chapter 2). Ray Noe suggested that an employee’s attitudes toward
career exploration and job involvement impact learning and its applications to
the job.39 Other research has shown that job involvement, expectations for training,
Personality is the stable set of personal characteristics that account for consistent
include locus of control, the need for achievement, activity, independence, and
sociability.41 Murray Barrick and Michael Mount reported the results of a metaanalysis
task performance and learning) affects learning, mood, and satisfaction with training.
43 They also found that individuals with low levels of cognitive playfulness
are affected more by positive feedback than individuals with higher levels of cognitive
that learning will occur. This approach to maximizing learning fits with Glaser’s
notion that knowing the initial state of the learner is an important part of effective
training.45
Training Design
Training design issues include (1) the conditions of practice that influence
learning and (2) the factors that impact retention of what is learned.
Although much of the research on this topic was conducted before 1970,
new research in instructional psychology has revived interest. While the information
training program, not all the findings will work in all situations. Recall Gagné’s
arguments cited earlier about traditional learning principles. There is no substitute
Conditions of Practice. At least six issues have been studied that relate to
practice and learning. They include active practice, massed versus spaced practice
sessions, whole versus part learning, overlearning, knowledge of results, and task
sequencing.
repeatedly perform the task or use the knowledge being learned. if a paramedic is learning how to operate the “jaws of life”
(to extract passengers
from vehicles damaged in accidents), the training sessions should include multiple
Researchers have also been interested in whether mental practice, the “cognitive
practice is effective for both cognitive and physical tasks (though more so for
cognitive tasks). This study also showed that the effect of mental practice on performance
rehearse the tasks they are learning to perform outside of the training environment
one session or divide it into segments separated by a period of time. For example,
skills can be learned either way, but spaced practice sessions with a reasonable
rest period between them lead to better performance and longer retention of
100 years of research, and concluded that massed practice or “cramming” was
the least effective strategy for learning and retention.49 As one recent example,
took six exams during the semester, plus a cumulative final exam, whereas students
in other sections took two exams during the semester, plus the identical
final exam. Students taking the bi-weekly exams outperformed students who
took the two midterm exams, both in final exam performance, as well as in
their overall course grade.50 For difficult, complex tasks, an initial massed session
Using a massed rather than a spaced practice session is often a matter of practicality
winning out over science. Time and resource constraints may influence
should realize that under these conditions retention can suffer. It may be necessary
by whether massed or spaced sessions are used. One research study found that
poorly than those told simply to do their best, whereas those in spaced sessions
assigned specific, difficult goals performed slightly better than those told to do
their best.52
Whole versus part learning concerns the size of the unit to be learned, that is,
should trainees practice an entire task (or study certain material as a whole), or
should the task or material be learned in separate parts or chunks? Gagné argued
that procedural material (material organized into a series of steps) should be analyzed
and divided into subunits, with the trainees mastering each subunit before
performing the entire procedure. Actually, the answer to which method is most effective appears to depend
on the nature of the task to be learned. When the subtasks are relatively easy to
perform and are well organized (interrelated), the whole method is superior. Otherwise,
the part method has proven to be more effective.54 For example, operating
a chain saw involves adding fuel, holding it properly, starting it, making various
cuts, and turning it off. Given that these subtasks are interrelated, it makes sense
that they be learned together. The task of supervising others, however, includes
and delegating work. These subtasks are less closely related and would best
associations between the parts of a task (or unit of knowledge), it is less likely
that situational changes will interfere with learning. Second, overlearning provides
for doing so in the job setting.56 For example, overlearning the procedure
don’t often face this situation when flying. Third, overlearning should make
and tasks, so when orders come to attack, these tasks will be second nature and
if you practice beyond the point of perfection.”59 Quite obviously, its major
drawback is that overlearning can increase the time and expense of training.
adequacy of one’s performance, and it can come from observers, the performer,
or the task itself. A sizable body of research suggests that feedback enhances
learning and retention.60 Trainers and educators generally agree that feedback
examined.61 Avraham Kluger and Angelo DeNisi theorize that this has to do
with the level of control individuals go through when learning and performing
tasks. They argue that individuals proceed through three hierarchical levels of
and that feedback changes the individual’s locus of attention to a particular level
of control. They suggest that the effectiveness of feedback decreases as the individual
moves through the levels from task learning to task motivation to metatasks.
Kluger and DeNisi’s theory supports the use of feedback during skill and knowledge acquisition, but suggests that feedback is
less effective (and may even
especially in regard to timing and specificity. To ensure that the learner clearly
understands the relationship between the feedback and the behavior, it should
inside or outside the trainee’s control) can affect efficacy beliefs, with feedback
efficacy. In addition, recall the research cited earlier that found that the impact
Finally, task sequencing suggests that tasks and knowledge can be learned more
effectively if what is to be learned is divided into subtasks that are arranged and
how task sequencing can help in learning intellectual skills, motor skills, and attitudes.
