Creativity in Performance
Creativity in Performance
net/publication/258173249
Creativity in performance
CITATIONS READS
53 1,250
1 author:
Eric Clarke
University of Oxford
89 PUBLICATIONS 3,351 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Eric Clarke on 26 February 2016.
In this paper I examine the different ways in which performance can be said to be
creative, discuss the significance of these different varieties of creativity, and explore in a
little more depth some of the varied manifestations of creativity that can be found in
performance.
Improvisation
The domain of performance where creativity is most conspicuously present is
improvisation. The ethnomusicologist John Baily writes that improvisation “implies
intentionality, setting out to create something new in each performance, ‘composition in real
time’ as it is sometimes described’ (Baily, 1999: 208), using this characterisation to point out
that many oral musical traditions are mistakenly assumed to be improvisatory, when they
actually involve none of the creativity that his definition requires. Psychological writing on
improvisation has been largely concerned with suggesting cognitive models for the way in
which this particular manifestation of creativity might be understood (e.g. Clarke, 1988;
Johnson-Laird, 1988; Pressing, 1988). Space precludes a review of that literature, and I will
confine myself to a couple of remarks about some social aspects of the phenomenon.
Although there is a tradition of solo improvisation (from church organists to free
improvisers), the great majority of musical improvisation is an explicitly social activity
involving complex interactions between performers, as well as between performers and
audience. Because of the predominantly cognitive orientation of the psychology of music, this
aspect of performance has only recently begun to be investigated, and only to the most
limited extent in improvisation. Ethnomusicologists have been more keenly aware of the
social dimension, as Monson, in a book on jazz improvisation makes plain:
Sansom (1997) has carried out a preliminary investigation of this ‘social construction’
within improvisation, in a study that focused creative processes in free improvising duos.
Sansom asked his participants first to play together in free improvisations that lasted from 5
to about 15 minutes, following which he interviewed the participants individually, asking
them while listening to a recording of the improvisation to comment on any aspect of the
music or their interaction. The interviews revealed the central role of personal interactions in
these improvised performances, and the kind of interweaving of social and structural factors
to which Monson draws attention above. The creative impetus in Sansom’s duos is at least as
much to do with the exploration of, and playing with, interpersonal dynamics as it is to do
with a direct confrontation with musical materials.
Different genres of improvisation engage different kinds of creative skills, and elicit
very different kinds of social and musical structures. A distinction is sometimes drawn
between idiomatic and non-idiomatic improvisation, the former referring to improvisation
that is based on or around material that has some particular stylistic identity (examples would
be improvised blues and jazz, or Raga-based improvisation of North Indian classical music).
Non-idiomatic improvisation (sometimes called free improvisation) provides opportunities
for extremely unpredictable and extreme social dynamics to develop, and the music that is
created in these circumstances often seems to be primarily a product of the particular social
context. If composed music seems by virtue of its structures and instrumental forces to
engender certain kinds of social arrangements (the social dynamics of the symphony
orchestra, the string quartet, the solo recitalist, the brass band), free improvisation seems
sometimes to work in the other direction: the music is created by, or is a reflection of, the
social arrangements characteristic of the ensemble.
Conclusions
In this paper I hope to have demonstrated that the psychology of music has made
significant progress in studying creativity in performance, particularly in understanding the
cognitive processes that underlie this highly regarded behaviour. Nonetheless there is still a
great deal more that is not well understood, partly because of the de-socialised way in which
performance has usually been studied. The engagement of cognitive processes with social
factors (performance traditions, socially constructed notions of ‘innovation’ and the limits of
acceptable radicalism, the interactions between narrowly defined musical processes and the
social context of performance) represents a considerable challenge to the psychology of
music, but one which is already being tackled in various ways, and which brings with it the
prospect of a less individualistic and ‘head-bound’ understanding not only of creativity in
music – but also of the human mind more generally.
Address for correspondence:
ERIC CLARKE
Professor of Music
Music Department
University of Sheffield
38 Taptonville Road
Sheffield S10 5BR
UK
E-mail: E.F.Clarke@sheffield.ac.uk
References
Baily, J. (1999) Ethnomusicological ideas on Sawyer’s ideas. Psychology of Music, 27, 208-
211.
Clarke, E. F. (1993) Imitating and evaluating real and transformed musical performances.
Music Perception, 10, (1993), 317-341.
Keller, H. (1990) The Keller Column. Essays by Hans Keller. London: Alfred Lengnick &
Co.