GUIDON supports this approach, as does the research of Philip Decker and
overlearning, feedback, and practice sessions spaced over time tend to increase
learning.
The goal of training goes beyond ensuring that the trainee learns the task or
retained. Three additional issues that influence retention are the meaningfulness
for the individual learner. For example, a new way of soldering circuits
and remember.67 For example, college humanities students were asked to rate
the meaningfulness of three Bible passages. Later in that same period, they were
given a pop quiz, and asked to recall as much of each passage as possible. For two
topics at the beginning of training sessions can help trainees understand the course content as a whole. Using examples and
terminology familiar to trainees
and mnemonic devices (such as creating a word out of the first letters of items in
writers (us included) often seek to use this principle when introducing and presenting
retained—after all, you can’t retain something you never had to begin with.
massed versus spaced practice, and whole versus part learning as ways to ensure
initial learning.
Interference can also affect the extent to which learning is retained. Interference
can be of two types.71 First, material or skills learned before the training session
can inhibit recall of the newly learned material. For example, an accountant
who is an expert on the New York tax code may have difficulty remembering
recent instruction regarding the tax code and procedures for Florida. The
follow New York procedures when helping a client who must file in Florida.
Second, information learned after a training session may also interfere with
retention. For example, a firefighter trained to operate the power ladder on the
city’s older fire trucks may have difficulty retaining that knowledge if a different
sequence of steps must be learned for the same operation on a newer fire truck.
Both types of interference are similar in that the learner is required to make
different responses to the same situation. The more responses one learns, the
Transfer of Training
Transfer of training is an important and recurring theme in the HRD literature.
also use it on the job to improve performance. The transfer of training to the job
Transfer can take different forms. Positive transfer occurs when job performance
unlikely, but recall the detrimental effects interference can have on learning
and performance. Tennis players, for example, may find that their tennis shots
become less accurate after learning how to play racquetball. Although the two
sports seem similar, an accurate tennis shot requires a locked wrist, yet racquetball
players use their wrists during the swing. Therefore, the player’s tennis stroke
may become more “wristy” after learning racquetball, leading to less accurate
shots in tennis.
Another distinction that should be made is near transfer versus far transfer.
Near transfer involves the ability to directly apply on the job what has been learned in training, with little adjustment or
modification; far transfer has to do
with expanding upon or using what was learned in training in new or creative
ways.73 Other writers have referred to this as a distinction between skill reproduction
a negative relationship was observed between near and far transfer: trainees
who had done well demonstrating their mastery of the training content did less
should be more concerned with near transfer, far transfer, or both, but in most
cases, far transfer is the best indicator that training has been successful.
Timothy Baldwin and Kevin Ford developed a model of the training transfer
transfer, with trainee characteristics and the work environment affecting transfer
directly. Baldwin and Ford were critical of the lack of a strong theoretical framework
and the limited number of research studies in this area, because this limited
their ability to generalize findings from studies of transfer of training to organizational
settings. Despite these concerns, these principles and the results of recent
research offer many ideas for maximizing training transfer. These include the use
Thorndike and Woodworth in 1901, suggests that the more similar the training
and the performance situations are in terms of the stimuli present and responses
required, the more likely it is that training transfer will occur.77 For example, if
impatient customers, practice with such customers (possibly via role playing) can
improve the transfer of training. But if the only examples used in training are
customers who are polite, reasonable, and patient, training transfer to the job is
less likely.
fidelity is the extent to which the conditions of the training program, such as
Building a highly realistic airline cockpit simulator, with the same controls,
extent to which trainees attach similar meanings to both the training and performance
that imposes training task time limits that are similar to those on the job.
transfer than physical fidelity, but more research is needed to support this
claim.78
training, such as with the use of case studies, business games, or role plays. However,
increasing fidelity often involves increasing complexity and costs, which determine the supportiveness of the work
environment during needs assessment,
and that areas found wanting should be modified to increase the chances
training will transfer back to the workplace. Also, organizations that promote a
learned transfer back to the job.85 Recently, Martin found that peer support
was an important determinant of whether or not training transferred in a
corporate field setting. The opportunity to perform what has been learned back on the job is an
important element of the work environment. Work by Kevin Ford and colleagues
to tasks for which he or she was trained.”91 The opportunity to perform is influenced
by both the organization and the individual. Ford and colleagues investigated
the effects of three groups of variables (organizational level, work level, and
did have different opportunities to perform trained tasks on the job and that
attitudes toward training, work group support, and the trainees’ self-efficacy